movie monsters
1
The Psychological Appeal of Movie
Monsters
Stuart Fischoff, Ph.D., Alexandra Dimopoulos, B.A., François Nguyen, B.A.
California State University Los Angeles
Media Psychology Lab
and
Rachel Gordon
Executive Editor, Journal of Media Psychology
Online Publication Date: August 25, 2005
Journal of Media Psychology, Volume 10, No. 3, Summer, 2005
movie monsters
2
ABSTRACT
A nationwide sample of 1,166 people responded to a survey exploring choices for
a favorite movie monster and reasons why a monster chosen was a favorite. The sample
was comprised of equal but culturally diverse numbers of males and females. Ages
ranged from 16 to 91. Results of the study indicated that, for both genders and across age
groups, the vampire, in general -- and Dracula in particular -- is the king of monsters..
With a few exceptions (women found vampires and the Scream killers more sexy and
ranked the demon doll, Chucky, significantly higher than males), males and females were
generally attracted to the same monsters and for similar reasons. As predicted, younger
people were the more likely to prefer recent and more violent and murderous slasher
monsters, and to like them for their killing prowess. Older people were more attracted to
non-slashers and attracted for reasons concerned with a monster's torment, sensitivity,
and alienation from normal society. While younger people also appreciated the classic
film monsters such as Frankenstein and King Kong, a parallel cross-over by older
respondents for more recent monsters, like Michael Myers, was not reciprocated.
Overall, though, monsters were liked for their intelligence, superhuman powers and their
ability to show us the dark side of human nature.
movie monsters
3
The Psychological Appeal of Movie Monsters
The open secret of why films have been so popular for over 100 years, in venues
ranging from the 2-dimensional, black and white, silent films viewed on five-inch screens
of the turn-of-the-century nickelodeons to the 21
st
century, stories-high, 3D IMAX
screens which fully immerse audiences in the booming high fidelity, color-saturated
action, may lie less in cinematic technology than in what film does for its viewers. Film
appeals to viewers' appetites for an extraordinary, vicarious experience, and the
convulsion of emotions that it so often delivers.
Given previous results of research on differential viewer reactions to films from
differing film genres (cf., Fischoff, 1997, Kaplan and Kickul, 1996), different film genres
may be expected to provide different vicarious and emotional experiences. In the case of
horror films, it is believed to be the thrill of fright, the awe of the horrific, the experience
of the dark and forbidden side of human behavior that lures people into the dark mouth of
the theater to be spooked (cf., Zillmann, Weaver, Mundorf, and Aust, 1986).
According to statistics provided by the online archives of the industry newspaper,
Variety, of the 250 or so top grossing films released by the American film industry each
year, approximately 15 films (6%) are of the horror genre. Numerous academics and non-
academics have written extensively on the topic of horror films, movie monsters, all-time
scary films, and the like. Some, like Michael Apter (1992) and his theory of detachment
and parapathic emotions, have looked at theoretical reasons why people seem to enjoy the
ostensibly negative experience of being frightened by a movie experience. According to
Apter, potential for escape into safe distance is paramount. Others, like Zillmann et al.,
(1986), for example, have looked at why horror films are good "date" movies or, like
Jonathan Crane (1994), have outlined how the horror genre has changed over the years,
evolving into a type which is far more violent and explicitly bloody. Researchers like Ed
Tan (1996) have demonstrated that film emotions are not ersatz stepchildren of authentic
emotions. Rather, film-induced emotions are themselves real experiences because the
film, in collusion with the audience eye and audience desire to be transported, can fool
the brain. In other words, a horror film can be “really scary” -- if we allow it!
movie monsters
4
Since the early part of the 20
th
century, when the horror film genre was born,
scary movies have developed into different clusters of themes. Silent film era horror
films, primarily European, were a mixed bag of legends and science fiction (e.g.,
Metropolis (1926), Nosferatu (1922), The Golem(1920), Edison's Frankenstein short
(1910), and The Cabinet of Dr. Caligari (1919)). Following the era of silent films came
the now-legendary era of the sympathetic monsters of the 1930's, as exemplified by
Universal Studio’s monster triumvirate, Frankenstein, Dracula, and the Mummy, and
their spin-offs and sequels. According to Crane (1994), these monsters were generally
seen as misunderstood outcasts from society, to be pitied, and even occasionally, as with
Dracula, found to be attractive. Audiences are said to have identified with these monsters
that were portrayed as existing on the outside of the normal community. Perhaps the
monsters’ onscreen plights tapped into audience feelings of social inequity and
recollection of social torment at the hands of their social peers.
