Affect, relationship schemas and social cognition Self injuring BPD inpatients

background image

AFFECT, RELATIONSHIP SCHEMAS, AND

SOCIAL COGNITION: SELF-INJURING

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER

INPATIENTS

Rachel Whipple

The George Washington University

J. Christopher Fowler

The Austen Riggs Center

Psychiatric patients who engage in nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) present specific
challenges to therapists because they often lack the capacities necessary to under-
stand the social and emotional triggers for their actions. This case-control study
empirically explores psychodynamic concepts of NSSI by examining quality of
affect, object representations, and social cognition manifest in verbatim Thematic
Apperception Test narratives (Murray, 1943). Sixty-five female borderline inpa-
tients engaging in NSSI served as the case group, while 68 matched female
inpatients with BPD without NSSI served as the control group. The TAT transcripts
were rated on the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale (Hilsenroth, Stein, &
Pinsker, 2007; Westen, 1995), then a priori hypotheses were subjected to statistical
analysis. While both groups functioned in the pathological range on all dimensions,
the NSSI group created narratives reflecting greater expectation of malevolent
treatment from others, expressed interpersonal relationships in more shallow terms
with little capacity for empathy, and had greater difficulty modulating aggressive
feelings. Their narratives also reflected greater difficulty with splitting and boundary
confusion, more primitive relationship schemas, and poorer understanding of social
causality of interpersonal interactions. Results add to a growing body of evidence
that NSSI is associated with more severe forms of BPD pathology, especially in
domains of malevolent object representations, misinterpretation of social interac-
tions, and expressions of hostility (Kernberg, 1984; Simpson & Porter, 1981;
Stolorow & Lachmann, 1980; Yeomans, Hull & Clarkin, 1994). These results
extend observations of the heterogeneity in the severity of patients diagnosed with
BPD.

Keywords: Borderline Personality Disorder, self-injury, affect, social cognition

Rachel Whipple, PsyD, Department of Psychology, The George Washington University; J. Chris-
topher Fowler, The Austen Riggs Center.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to J. Christopher Fowler, The Austen

Riggs Center, 25 Main St., Stockbridge, MA 01262. E-mail: christopher.fowler@austenriggs.net

Psychoanalytic Psychology

© 2011 American Psychological Association

2011, Vol. 28, No. 2, 183–195

0736-9735/11/$12.00

DOI: 10.1037/a0023241

183

background image

Psychiatric patients who engage in nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) such as cutting, burning
or abrading their skin can be quite difficult to treat, in part because the behaviors serve
multiple functions (Nock & Prinstein, 2005), and as a result the behavior is relatively slow
to change over time compared to symptoms and suicide attempts (Bateman & Fonagy,
2008; Doering et al., 2010; Perry et al., 2009). Because borderline patients who engage in
NSSI have great difficulty with affect regulation and interpersonal communication, they
frequently stir intense emotional reactions in those caring for them. It is not uncommon for
therapist to experience great difficulty maintaining a therapeutic stance by being drawn away
from exploration of affective states and cognitions in an attempt to manage behaviors (Plakun,
1994; Fowler, Nolan & Hilsenroth, 2000). The complexities of treatment, the risk factors
associated with this symptom complex, and the rise in prevalence in America’s adolescents
and young adults provided the impetus for the current study.

Currently, the field is inundated with definitions and names for the phenomena under

investigation; however, the most parsimonious and widely accepted term is nonsuicidal
self-injury
(NSSI). Nonsuicidal self-injury refers to the direct, deliberate destruction of
body tissue in the absence of lethal intent (Nock & Favazza, 2009; Nock, Wedig, Janis,
& Deliberto, 2008), and includes behaviors such as self-inflicted cutting, burning, or
abrading skin (not substance abuse or generally acceptable tattooing or body piercing).
Studies estimate between 14 and 39% of adolescents, and 35% of college students report
histories of NSSI (Gratz, 2001; Nock & Prinstein, 2005). Prevalence rates are far higher
among inpatients diagnosed with Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), with estimates
ranging from 50% (Dulit, Fyer, Leon, Brodsky, & Frances, 1994), to 90% (Zanarini et al.,
2008).

Prior research suggests early adverse events are risk factors for NSSI including

physical abuse (Gratz, Conrad, & Roemer, 2002; Simpson & Porter, 1981), early sepa-
ration/loss, and unpredictable childhood environments, (Leibenluft, Gardner, & Cowdry,
1987), and interruption in the attachment process (Simpson & Porter, 1981; Bach-Y-Rita,
1974). Researchers employing interview and self-report measures assessing current per-
sonality functioning report excessive hypervigilance, resentful attitudes, and paranoid
reactions in social and intimate relationships (Yeomans, Hull & Clarkin, 1994), as well as
greater verbal and physical aggressiveness than in other psychiatric patients (Hillbrand,
Krystal, Sharpe, & Foster, 1994; Simeon et al., 1992). This may be related to underlying
deficits in verbal and nonverbal communications skills— BPD patients engaging in NSSI
demonstrate poorer nonverbal communication skills, and have more difficulty communi-
cating and interpreting emotional information than those with BPD without NSSI
(McKay, Gavigan, & Kulchycky, 2004). The intensity and frequency of NSSI is associ-
ated with greater impulsivity, chronic anger, and somatic anxiety, while NSSI and Axis II
comorbidity is associated with greater dysphoric affect, anxiety, anger, rejection sensi-
tivity, and emptiness (Simeon et al., 1992). Borderline patients who engage in NSSI also
suffer from higher rates of de-realization and drug-free hallucinations/delusions (Soloff,
Lis, Kelly, Cornelius, & Ulrich, 1994).

