Lecture XIV
Lecture XIV
First language (L1)
acquisition
November 14th, 2008
Structure of the lecture
Structure of the lecture
Chronology of L1 acquisition
Babbling
Chronology of L1
Chronology of L1
acquisition
acquisition
Canonical babbling
Canonical babbling
Begins at the age of six months
Children experiment with and gain
control over their vocal apparatus
Children manifest significant
cross-linguistic similarities in their
babbling
Cross-linguistic
Cross-linguistic
similarities in babbling
similarities in babbling
Cross-linguistic
Cross-linguistic
differences in babbling
differences in babbling
Sound frequency plays an important
role in babbling, e.g. in English the
fricative // appears late, while in
Estonian, Bulgarian or Swedish the
sound is acquired much earlier
Ingram (1999) suggests that the
function a given sound fulfils in the
phonology of a language plays a crucial
role in the acquisition process.
Early phonetic processes
Early phonetic processes
Syllable simplification – reduction of
consonant clusters:
- /s/-deletion – stop, small, desk
- liquid deletion – try, crack, bring, from,
sleep
- nasal deletion – bump, tent
Substitution – the process of systematic
replacement of one sound by another that
the child finds easier.
Substitution in child
Substitution in child
language
language
David Stampe (1979: 1) defines the term phonological process as
“a mental operation that applies in speech to substitute, for a
class of sounds or sound sequences presenting a specific
common difficulty to the speech capacity of the individual, an
alternative class identical but lacking the difficult property”.
According to natural phonologists, the functioning of
phonological processes is observable in the speech of children
acquiring their first language or, more precisely, at the early
stages of the process as “in its language-innocent state, the
innate phonological system expresses the full system of
restrictions of speech: a full set of phonological processes,
unlimited and unordered”
In the process of acquiring adult pronunciation, the child has to
overcome some serious articulatory obstacles and he/she does
that by means of suppression, limitation and ordering of some of
the natural tendencies.
Phonological substitutions in
Phonological substitutions in
lamb
lamb
Underlying representation
/ /
Adult form
Denasalisation
Devoicing
Gliding
Frication
Processes Stopping
Assimilation
Neutralisation
Surface form [ ]
Child’s form
(adapted from Shriberg – Kwiatkowski 1980: 11)
Strong innateness hypothesis
Strong innateness hypothesis
The founders of Natural Phonology claim that only
children at the very beginning of first language
acquisition have access to all processes
In the course of the acquisition period, the
processes undergo suppression, limitation and
ordering under the influence of the input the child
receives from his/her native language environment.
The suppressed processes that are no longer
needed in the speaker’s native language lie
dormant and can, but do not have to, be activated
in second language acquisition (Dziubalska-
Kołaczyk 2002a: 35)
Morphological
Morphological
development
development
Morphological development emerges
over the period of a few years.
Developmental order:
-
case-by-case learning
-
overuse of general rule
-
mastery of exceptions to the general
rule
Overgeneralisation vs. overregularisation
Developmental sequence
Developmental sequence
– bound morphemes
– bound morphemes
1.
-ing
2.
Plural –s
3.
Possessive -‘s
4.
The, a
5.
Past tense –ed
6.
Third person singular –s
7.
Auxiliary be
Determining factors
Determining factors
Frequent occurrence in utterance final
position
Syllabicity
A straightforward relation between
form and meaning
Few or no exceptions in the way it is
used
Allomorphic invariance
Clearly discernible semantic function
Word formation rules
Word formation rules
Derivational affixes and compounding
are also acquired in a fixed order.
-
agentive – er
-
compounds
-
adjectival – y
-
instrumental – er
-
adverbial – ly
Syntactic development
Syntactic development
The one-word stage (holophrases): one word
stands for a whole meaningful utterance,
e.g. Daddy – ‘I see daddy’.
The most informative word is always chosen,
e.g. I want up is usually expressed as ‘up’.
Two-word stage – in the vast majority of
cases employ the correct word order
The telegraphic stage: utterances lack
bound morphemes and most non-lexical
categories
The development of phrase
The development of phrase
structure
structure
Holophrastic – 1 – 1.5 years
Two-word – 1.5 – 2 years
Telegraphic – 2 – 2.5 years
Later – 2.5 years up
Development of question
Development of question
formation
formation
Rising intonation
Inversion: note *Can he can look?
Wh questions – takes place between 2 –
4 years
Three stages:
-
Where that?
-
What he did yesterday?
-
Where should I go?
Semantic development
Semantic development
The first 50 words:
1.
Names for people: mummy, daddy, etc.
2.
Words referring to:
-
humans: baby
-
Food and drink: juice, milk, water, bread, apple
-
Animals: cat, dog, duck
-
Clothes: dress, shoes, hat
-
Toys: ball, doll, blocks
-
Vehicles: car, boat
-
Other: bottle, key
3.
Properties: hot, cold, dirty
4.
Actions: go, see, sit
5.
Personal-social: bye, yes, no, thank you
Determinants of language
Determinants of language
acquistion
acquistion
Imitation and correction
Correction
The role of adult speech
The features of motherese
The features of motherese
Slow speech
Higher pitch
Exaggerated intonation and stress
Longer pauses
More restricted vocab
Concrete reference to here and now
Fewer incomplete sentences
Short sentences
More commands and questions
More repetitions