The 1950s was awash in science fiction-fantasy monster pictures addressing such
things as science run amok, fear of alien invaders (The Thing [From Another World],
1951, Invasion of the Body Snatchers, 1956) and the disastrous and unanticipated
consequences of radioactive fallout (e.g., Them, 1954 and Godzilla, 1956). The late
1950s brought a new wave of monsters directed toward a different audience than those
who sought out monsters in the earlier decades. The new audience was the youth market,
and the monsters and their monstrous behaviors addressed the sensibilities of young
males and females. According to Skal (1993), this transition from multi-generational
appeal of films in general, and of horror films in particular, to a principally youth-
oriented market developed as the buying power of the young began to increase in the late
1950s. Beside youth oriented dramas like Rebel Without A Cause (1955), the angst of
adolescence was further explored with films like I Was A Teenage Werewolf (1957) and I
Was A Teenage Frankenstein (1958). Here, the monsters were not “the other” but, “us,”
possibly in all our adolescent hormonal rage and confusion. Filmmakers Roger Corman
and Sam Arkoff, among others, opened up a treasure trove of box office dollars by
appealing to this hungry market of young filmgoers and the youth-oriented film market
made its move to become the 800 pound behemoth it is today.
movie monsters
5
Hollywood, helped by the collapse of the old Hayes or Motion Picture Code in the
1960s, issued itself the license to shock, titillate and nauseate. This was coupled with
advances in the technology of special effects, and assured that the old tradition of almost
sanitary, often unseen horror, and gradual, enveloping, suspense was traded in for a new
tradition of horror, one of shock and blood-drenched gore, all to the delight of this
bulging youth market (Baird, 2000; Crane, 1994). Extremism in the pursuit of the
monster box office by monsters on screen became a mantra, not a vice. It is no wonder
that the cinematic vehicles for these emerging horror icons ran with blood, guts, and free-
standing heads and limbs. "The central focus are scenes that dwell on the victim's fear
and explicitly portray the attack and it's aftermath" (Weaver & Tamborini, 1996, p.38).
The slasher movie had arrived. Examples of slasher killers include Michael Myers from
Halloween (1978), Freddy Krueger from A Nightmare on Elm Street (1985) and
Leatherface from The Texas Chainsaw Massacre (1974).
As horror films became more gruesome, more explicit, more horrifying than
terrifying, more shock than suspense, the older audiences began to stay away in droves.
According to research by Fischoff (1998), a trend could be observed: as viewers age,
their appetite for violence decreases and their attraction to the new, bloodier horror genre
decreases as well. A momentum begins and, in response, Hollywood shifts from
targeting adult market audiences to targeting primarily teen market audiences. Box office
goals dictate that such movies increasingly and violently focus on the plights of young
people, thereby further alienating middle age and older moviegoers, and further locking
the horror genre into what has become the youth culture juggernaut.
Film monsters have proven to be such unforgettable characters that in many
instances they have become part of our culture. Most Americans would recognize a
picture of Frankenstein, Dracula, King Kong, Godzilla or the Mummy before recognizing
a Supreme Court Justice. Like so many popular culture figures, these monsters have
become such recognizable icons, either through novel characterizations and product
merchandising, or through repeated film presentations on TV or via home video. But this
phenomenon is not exclusive to monsters from older films. Freddy, Jason, and Michael
have made their bid for fright “immortality.”
movie monsters
6
These newer slasher monsters are very different from cinematic horror monsters
of the past. As social climates, film technology, and local and national film policies
changed, so did monsters, in form, behavior and sensibilities. Are changes always
welcome? Are these changing monster types appealing to some groups and not to others?
Cowan and O’Brien (1990) found that in slasher monster films, the slashers are primarily
men, and sexy women were more likely to die than non-sexy women. Males on the other
hand, were targets for death if they possessed negative masculine traits (their sexual
allure didn’t matter).