While self-report and interview based studies provide crucial evidence of global

impairment, the study of NSSI is complicated by the fact that more psychologically
impaired patients who engage in the behavior may be reticent to disclose information, or
lack the basic capacity to recognize or express thoughts and emotions that precipitate an
urge to damage their bodies. Thus, relying solely on self-report or interview methods to
understand psychological vulnerabilities associated with NSSI is particularly problematic.
Some research teams circumvent problems associated with self-report bias by applying

184

WHIPPLE AND FOWLER

background image

performance-based methods to assess implicit psychological processes (Baity et al., 2009;
Fowler et al., 2000; Nock, & Banaji, 2007).

In light of the broad-based impairments in affect regulation, interpersonal, and

cognitive domains associated with NSSI (especially in those with comorbid BPD), we
hypothesized that underlying dimensions of social cognition and object relations represent
specific vulnerabilities to NSSI, and could best be assessed using implicit measures of
object-relations and social cognition. We chose the Thematic Apperception Test (Murray,
1943) because it represents a narrative-based implicit measure of personality functioning
that allowed us to explore quality of affect, relationship schemas, and social cognition
imbedded in narrated stories of interpersonal interactions. Applying the Social Cognition
and Object Relations Scale (SCORS: Hilsenroth, Stein, & Pinsker, 2007; Westen, 1995)
to transcribed verbatim narratives provides a unique opportunity to examine implicit
affective representations of relationship schemas, relational expectations, and social and
interpersonal impairment of NSSI.

Hypotheses

We hypothesized that female inpatients with BPD who actively engage in NSSI (the
case group) would narrate stories involving more tumultuous relationships with
others, have more malevolent expectations of relationships, and have more intense,
volatile feelings of aggression compared to a matched control group of BPD female
inpatients (control group). Utilizing ANOVA models we planned two a priori anal-
yses assessing global affective and cognitive factors of the SCORS, and six a priori
analyses of specific domains of social causality and object relations. Within the
domain of object-relations we predicted that the NSSI case group would score more
pathologically on: (a) Affect Tone of Representations, (b) Emotional Investment in
Relationships, (c) Experience and Management of Aggression, and Aggressive Im-
pulses, and (d) Self-esteem. Within the Social Cognition domain we predicted that
patients in the NSSI case group would: evidence greater pathology on the social
cognition variables: (a) Complexity of Representations of People, and (b) Understand-
ing of Social Causality. Because early experiences of trauma (physical and sexual) are
correlated with NSSI, we conducted comparisons between the groups for history of
sexual and physical abuse.

Method

Procedures

Archival records from July 1997 to July 2004 were collected for this study. Patient records
(including identification numbers, diagnostic codes, history of sexual/physical abuse, and
detailed descriptions of specific behavioral manifestations of NSSI) were masked to
disguise patient identity then downloaded from the Center’s database. Diagnoses were
assigned by a board certified and licensed psychologist and psychiatrist according to the
diagnostic criteria of the DSM–IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
utilizing the longitudinal expert evaluation using all data (LEAD: Pilkonis, Heape, Ruddy,
& Serrao, 1991). The diagnosis of BPD was confirmed in 100% of the cases by
independent ratings conducted by a psychiatrist or psychologist as an ongoing aspect of
the hospital’s performance improvement practice— diagnosis of BPD was determined
independent of TAT data.

185

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER AND NSSI

background image

The first author classified behavioral records into NSSI case and control groups by

reviewing all nursing records of the BPD female subjects, prior to rating archival TAT
records. The data extracted from the medical records can be considered a reliable and
accurate representation of patients NSSI activity during hospitalization because the
nursing staff were required to record all incidences of self-inflicted lacerations and burns.
Discriminations between NSSI and suicide attempt were based on the Lethality of Suicide
Attempt Rating Scale-II (LSARS-II: Berman, Shepard, & Silverman, 2003): Suicide
attempts were not coded as NSSI. Ratings were made blind to any identifying information,
eliminating the risk of criterion contamination.

Participants

Criteria for inclusion into this study were as follows: (a) Females at least 18 years of age,
(b) Confirmed diagnosis of BPD, (c) Minimum of 60 consecutive days of hospitalization
in order to obtain a representative sample of patient behavior, (d) Complete battery of
psychological tests during the first 20 days of hospitalization. Inclusion in the NSSI group
required a minimum of two episodes of NSSI within six weeks following test adminis-
tration in order to ensure a pattern of NSSI. The control group required a total absence of
NSSI in three months prior to admission, and no incidence of NSSI during hospitalization.

Measures

The Thematic Apperception Test (Murray, 1943) was developed as a narrative test of
personality functioning consisting of 31 scenic pictorials from which subjects are in-
structed to create narratives about the scenes and human representations. The TAT is one
of the most commonly used performance-based personality assessment technique regard-
less of patient demographics or purpose of the evaluation (Archer, Maruish, Imhof, &
Piotrowski, 1991). The TAT elicits information not readily accessible with self-report
methods because the narratives developed express implicit self and object representations
(schemas), expectations or biases regarding interpersonal interactions, and competence in
understanding social interactions. Therefore, the TAT provides rich data about an indi-
vidual’s capacity for interpersonal relatedness in many situations such as family, work,
friendship, and treatment relationships. The TAT data was collected as part of the
hospital’s routine admission and assessment procedures, and used for the purposes of this
study at a later date. Eight standard TAT cards were administered during the initial
evaluation phase of treatment using the procedures and guidelines articulated by Murray
(1943). The following TAT cards were administered in the same order to all patients: 1,
5, 14, Picasso’s La Vie,

1

13MF, 12M, 2, and 18GF. All TAT narratives were tape-recorded

and transcribed. Protocols were scored blind to identifying information and group inclu-
sion, mitigating criterion contamination.

Westen, Lohr, Silk, Kerber, and Goodrich (1990) developed the Social Cognition and

Object Relations Scale (SCORS) to enable clinicians to look beyond the overt presentation
of the patient and evaluate more implicit, dynamic facets of personality functioning.
Integrating social, cognitive, and psychoanalytic theories, the SCORS assesses social,
affective, and self/object representations. The SCORS is one of the most widely studied
rating system for the TAT. Construct validity of the SCORS dimensions is well estab-
lished (Ackerman, Hilsenroth, Clemence, Wetherill & Fowler, 2001). Convergent validity

1

Following David Rapaport’s original guidelines, the fourth card in the sequence administered

at Austen Riggs is a reproduction of Pablo Picasso’s La Vie.

186

WHIPPLE AND FOWLER

background image

of the SCORS has been established comparing studies of normal and clinical samples
diagnosed with DSM–IV Axis II personality disorders (Barends, Westen, Byers, Leigh, &
Silbert, 1990). Moreover, SCORS ratings effectively differentiate DSM–IV personality
disorders, with BPD subjects scoring in the more pathological range on nearly every
variable when compared to other Cluster B and Cluster C groups (Ackerman, Clemence,
Weatherill, & Hilsenroth, 1999). The SCORS dimensions are predictive of psychotherapy
termination and continuation (Ackerman, Hilsenroth, Clemence, Weatherill, & Fowler,
2000), demonstrate good to excellent ecological validity with global relationship func-
tioning scores (Peters, Hilsenroth, Eudell-Simmons, Blagys, & Handler, 2006) and change
in predictable directions over the course of long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy
(Fowler et al., 2004; Porcerelli et al., 2006).

The most recent version of the SCORS (Hilsenroth, Stein & Pinsker, 2007) was used

in this study and is made up of eight variables scored on a 7-point global rating. Lower
scores (e.g., 1 or 2) indicate severe pathological responses, whereas higher scores (e.g., 6
or 7) are indicative of healthy functioning. Ratings for each SCORS variable were
averaged across eight narratives to create mean score. The following SCORS dimensions
were rated:

Affect-Tone of Representations (AT) assesses an individual’s expectations from others

in relationships and how the patient describes significant relationships. Capacity for
Emotional Investment in Relationships
(EIR) assesses the degree to which representations
reflect a need-gratifying, narcissistic orientation versus an orientation of mutuality and
reciprocity. The Emotional Investment in Values and Moral Standards (EIM) assesses
morality along a continuum from TAT narratives articulating characters who “behave in
selfish, inconsiderate, or aggressive ways without any sense of remorse or guilt” from
those who “think about moral questions in a way that combines abstract thought, a
willingness to challenge or question convention, and genuine compassion and thought-
fulness in actions.” Experience and Management of Aggressive Impulses (AGG) assesses
narrative accounts of character’s ability to control and appropriately express aggression.
Self-esteem (SE) assesses representations of characters in TAT narratives along a contin-
uum from self-loathing to relatively positive without appearing grandiose. Identity and
Coherence of Self
(ICS) assesses the coherence and articulation of character identity from
fragmented and poorly articulated to well-integrated characters with a stable and enduring
sense of purpose. Complexity of Representations (COM) assesses a patient’s psycholog-
ical capacity to represent self and others along a continuum from a lack of differentiation
of self and others, to “split” representations along good and bad dimensions, to integration
of positive and negative attributes of self and others in complex ways. Understanding
Social Causality
(SC) assesses narratives for coherence of characters’ thoughts, feelings
and behavior as logical, accurate, and psychologically minded. In addition to single
dimensions, we computed overall mean scores for the affective/relationships component
(AT

⫹ IER ⫹ AGG) and social cognition component (COM ⫹ SC).

Four raters with varying levels of experience (Undergraduate to PhD psycholo-

gists) performed 10 hours of SCORS training (Hilsenroth, Stein & Pinsker, 2007).
Raters scored a random sample of 25 study protocols consisting of 200 narratives.
Table 1 reports individual subscale reliabilities (ICC

1,1

; Shrout & Fliess, 1979).

Reliability estimates were considered in the “good” to “excellent” range for the
subscales used for assessing hypotheses. Two subscales (Emotional Investment in
Morals and Identity and Self-Coherence) had fair reliability and were therefore were
not included in the analyses.

187

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER AND NSSI

background image

Results

The initial sample consisted of all 141 female borderline patients admitted to the Austen
Riggs Center between July 1997 and July 2004. While all patients with a BPD diagnosis
had multiple Axis I and Axis II disorders, those patients with a comorbid Axis I psychotic
disorder were excluded from the study (n

⫽ 2) as were those who declined the TAT (n

6). The final sample consisted of 133 female with BPD (65 NSSI case and 68 control) who
were predominantly Caucasian (n

⫽ 128), with three Asian Americans, and two African

Americans. Ninety-eight (73%) patients were single, 28 (21%) were married, and seven
(6%) divorced or widowed. Table 2 provides data on demographic and diagnostic
variables for each group and the total sample. Average age at admission was 30.2 years
(SD