One might expect that females would be put off more by slasher monsters than
males because women are punished for being sexual while men are merely punished for
being arrogant, pushy, or selfish, suggesting an implicit equation between female
sexuality and negativity. Yet, Fischoff (1994) found that although females and males do
not differ in their attraction to the horror film genre, they do differ in their attraction to
violence, males liking violence in movies more than females do. Are females less
attracted to violent movie monsters as well?
Further, does age matter? Do people of different ages wax nostalgic about
different monsters? If so, why? In other words, what makes horror monsters attractive
and what makes them unattractive, to different age groups, to different genders? It is
likely the case that, when analyzing people’s attraction to the “stars” of this genre called
Horror, one size does not fit all. If people are attracted to movies because of what
emotions it invites in them, what biographic resonances it incites (Fischoff, 1978), what
vicarious payoffs are meted out, it would be of interest to students of the genre as well as
filmmakers who keep the genre’s pipeline gurgling and churning.
Sadly, there is little empirical research on what or who are people’s favorite
monsters and what reasons underlie such affections. No study of which the present
authors are aware has systematically sampled a national population for their individual
preferences on these movie monster matters. This study was designed to explore our
favorite monsters and why we feel connected to them. It also sought to explore the
following research questions and hypotheses derived from the abundant but solely
speculative literature on horror movies and movie monsters.
Hypotheses:
movie monsters
7
H
1
. Young people will prefer more recently conceived movie monsters while
older people will prefer vintage film monsters.
H
2
. Young people will prefer film monsters that are more violent and disposed to
killing large numbers of people than will be older people.
H
3
: Young people, rather than older people, will be more likely to prefer film
monsters that are attractive because of their killing inclinations.
H
4
: Males will prefer more violent movie monsters than Females.
Research Questions:
RQ
1
. What are the favorite film monsters?
RQ
2
. Do males and females differ in terms of the specific monsters they find
favorites?
PROCEDURE AND METHODOLOGY
Members of the Media Psychology Lab
1
at California State University, Los
Angeles under the direction of the first author conducted a year-long, nation-wide survey
of (among other things) the preferences people have for certain movie monsters. Data
collection took place between September 2000 and August 2001. A variety of direct and
indirect contact venues were employed to garner responses from academic and
nonacademic settings. This resulted in a cross-sectional, convenience sample of 1,166.
This included 597 females, 567 males, and two individuals who withheld gender
information.
The participants ranged in ages from 6 through 91 with a mean age of 34.2. The
total number of people who were classified as “young,” (25 years or younger), “middle”
(26-49 years) and "older"(50+) is 531, 371 and 253 respectively. The sample, therefore,
is skewed toward younger respondents. These three age range categories were found to
be highly effective for comparing age groups in previous research on film preferences (cf.
Fischoff, 1998). For those respondents who filled out the long version of the survey, the
age distribution was even less representative of those over 50 (n = 38), further attenuating
1
We would like to thank Ana Franco, Angela Hernandez and Leslie Hurry for
their assistance on this research.
movie monsters
8
the proportionate presence of older respondents in our sample. Respondents came from
the four major racial/ethnic groups, Caucasian, Asian, Hispanic and African-American.
Ethnicity was included to assure sample representativeness rather than as an intended
independent research variable.
The survey questionnaire was developed over a number of open-ended pilot
studies to elicit a range of items addressing reasons for individual monster preferences
2
.
People were asked to respond to potential monster preference reasons on a 4-point Likert-
type Scale ranging from 0 (no influence) to 3 (very influential). The final survey
contained 43 closed-ended reasons for liking a monster. In order to collect more data on
favorite movie monsters citations when people did not have the time to fill out a long list
of reasons behind the selection, a short version of the survey was designed and
administered in rapid response street interviews.
Slasher Monsters
For purposes of specific hypothesis-related analyses, all monsters cited by
respondents were classified into one of two categories: slasher or non-slasher. The
operational definition of a slasher monster in the present study was that it was portrayed
on screen as a serial or mass murderer, motivated by some deluded or self-justifying
revenge or outrage. It was also necessary that the murders committed by the monster
generally were unrelated to the monster’s actual survival needs (e.g., vampires and blood
needs). Further, a slasher monster should be portrayed as generally experiencing no
remorse for its murderous rampages. Monsters that murdered for reasons such as fear,
survival, or procreational needs and were not necessarily mass murderers, were classified
as non-slashers.