⫽ 9.6). Level of education completed was 14.9 years (SD ⫽ 2.6), mean full scale IQ

was 108 (SD

⫽ 14.5). The sample manifest a high degree of comorbidity with an average

of 4.2 Axis I and II diagnoses (SD

⫽ 1.5). Thirty-three (25%) patients had comorbid

diagnoses of BPD and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder, and 119 (89%) patients were
diagnosed with comorbid BPD and Major Depressive Disorders. Global Assessment of

Table 2
Demographic and Diagnostic Variables

NSSI BPD

BPD control

Total sample

(n

⫽ 65)

(n

⫽ 68)

(N

⫽ 133)

Mean (SD) (%)

Mean (SD) (%)

Mean (SD) (%)

Age

29.0 (9.3)

31.8 (9.7)

30.2 (9.6)

Education

15.3 (2.9)

14.5 (2.2)

14.9 (2.6)

FSIQ

107 (15.7)

108 (13.0)

108 (14.5)

Total DX

4.3 (1.7)

4.1 (1.3)

4.2 (1.5)

GAF

40.0 (7.1)

39.9 (7.1)

40.0 (7.1)

MDD

55 (85%)

64 (94%)

116 (89%)

PTSD

13 (31%)

20 (29%)

33 (25%)

Physical Abuse

19 (29%)

18 (26%)

37 (28%)

Sexual Abuse

19 (29%)

20 (29%)

39 (29%)

Note.

FSIQ

⫽ Wechsler Full Scale IQ; Total DX ⫽ Total Diagnosis; GAF ⫽ Global Assessment of

Functioning; MDD

⫽ Major Depressive Disorder; PTSD ⫽ Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder.

Table 1
Interrater Reliability of SCORS Variables (n

25)

SCORS variable

ICC

Complexity of Representations

.73

Affect-Tone

.81

Emotional Investment in Relationships

.77

Emotional Investment in Morality

.61

Understanding Social Causality

.70

Understanding and Containing Aggressive Impulses

.70

Self-Esteem

.72

Identity and Coherence of the Self

.58

Note.

ICC

⫽ Two-way random effects model Intraclass Correlation

Coefficients.

188

WHIPPLE AND FOWLER

background image

Functioning (GAF: American Psychiatric Association, 1994) score at admission indicated
severe functional disturbance (M

⫽ 40; SD ⫽ 7.1).

Analysis of variance contrasting age, years of education, full scale IQ, total number of

diagnoses, and GAF yielded no significant differences between the two groups. Goodman
and Kruskal’s gamma tests were performed on categorical and dichotomous variables to
detect the association among NSSI and major depressive disorders, PTSD, reports of past
sexual and physical abuse. No significant findings emerged for comorbid diagnoses or
history of past trauma.

A series of one-way ANOVAs were conducted to test a priori hypotheses (see Table

3). In addition to F statistic and significance level, we calculated effect sizes, utilizing
Cohen’s d—values approximating .2, .5, and .8 represent small, medium, and large
effects, respectively (Cohen, 1977).

Hypothesis 1

The NSSI group will manifest greater expectation of malevolence in relationships (AT),
express less investment in relationships (EIR), and have greater difficulty modulating
expression of aggression (AGG). The NSSI case group demonstrated greater pathology on
the overall affective factor (F

(1, 131)

⫽ 9.5, p ⬍ .003; Cohen’s d ⫽ .53, 95% CI ⫽

.44 –.63), created narratives that expressed more malevolent expectations and experiences
of relationships (AT: F

(1, 131)

⫽ 7.4, p ⬍ .008; Cohen’s d ⫽ .49, 95% CI ⫽ .36–.61), less

depth and commitment to emotionally investing in relationships (EIR: F

(1, 131)

⫽ 10.7,

p

⫽ .001; Cohen’s d ⫽ .58, 95% CI .45–.70), and expressed greater intensity of

unmodulated hostility and aggression in narrating interpersonal relationships (AGG:
F

(1, 131)

⫽ 6.4, p ⬍ .01; Cohen’s d ⫽ .44, 95% CI .34–.53). The NSSI case group also

manifested greater difficulty with feelings of self-loathing or grandiosity than the control
group (SE: F

(1, 131)

⫽ 4.9, p ⬍ .03; Cohen’s d ⫽ .40, 95% CI .29–.50).

Hypothesis 2

The NSSI group will create narratives that expressed greater boundary confusion between
self and others (COM) and will manifest greater difficulty understanding interpersonal
interactions (SC). The NSSI case group demonstrated greater pathology on the overall

Table 3
Analysis of Variance for SCORS Variables

NSSI case

BPD control

(N

⫽ 65)

(N

⫽ 68)

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

F

p

ES

95% CI

Affect Factor

3.08

.52

3.39

.64

9.5

.003

.53

.44–.63

Affect Tone

2.99

.65

3.35

.83

7.4

.008

.49

.36–.61

Emotional Investment

2.48

.63

2.90

.82

10.7

.001

.58

.45–.70

Aggression

3.19

.54

3.44

.61

6.4

.01

.44

.34–.53

Cognitive Factor

2.82

.66

3.20

.95

6.9

.01

.47

.33–.61

Complexity

2.93

.64

3.27

.91

6.2

.01

.42

.28–.56

Social Causality

2.72

.71

3.12

1.02

7.0

.009

.46

.31–.61

Post-Hoc Analysis

Self-Esteem

3.22

.53

3.46

.68

4.9

.03

.40

.29–.50

Note.

ES

⫽ Cohen’s d effect size.

189

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER AND NSSI

background image

cognitive factor (F

(1, 131)

⫽ 6.9, p ⫽ .01; Cohen’s d ⫽ .47, 95% CI .33–.61), created

narratives that expressed greater boundary confusion between self and others, and more
difficulty integrating positive and negative aspects of self and others (COM:
F

(1, 131)

⫽ 6.2, p ⬍ .01; Cohen’s d ⫽ .42, 95% CI ⫽ .28–.56), and were less skilled in

understanding interpersonal exchanges, reflecting on possible motivations responsible for
others’ behaviors, and predicting how their behaviors may impact others (SC:
F

(1, 131)

⫽ 7.0, p ⬍ .009; Cohen’s d ⫽ .46, 95% CI ⫽ .31–.61).

These findings support the distinctions between BPD inpatient subgroups, and provide

strong evidence in support of psychodynamic formulations of the correlates of NSSI;
however, it could be argued that these differences would not hold up when comparing
SCORS to outpatient BPD records. While we did not have access to an outpatient sample,
we were able to compare the NSSI case group to a BPD outpatient sample served by a
university-based clinic (Ackerman et al., 1999). We computed effect sizes from Acker-
man’s mean and standard deviations and our NSSI case group data. While both groups of
BPD patients functioned in the pathological range, the NSSI group demonstrated greater
pathology on all SCORS dimensions with large effect size (Cohen’s d ranging from 1.0
to 1.6) for all variables except Aggression that demonstrated a medium effect size
(Cohen’s d

⫽ .66). Still further, could the differences in social causality and object

relations evaporate when comparing clinical and nonclinical samples? To address this
question we computed effect sizes from the means and standard deviations

2

from an early

study by Westen and colleagues (Westen, Lohr, Silk, Gold, & Kreber, 1990) examining
Affect Tone, Emotional Investment in Relationships, Complexity of Representations, and
Social Causality from 30 normal controls comprised of screened for serious psychopa-
thology. Here the differences were far more striking with normal controls creating more
positive and benign narratives whereas the NSSI borderline group demonstrated malev-
olently tinged interpersonal narratives (AT: Cohen’s d

⫽ 2.9). Similar large effect size

differences were observed between normal controls and our NSSI inpatient sample on the
dimension of emotional investment in relationships (EIR: Cohen’s d

⫽ 2.3), understand-

ing social interactions (SC: Cohen’s d

⫽ 2.5), and the degree to which characters were

understood to have complex internal thoughts and feeling and were differentiate from one
another (COM: Cohen’s d

⫽ 2.9). In addition to large effect size differences, the group

means for the normal controls were in the healthy range of functioning, whereas the BPD
NSSI group means were clearly in the pathological range of functioning on all dimensions.

Discussion

Individuals living with borderline personality disorder suffer from impairments in impulse
control, emotional lability, self and other differentiation, and interpersonal deficits con-
sisting of impairments in empathy, intimacy, and complexity and integration of represen-
tations of others (Skodol, 2009). We anticipated that our BPD inpatients’ would narrate
stories dominated by themes of stormy, chaotic, and often violent relationship themes with
little capacity to describe the characters in complex or well-differentiated ways. What was
surprising to us was the degree to which those patients who engaged in self-injury during
treatment were significantly more impaired on every SCORS dimension assessed than a

2

Using Dawes’ (2008) formula, we converted the SCORS variables to a 10-point scale due to

unequal scaling of the SCORS—the original SCORS was a 4-point scale, while the 2007 version is
a 7-point scale.

190

WHIPPLE AND FOWLER

background image

well-matched comparison group, yet did not have more Axis I & II disorders, suffer from
greater impairment in symptom functioning (GAF), nor higher rates of sexual or physical
abuse.

Those who engaged in cutting, burning, or abrading their skin manifested greater

expectations of malevolence from others, expressed less investment in interpersonal
relationships, and manifest more blatant hostility and aggression in their relationship
narratives. Expressions of hostility may be related to NSSI patients’ cognitive bias toward
perceiving others as capricious and rejecting. This cycle may ultimately contribute to
increased incidents of NSSI, because these patients have difficulty tolerating emotions and
rely on self-harming as a pathological way to regulate their affect (Haines, Williams,
Brain, & Wilson, 1995; Ivanoff, Linehan, & Brown, 2001; Kemperman, Russ & Shearin,
1997). Researchers and clinicians have suggested that patients may also use self-injury to
discharge anger or rage toward themselves (Freud, 1949; Soloff et al., 1994) or toward
others (Kernberg, 1984; Simpson & Porter, 1981).

The NSSI patients’ underlying vulnerability to blurring self and other boundaries, and

significant deficits interpreting the motivations of others may predispose them to misin-
terpret many interpersonal interactions as hostile, rejecting, and confirming expectations
of malevolence. The cyclical pattern of interpreting other’s ambiguous behavior as
evidence of cruelty, capriciousness, and rejection may represent what Bateman and
Fonagy (2004) refer to as the cognitive mode of psychic equivalence in which fantasies
and personal expectations are experienced as if they are external reality. This may help to
explain the consistent clinical observations that NSSI patients are more sensitive to
rejection (Simeon et al., 1992), frequently resentful, hypervigilant, and paranoid in their
interactions with others (Yeomans et al., 1994) and experience greater difficulty commu-
nicating and understanding emotion-laden communications (McKay et al., 2004).