Examples of familiar slasher monsters are Freddie Krueger from A Nightmare on
Elm Street series of films, and Chucky, the demon doll from the Child’s Play series.
Examples of familiar non-slasher monsters are Frankenstein, Dracula and Gill Man from
the Creature From the Black Lagoon film series.
2
The survey also looked at scariest films. That data will be presented at a later
date.
movie monsters
9
The determination of a monster as a slasher or non-slasher was derived from
judgments and assessments of film monsters by academics authors such as Crane (1994),
Pinedo (1993), Twitchell (1985), and Cowan & O’Brien (1990), comments obtained
during pilot studies, and the judgments and observations of the research team. Two
members of the research team decided into which category a monster would fall and, if
no agreement was obtained, a third team member helped decide the classification. In
95% of the cases, there was no such disagreement. Of the 1, 038 monsters cited, the total
number of monster citations falling into the slasher category is 290 while 748 fall into the
non-slasher category,
χ
2
(1, N = 1,038) = 202.1, p < .001.
Adapting a data reduction procedure employed by Wilkins (2000), an additional
classification of the 43 reasons for a monster being a favorite was used for ease of
interpretation. Forty-two of the 43 reasons were collapsed into 9 Scales. The number of
the scales, their rank in terms of frequency of citation, and their meaning, are presented in
Table 1. Reasons were placed in scale categories on the basis of shared dimensions of
surface meanings. For example, there are two items which comprise the Self-Reference
Scale (Scale 1): Reason 2 (“monster reminds me of myself”) and Reason 36 (“I first
experienced the monster as a child”). Of particular interest in the present study and its
hypotheses, Scale 6, Dimensions of Killing, contained nine items with reference to such
reasons as “monster enjoys killing” (Reason 11) and “monster kills lots of people”
(Reason 14).
movie monsters
10
Table 1.
Reasons and Rankings for Monsters Being Favorites
All Monsters
Scale
Number
Rank Scale
Meaning
1
9
viewer autobiographic reference
2 4
monster's
appearance
3 3
positive psycho-social
characteristics of monster
4 1
negative psycho-social
characteristics of monster
5 6
supernatural
powers
6
5
dimensions of killing
7
7
enlightenment provided by monster
8 8
sex/romance/attractive
9
2
empathy, pity, compassion for
monster
For analytic purposes, each respondent’s score on a scale was the sum of the
scores on each of the reasons comprising the scale. Recall that each reason could range
in score from 0-3. Using reason sums allows for comparisons between monsters on each
scale, but not for comparisons between scales because scales varied in number of
component reasons. The number of items comprising a scale ranged from 2 to 9.
RESULTS
Data was collected from a respondent pool of 1,166. However, as only a subset of
this total answered the long form of the survey with the 43 reasons, the N for analysis of
reasons why a person liked a particular monster was limited to 700 survey protocols
while the tally of favorite monsters is based on an N of 1,034.
movie monsters
11
Favorite Monsters
Tables 2, 3, and 4 contain, respectively, the “Top 25” favorite monsters for All
Respondents, for Males and Females separately and for Young, Middle and Older
Respondents separately.
Table 2.
Frequency Citations for “Top 25” Monsters for All Respondents
All Respondants
Monster
Rank
Vampires (Dracula)
1
Freddy Krueger
2
Godzilla 3
Frankenstein 4
Chucky 5
Michael Myers (Halloween)
6
King Kong
7
Hannibal Lecter
8
Jason Voorhees (Friday 13
th
) 9
Alien (Alien series)
10
Exorcist girl (Linda Blair)
11
Scream killers
12
Predator 13
E. T.
14
Mummy 15
Darth Vader
16
Shark from Jaws
17
It (The Clown)
18
Jack Nicholson (The Shining)
19
Werewolf 20
Blob 21
Gill-Man (Creature From the Black Lagoon)
22
The Thing
23
Beast (Beauty and the Beast)
24
Candyman 25
movie monsters
12
All Respondents
There were 205 different, favorite individual movie monsters cited ranging from
Freddy Krueger, Frankenstein, and the Werewolf to facetiously nominated outliers such
as Shelley Winters, Barbra Streisand, and Michael Jackson. Ten percent (132) of
respondents reported no favorite movie monster. Of that number, 78 were female (60%)
and 54 were male (40%). Thus, 6.7% of females and 4.6% of males had no favorite
monsters,
χ
2
(1, N = 1,166) = 3.63, p< .06. While previous research (Fischoff, Antonio,
and Lewis, 1997) has shown that females and males do not significantly differ in terms of
preference for films of the Horror genre, results from this study suggests that they do
differ in terms of likelihood of having a favorite movie monster.