Our findings related to deficits in affect competence (AT, IER, and AGG) are

consistent with Rosenthal’s (Rosenthal, Rinzler, Wallsh, & Klausner, 1972) interviews
with individuals immediately following an episode of cutting who described an inability
to deal with specific feelings, leading to a state of depersonalization. The subjects in
Rosenthal’s study cut themselves in an effort to reintegrate, and seemed to understand that
cutting helped to reestablish a coherent sense of self.

So what might a clinician take away from the study findings, and what specific

interventions might help the patient deal more effectively with the symptom of self-
injury? First, therapists will be well-served to keep in mind that self-injury tends to change
slowly in comparison to many other symptoms (Perry et al., 2009), in part because the
behavior serves multiple functions, and is likely to be triggered by upsetting interpersonal
events, emotional upheaval, and misinterpretation of interpersonal interactions—some of
which inevitably represent ruptures in reaction to the therapist’s interventions (Safran &
Muran, 2000).

While NSSI is relatively slow to change in comparison to other symptoms, there is

clear and compelling evidence from well-designed randomized clinical trials that several
forms of psychotherapy help borderline patients decrease the frequency of NSSI and other
self-destructive behaviors (Bateman & Fonagy, 2008; Clarkin, Levy, Lenzenweger, &
Kernberg, 2007; Doering et al., 2010; Levy et al., 2006; Linehan et al., 2006). Psychody-
namic treatment protocols emphasize different mutative interventions for the treatment of
NSSI (Levy, Yoemans, & Diamond, 2007); yet, deconstruction studies assessing specific
interventions impact on rates of NSSI and suicide attempts have not been completed.
Despite the current limitations of our knowledge, we can imagine the potentially palliative
effects on social relationships, as well as decrease in NSSI for BPD patients when

191

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER AND NSSI

background image

interventions successfully enhance the patient’s capacity for: (a) self-reflection and
curiosity about emotional precipitants for NSSI, (b) affect modulation during affect
storms, especially in the context of alliance ruptures, (c) alternative explanations for the
internal motivations for the other’s behaviors beyond the patient’s cognitive bias, (d)
stronger self-other boundary differentiation, and (e) thought experiments to consider the
patient’s influence on social exchanges that are problematic. Given the clear impairments
in the BPD, and especially the NSSI case group’s capacity for understanding complex
social interactions, it may be particularly counterproductive to make complex interpreta-
tions of social exchanges or the patient’s mental states when the patient is affectively
overwhelmed, or when they lack the basic capacities to understand complex social
exchanges.

Several manualized treatments are tailored to enhance the above mental skill sets, and

psychodynamic psychotherapy, more broadly conceived, aims to increase self-awareness,
curiosity, and greater understanding of interpersonal relationships; however, to the best of
our knowledge, only Bateman and Fonagy’s (2004) mentalization-based treatment ex-
plicitly targets the specific deficits of affect regulation, and social causality through four
specific strategies: (a) enhancing mentalization

⫺the ability to understand the mental states

of self and others and how those states influence overt behavior, (b) bridging the gap
between affects and their representations, (c) working mostly with current mental states,
and (d) keeping in mind the patient’s deficits (Bateman & Fonagy, 2004, p. 203). Our
findings of the underlying psychological deficits associated with NSSI highlight the
necessity of treatments to enhance these deficits in order to increase the likelihood of
sustained remission and recovery. Recent evidence (Bateman & Fonagy, 2008) supports
this contention—BPD patients in an 18-month mentalization-based treatment arm dem-
onstrated sustained improvements in functioning (suicide attempts, service use, global
psychiatric functioning, and ratings of borderline functioning) at 5-years postdischarge.
This is particularly promising given that most positive effects of psychotherapy treatment
tend to diminish over time.

Transference-Focused Psychotherapy (TFP) has also proven effective in decreasing

self-destructive behavior in the treatment of patients with borderline personality disorder
(Clarkin et al., 2007; Doering et al., 2010). Importantly, TFP transforms underlying
representational structures—Levy and colleagues (Levy et al., 2006) found that patients
randomized to a TFP condition evidenced increased attachment coherence and improved
reflective functioning after one year of treatment, whereas patients randomized to a DBT
or supportive psychodynamic psychotherapy did not. This finding suggesting that TFP
interventions of transference interpretations, embedded in the therapy process, specifically
helps to bring about greater integration of disparate or split-mental representations and
their accompanying affects. Both Mentalization-Based Treatment and Transference-
Focused Psychotherapy appear to be effective treatments for reducing self-destructive
behavior in borderline patients. The mounting evidence for effective psychodynamic-
based treatments in reducing self-destructive behaviors and improving social causality and
quality of object representations for patients with borderline personality disorder is
promising for patients suffering with borderline personality disorders.

Conclusions

This study provides empirical support for specific impairments in NSSI patients object
relations and ego functions. In addition to providing support for the clinical observations

192

WHIPPLE AND FOWLER

background image

and theories about NSSI, these results support the utility and sensitivity of the SCORS
measure in assessing the object relations and social cognition of these complex individ-
uals, as well as supporting the value of attending to patient narratives in order to
understand the underlying dynamics of NSSI. Additionally, findings from this study
demonstrate the vast heterogeneity within the BPD category (Oldham, 2006; Skodol &
Bender, 2009).

References

Ackerman, S. J., Clemence, A. J., Weatherill, R., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (1999). Use of the TAT in the

assessment of DSM–IV Cluster B personality disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73,
422– 442.

Ackerman, S. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., Clemence, A. J., Weatherill, R., & Fowler, J. C. (2000). The

effects of social cognition and object representation on psychotherapy continuation. Bulletin of
the Menninger Clinic, 64,
386 – 408.