Age and Vintage of Favorite Monster
H
1
predicted that young people will prefer more recently conceived movie
monsters while older people will prefer monsters of an earlier vintage. The correlation
between the average age of the respondent selecting a monster and the year that the film
introducing the monster was initially released (or, if the there were many sequels, the
average year of release of the sequels) is r = -.63, p < .001; as the monster film’s release
year increases, the average age of the respondent selecting it decreases, supporting H
1
.
Taking another angle of regard and looking more closely at the 25 most favorite
monsters in terms of citations frequencies, Table 3 contains data arranged according to
the average or mean age of respondents selecting one of these monsters, as well as the
standard deviation of the age mean. The monsters are ranked from the lowest mean age
of respondents selecting them to the highest mean age.
movie monsters
13
Table 3.
Mean Age of Respondents Choosing Individual Monsters
Monster
Mean Age of
Selecting
Respondents
SD
Film Source
Release Year
Scream killers
s
18.4 4.9
1996
It (The Clown)
s
19.7 2.9
1990
Candyman
s
20 2.3
1992
Chucky
s
21.9 6.5
1988
Michael Myers
s
22.7 5.5
1986
Exorcist Girl
24.2
10.6
1973
Freddy Krueger
s
24.6 8.8
1989
Jason Voorhees
s
25.8 11.9
1987
Hannibal
s
28.8 12.8
1996
Predator
s
29.5 12.6
1988
Darth Vader
29.6
13.7
1980
Jaws shark
32
8.9
1975
Beast (Beauty and the Beast) 33.1
20.7
1991
Werewolf (Chaney)
33.5
17
1941
Vampire (Dracula-Lugosi)
33.6
16.7
1931
Godzilla (Japanese)
34.3
14.7
1975
Alien Creature
35.6
13.2
1982
Terminator 36.9
18.1
1988
The Thing
37.1
16.3
1951
Mummy (Karloff)
39.9
21.9
1932
Blob (The Blob) 40.6
16.3
1958
E.T. 44.1
15.9
1982
Frankenstein (Karloff)
44.3
20.5
1933
Jack Nicholson (The Shining) 46.4
14.8
1980
Gill-Man (Black Lagoon) 48.9
18.1
1954
King Kong
52
18.1
1933
s = slasher monster
It is clear that, with minor exceptions, monsters from the 1980s and 1990s
dominate the top or younger domain of the list, and monsters from the 1930s through the
1950s, again with minor exceptions, occupy the bottom or older domain of the list. In
other words, younger respondents, who dominated the sample, were partial to more
movie monsters
14
recent vintage movie monsters while older people, who were in the minority of the
sample, were partial to earlier vintage movie monsters.
Looking at the adjacent standard deviation (SD) statistics (degree of dispersion of
individual scores around the mean of all the scores) in Table 3, another trend emerges.
The five monsters which topped the list (Scream Killers, It [The Clown], Candyman,
Chucky, Michael Myers) fall into the slasher monster category. These slasher have both
the lowest average age (ranging from 18.4 to 22.7) of respondents selecting them, and the
lowest SDs (e.g. SD = 2.9 when compared with a mean of 44.3 and a SD of 20.5 for
Frankenstein). By contrast, the more classic non-slasher monsters have yielded data with
higher average ages but also have larger SDs. The implication here is that earlier, more
classic Hollywood monsters have a broader age appeal than do later Hollywood
monsters.
Monster preferences by respondents in the Middle Age range show the broadest
generational straddle, finding monsters from the ‘30s to the ‘80s very appealing but, with
the exception of Hannibal Lecter, finding few ‘90s monsters with any appeal.
Monster and Violence
Age and Monster Violence
H
2
predicted that young people will prefer more violent, slasher-type monsters
than will older people. Results provide strong and consistent support for this prediction.
As predicted, 45.4% of younger people cited monsters classified as slashers while the
figures were 21% and 9.7% for Middle and Older people respectively,
χ
2
(2, N = 1,034) =
104.59, p < .001. Each age group was significantly different from each other and in the
expected direction.
movie monsters
15
Table 4.
Frequency Citations for “Top 25 Monsters for All Age Groups
Young
Middle
Older
Monster Rank
Monster Rank
Monster Rank
Freddy Krueger
1
Vampires
1
Vampires
1
Vampires 2
Godzilla
2
Frankenstein
2
Chucky 3
Frankenstein
3
King
Kong
3
Michael Myers
4
Freddy Krueger
4
Godzilla
4
Godzilla 5
Alien
5
E.
T.
5
Jason Voorhees
6
Shark from Jaws
6
Mummy
6
Hannibal Lecter
6
King Kong 7
Jack
(The
Shining)
7
Exorcist Girl
8
Jason Vorhees
8
Gill-Man
7
Scream Killers
9
Hannibal Lecter
9
Alien
7
Frankenstein 10
Michael
Myers 10
Freddy
Krueger
10
It (The Clown)
11
Chucky
10
Hannibal Lecter
10
Predator 12
Predator 10
Blob
10
Candyman
13
Jack (The Shining)
10
The Thing
10
Mummy 14
E.
T.
10
Jason
Vorhees
14
Darth Vader
14
Blob
10
Werewolf
14
Beast (Beauty and) 14 Darth
Vader
16 Predator
14
Alien 17
Werewolf
17
Exorcist
girl
14
Werewolf 18
Mummy
17
Darth
Vader
14
King Kong
19
Gill-Man
19
Beast (Beauty and)
14
E. T.
19
Exorcist girl
19
Michael Myers
0
The Thing
19
The Thing
19
Chucky
0
Shark from Jaws
22
It (The Clown)
22
Shark from Jaws 0
Gill-Man 22
Beast
(Beauty
and)
22
Scream Killers
0
Jack (The Shining) 24 Scream Killers
24
Candyman
0
Blob
24
Candyman
25
It (The Clown)
0
Gender and Monster Violence
When it comes to gender, results were opposite to that predicted in. It was
predicted in H
4
that males would prefer the more violent and rapacious movie monsters,
the slashers as it were. Surprisingly, results show that females, not males, cited the
movie monsters
16
higher percentage of slasher movie monsters, 34.5% compared with a male citation
percentage of 27.9%,
χ
2
(1, N = 1,034) = 5.23, p < .02. This is a complete reversal of
prediction.
Table 5.
Males and Female “Top 25” Monster Rankings Based on Citations
Frequencies
Monsters Cited - Males
Rank
Monsters Cited - Females
Rank
Vampires 1
Vampires
1
Godzilla 2
Freddy
Krueger
2
Frankenstein 3
Godzilla
3
Freddy Krueger
4
Chucky
3
King Kong
5
Frankenstein
5
Michael Myers
6
Hannibal
6
Alien 7
Michael
Myers
7
Jason Voorhees
8
Exorcist girl
7
Hannibal 9
Jason
Voorhees
9
Predator 9
Scream killers
10
Darth Vader
11
King Kong
11
Chucky 12
E.T. 12
Mummy 12
Alien 12
Jack (The Shining) 14 It
(Clown)
14
Exorcist girl
14
Mummy
15
Jaws 16
Beast
(Beauty and) 15
E.T. 16
Jaws
17
The Thing
16
Werewolf
18
Werewolf 19
Predator 18
Gill Man (Black Lagoon) 19
Candyman
18
Blob 19
Blob
18
Scream killers
22
Jack (The Shining) 22
It (Clown)
22
Gill Man (Black Lagoon) 22
Candyman 24
Darth
Vader
24
Beast (Beauty and) 25 The
Thing
24
Similar gender results are obtained when viewed from a slightly different angle.
Table 5 shows the top-ranked monsters cited by males and females. The Spearman rank
movie monsters
17
order correlation for male and female rankings of Top 25 Monsters is significant, r
s
= 68,
p < .001. Males and females essentially tallied similar lists of favorite monsters with
minor exceptions in terms of the ranking of certain monsters, specifically Chucky (ranked
4
th
for females and 12
th
for males) and Regan, the possessed girl in the original Exorcist,
played by Linda Blair (ranked 7
th
for females and 14
th
for males). Furthermore, females
were about 40% more likely to mention vampires than males and twice as likely to
mention killers from Scream.