Ackerman, S. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., Clemence, A. J., Weatherill, R., & Fowler, J. C. (2001).

Convergent validity of Rorschach and TAT scales of object relations. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 77,
295–306.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Archer, R. P., Maruish, M., Imhof, E. A., & Piotrowski, C. (1991). Psychological test usage with adolescent

clients: 1990 survey findings. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 22, 247–252.

Bach-Y-Rita, G. (1974). Habitual violence and self-mutilation. American Journal of Psychiatry,

131, 1018 –1020.

Baity, M. R., Blais, M. A., Hilsenroth, M. J., Fowler, J. C., & Padawer, J. R. (2009). Self-mutilation,

severity of borderline psychopathology, and the Rorschach. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 73,
203–225.

Barends, A., Westen, D., Leigh, J., Silbert, D., & Byers, S. (1990). Assessing affect-tone in

relationship paradigms from TAT and interview data. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2,
329 –332.

Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2008). 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality

disorder: Mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. The American Journal of
Psychiatry, 165,
631– 638.

Bateman, A. W., & Fonagy, P. (2004). Psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder: Men-

talization based treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Berman, A. L., Shepard, G., & Silverman, M. M. (2003). The LSARS-II: Lethality of suicide

attempt rating scale-updated. Suicide and Life Threatening Behavior, 33, 261–276.

Clarkin, J. F., Levy, K. N., Lenzenweger, M. F., & Kernberg, O. F. (2007). Evaluating three

treatments for borderline personality disorder: A multiwave study. American Journal of Psychi-
atry, 164,
922–928.

Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York:

Academic Press.

Dawes, J. (2008). Do data characteristics change according to the number of scale points used? An

experiment using 5-point, 7-point and 10-point scales. International Journal of Market Re-
search, 50,
61–77.

Doering, S., Hörz, S., Rentrop, M., Fischer-Kern, M., Schuster, P., Benecke, C., Buchheim, A.,

Martius, P., & Buchheim, P. (2010). Transference-focused psychotherapy v. treatment by
community psychotherapists for borderline personality disorder: Randomised controlled
trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 389 –395.

Dulit, R. A., Fyer, M. R., Leon, A. C., Brodsky, B. S., & Frances, A. J. (1994). Clinical correlates

of self-mutilation in borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152,
1305–1311.

193

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER AND NSSI

background image

Fowler, J. C., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Nolan, E. (2000). Exploring the inner world of self mutilating

borderline patients: A Rorschach investigation. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 64, 365–385.

Fowler, J. F., Ackerman, S. J., Speanburg, S., Bailey, A., Blagys, M., & Conklin, A. C. (2004).

Personality and symptom change in treatment-refractory inpatients: Evaluation of the phase
model of change using Rorschach, TAT, and DSM–IV Axis V. Journal of Personality Assess-
ment, 83,
306 –322.

Freud, S. (1949). An Outline of Psychoanalysis. New York: Norton.
Gratz, K. (2001). Measurement of deliberate self-harm: Preliminary data on the deliberate self-harm

inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 253–263.

Gratz, K. L., Conrad, S. D., & Roemer, L. (2002). Risk factors for deliberate self-harm among

college students. American Board of Orthopsychiatry, 72, 128 –140.

Haines, J., Williams, C. L., Brain, K. L., & Wilson, G. V. (1995). The psychophysiology of

self-mutilation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 471– 489.

Hillbrand, M., Krystal, J. H., Sharpe, K. S., & Foster, H. G. (1994). Clinical predictors of self-mutilation

in hospitalized forensic patients. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182, 9 –13.

Hilsenroth, M., Stein, M., & Pinsker, J. (2007). Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale: Global

Rating Method (SCORS-G; 3rd ed.). Unpublished manuscript, The Derner Institute of Advanced
Psychological Studies, Adelphi University, Garden City, NY.

Ivanoff, A., Linehan, M. M., & Brown, M. (2001). Dialectical Behavior Therapy for impulsive

self-injurious behaviors. In Simeon, D. & Hollander, E. (Eds.), Self-injurious Behaviors: As-
sessment and Treatment.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Kemperman, I., Russ, M. J., & Shearin, E. (1997). Self-injurious behavior and mood regulation in

borderline patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, 146 –157.

Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders: Psychotherapeutic strategies. New Haven:

Yale University Press.

Leibenluft, E., Gardner, D. L., & Cowdry, R. W. (1987). The inner experience of the borderline

self-mutilator. Journal of Personality Disorders, 1, 317–324.

Levy, K. N., Meehan, K. B., Kelly, K. M., Reynoso, J. S., Weber, M., Clarkin, J. F., & Kernberg,

O. F. (2006). Change in attachment patterns and reflective function in a randomized control trial
of transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 74,
1027–1040.

Levy, K. N., Yeomans, F. E., & Diamond, D. (2007). Psychodynamic treatments of self-injury.

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 1105–1120.

Linehan, M. M., Comtois, K. A., Murray, A. M., Brown, M. Z., Gallop, R. J., Heard, H. L.,

Korslund, K. E., Tutek, D. A., Reynolds, S. K., & Lindenboim, N. (2006). Two-year randomized
controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behavior therapy vs. therapy by experts for suicidal
behaviors and borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 757–766.

McKay, D., Gavigan, C. A., & Kulchycky, S. (2004). Social skills and sex-role functioning in

Borderline Personality Disorder: Relationship to self-mutilating behavior. Cognitive Behavioral
Therapy, 33,
27–35.

Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test: Manual. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University

Press.