But, other than shifts in rankings, males and females were effectively in
agreement when it came to favorite monsters. H
4
- females will prefer less violent
monsters than males - seems to have found no support. Moreover, in response to RQ2,
“do males and females differ in terms of the monsters they find favorites?” The answer
appears to be not much.
What differences there are between males and females may be more readily found
when looking at reasons for selecting a monster as a favorite.
Rationales Behind Favorite Monster Choices: Scale Scores
Space limits a detailed presentation and discussion of the nine scale variables
developed for the present study. Focus will be confined principally to Scale 6,
Dimensions of Killing. H
3
predicted that, compared with older people, younger people
will be more likely to prefer film monsters that are attractive because of their killing
inclinations. Results cited above concerning H
2
established that there is essentially a
negative relationship between age and preference for very violent film monsters such that
as age goes up, preference for violent, murderous film monsters goes down. But as to the
reasons for preferring a monster, comparisons between genders and between age groups
are instructive.
Gender
Scale 6 of the 9 scales developed for analysis of reasons concerns all items
concerned with reasons underlying dimensions of killing, e.g., monster enjoys killing,
kills many people, kills deserving people, etc. ANOVAS reveal significant differences
between age groups and genders in scores on this scale. Males were significantly more
likely to favor monsters because of their killing capacity than were females, t (577.5) =
1.99, p < .05. Males had a mean score on Scale 6 of 9.83, SD = 5.82 while females had a
movie monsters
18
mean score of 8.91, SD = 5.94. Thus, although females were somewhat more likely to
prefer monsters that were classified as slashers, they were somewhat less likely to prefer
them for their wide range of killing parameters as expressed in Scale 6. Instead, women
were more likely to prefer monsters because of positive psycho-social characteristics
(Scale 3), e.g., monster has a sensitive side or shows compassion.
Age
Regarding age, an ANOVA for age on Scale 6 yielded a statistically significant
effect for age, F( 2,687)= 13.55 p < .001. A post hoc comparison of significance of
differences between Young, Middle and Older age group means supports the hypothesis,
M
young
= 10.07, SD = .27, M
middle
= 8.33, DF = .42, M
older
= 6.09, DF = .86. Each age
group is significantly different from each other at alpha levels greater than .05. Thus, age
and preference for a monster because of the variety of ways and the variety of types of
people it kills were, as predicted, inversely related. Hence data support prediction of H
3:
young people prefer monsters more for their dimensions of killing than do older people.
Slashers and Non-Slashers
Looking the data in terms of how citers of slasher and non-slasher monsters
scored on Scale 6, results indicate that, as might be expected, persons who selected
slasher monsters, such as Freddy Kreuger or Michael Myers, had significantly higher
scores on the Dimensions of Killing scale than those who selected non-slashers, such as
Dracula or Godzilla. Slasher citers obtained a mean score on Scale 6 of 12.27, SD = 4.35
while Non-Slasher choosers had a mean score of 7.84, SD = 6.03, t(627.8) = 11.01, p <
.001.
Rationales Behind Favorite Monster Choices: Reason Scores
All Monsters
Table 6 contains an overall rank ordering of reasons why a monster was chosen
as favorite. Ranking was derived from computing the mean of all respondents rating of
that reason as it applied to their monster choice. The top five reasons have nothing
explicitly to do with degree of monster murderousness. Rather, the qualities of
intelligence, superhuman strength, embodying pure evil, not being inhibited or morally
constrained, and showing us the dark side of human nature, garner the most appeal.
Thus, a theme running across most monster preferences concerns issues regarding evil,
movie monsters
19
absence of moral inhibition, and an exploration of the dark side of human nature. Only in
later reasons offered do we find dimensions of killing to be of primary importance. But,
for young men, recall, this is an extremely potent rationale.
Table 6.