Nock, M. K., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). Prediction of suicide ideation and attempts among adolescents

using a brief performance-based test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 707–715.

Nock, M. K., & Favazza, A. (2009). Non-suicidal self-injury: Definition and classification. In M. K.

Nock (Ed.), Understanding non-suicidal self- injury: Origins, assessment, and treatment (pp.
9 –18). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2005). Contextual features and behavioral functions of self-

mutilation among adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 140 –146.

Nock, M. K., Wedig, M. M., Janis, I. B., & Deliberto, T. L. (2008). Self-injurious thoughts and

behaviors. In J. Hunsely & E. Mash (Eds.), A guide to assessments that work (pp. 158 –177). New
York: Oxford University Press.

194

WHIPPLE AND FOWLER

background image

Oldham, J. M. (2006). Borderline Personality Disorder and Suicidality. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 163, 20 –26.

Perry, C. J., Fowler, J. C., Bailey, A., Plakun, E. M., Speanburg, S., Zheutlin, B., & Clemence, A. J.

(2009). Improvement and recovery from suicidal and self-destructive phenomena in treatment-
refractory disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 197, 28 –34.

Peters, E. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., Eudell-Simmons, E. M., Blagys, M. D., & Handler, L. (2006).

Reliability and validity of the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale in clinical use.
Psychotherapy Research, 16, 606 – 614.

Pilkonis, P. A., Heape, C. L., Ruddy, J., & Serrao, P. (1991). Validity in the diagnosis of personality

disorders: The use of the LEAD standard. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 3,
46 –54.

Plakun, E. M. (1994). Principles in the psychotherapy of self-destructive borderline patients. Journal

of Psychotherapy Practice & Research, 3, 138 –148.

Porcerelli, J. H., Shahar, G., Blatt, S. J., Ford, R. Q., Mezza, J. A., & Greenlee, L. M. (2006). Social

Cognition and Object Relations Scale: Convergent validity and changes following intensive
inpatient treatment. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 407– 417.

Rosenthal, R. J., Rinzler, C., Wallsh, R., & Klausner, E. (1972). Wrist-cutting syndrome: The

meaning of a gesture. American Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 1363–1368.

Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeutic alliance. New York: Guilford

Press.

Shrout, P., & Fleiss, J. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psycho-

logical Bulletin, 86, 420 – 428.

Simeon, D., Stanley, B., Frances, A., Mann, J. J., Winchel, R., & Stanley, M. (1992). Self-mutilation

in personality disorders: Psychological and biological correlates. American Journal of Psychi-
atry, 149,
221–226.

Simpson, C. A., & Porter, G. L. (1981). Self-mutilation in children and adolescents. Bulletin of the

Menninger Clinic, 45, 428 – 438.

Skodol, A. E. (April, 2009). Report of the DSM-V Personality and Personality Disorders Work

Group. Retrieved from http://www.psych.org/MainMenu/Research/DSMIV/DSMV/
DSMRevisionActivities/DSM-V-Work-Group-Reports/Personality-and-Personality-
Disorders-Work-Group-Report.aspx

Skodol, A. E., & Bender, D. S. (2009). The Future of Personality Disorders in DSM-V? American

Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 388 –391.

Soloff, P. H., Lis, J. A., Kelly, T., Cornelius, J., & Ulrich, R. (1994). Self-mutilation and suicidal

behavior in Borderline Personality Disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 8, 257–267.

Stolorow, R. D., & Lachmann, F. M. (1980). Psychoanalysis of Developmental Arrests: Theory and

Treatment. New York: International Universities Press, Inc.

Westen, D. (1995). Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale: Q-sort for projective stories.

Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University Medical School, Boston, MA.

Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K., Gold, L., & Kreber, K. (1990). Object relations and social cognition

in borderlines, major depressives, and normals: A Thematic Apperception Test analysis. Psy-
chological Assessment, 2,
355–364.

Yeomans, F. E., Hull, J. W., & Clarkin, J. C. (1994). Risk factors for self-damaging acts in a

borderline population. Journal of Personality Disorder, 8, 10 –16.

Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Reich, D. B., Fitzmaurice, G., Weinberg, I., & Gunderson, J. G.

(2008). The 10-year course of physically self-destructive acts reported by borderline patients and
Axis II comparison subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 117, 177–184.

195

BORDERLINE PERSONALITY DISORDER AND NSSI


Wyszukiwarka

Podobne podstrony:
(psychology, self help) Shyness and Social Anxiety A Self Help Guide
(autyzm) Hadjakhani Et Al , 2005 Anatomical Differences In The Mirror Neuron System And Social Cogn
Revealing the Form and Function of Self Injurious Thoughts and Behaviours A Real Time Ecological As
Human Relations and Social Responsibility
Making Contact with the Self Injurious Adolescent BPD, Gestalt Therapy and Dialectical Behavioral T
Family and social life Relationship Frendship, love and sex (tłumaczenie)
Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition Jost, Glaser,Kruglanski and Sulloway
Self Injurious Thoughts and Behaviors Interview
Family and social life Relationship Marriage (55)
cognitive and social sides of epistemology
Physiological Arousal, Distress Tolerance, and Social Problem Solving Deficits Among Adolescent Self
Family and social life Relationship Marriage (tłumaczenie)
Culture, Trust, and Social Networks
Bussey, Bandura Social Cognitive Theory Gender Development
business groups and social welfae in emerging markets
No Man's land Gender bias and social constructivism in the diagnosis of borderline personality disor
The Relationships?tween Semiotics and The Quaker Company

więcej podobnych podstron