Rank Ordering of Reasons for a Monster Being a Favorite
Mean Rank
Code
Meaning
2.08 1
superhuman
strength
1.97 2
very
intelligent
1.84
3
monster is pure evil
1.8
4
monster is not inhibited or morally constrained
1.79
5
shows us dark side of human nature
1.78
6
monster enjoys killing
1.74
7
monster never ages or dies
1.74
7
monster is an outcast
1.7 9
looks
realistically
horrifying
1.64
10
monster kills lots of people
1.64
10
monster kills good people
1.61
12
monster acts out of self-protection or rage
1.6
13
monster has serious psychological problems
1.59
14
never know who monster is going to kill
1.57
15
monster has own subculture
1.57
15
monster has a sense of humor
1.52
17
helps us understand evil
1.48
18
I enjoy being frightened and this monster really frightens me
1.45
19
helps us understand insanity
1.44
20
monster is misunderstood by society
1.42
21
monster has a sensitive side
1.41
22
can't control his violence
1.33
23
monster can disguise its evil ways
1.3
24
monster can alter his/her body shape
1.3
24
monster can take control of victim's minds
1.16
26
like different ways monster kills people
1.14
28
I like what the monster wears
1.01
29
monster consumes human flesh/blood
movie monsters
20
1
30
monster is compassionate
0.99
31
reflects ancient myths
0.93
32
monster can fly or levitate
0.87
33
monster can become invisible
0.87
33
monster can read a person's mind
0.8
35
turns victim into monster
0.75
36
monster reminds me of myself
0.71
37
reassures me there's life after death
0.71
37
monster is sexy, charming
0.69
39
can have sex whenever he/she wants
0.67
40
kills deserving teenage males
0.65
41
kills deserving teenage females
0.55
42
experienced it first as a child
0.42
43
like the way monster uses humans for reproduction
Reasons by Gender
Male and female participants answered with similar reasons as to why a monster
was their favorite. When analyzed by multiple t-tests, in only three instances were
significant differences revealed. But three reasons out of 43 being significantly different
could have easily occurred by chance and only one of the three reasons was related to
violence while the other two dealt with identification. Males were more likely to explain
their selection of Godzilla (M
males
= 1.15, M
females
= 0.25, t(691) = 3.42, p<.001). and
King Kong (M
males
=1.71, M
females
= 0.43, t(691) = 2.14, p<.05) because they felt the
monsters “reminds me of myself.” As regards reasoning related to violence, Chucky was
selected by males more often than females because “I like the way the monster kills
people” (M
males
= 2.67, M
females
=1.00, t(691) = 6.5, p<.001). All other monster
comparisons showed little difference or no discernible pattern of differences in selected
reasoning among males and females. Consequently, Hypothesis 4 predicting that males
would be less attracted to monsters that were violent than would females was not
supported by the present data.
movie monsters
21
Reasons by Age
Hypothesis 3 predicted that young people would prefer film monsters for
different, more violent reasons, than older people. Four items address the issues
surrounding killing or dimensions of killing: Monsters enjoy killing, monsters kill lots of
people, monsters kill deserving teenage males, and monster kills deserving teenage
females. Results of an ANOVA of the mean of the sum of these four variables supports
the prediction. In contrast to respondents of middle and older age ranges, younger
respondents find a monster attractive because of the numbers of people it kills and who in
particular it kills to a significantly greater degree F(2,683) = 11.29, p < .001. A post hoc
analysis revealed the differences between Older (M = .77) and Younger (M = 1.24) to be
significant and that between Younger and “Middle” (M = .96), to be significant. The
differences between Older and Middle, while in the predicted age direction, were not
statistically significant. Older people, by contrast, found reasons of social rejection and
alienation to be the bulwark for their monster preferences.
Reasons by Individual Monsters
Table 7 displays the mean scores on all 43 reasons for the Top 10 Favorite
Monsters. Recall the scores o each reason can range from 0 to 3. Any mean score less
than one would indicate that that reason was not particularly important in that monster
being considered a favorite. Hence, data discussion will generally be restricted to reasons
with mean scores above 1.
movie monsters
22
Table 7
Mean Reason Scores* for "Top 10" Favorite Movie Monsters
Reason
Vampire
N = 134
Freddy
Krueger
N = 87
Frankenstein
N = 61
Jason
Voorhees
N = 29
Michael
Myers
N = 37
Godzilla
N = 75
Chucky
N = 39
Hannibal
Lecter
N = 30
King
Kong
N
=34
Alien
N =
26
turns victim
into monster
2.24
0.49 0.29 0.4
0.16
0.22 0.75 0.56 0.23
1.1
monster
reminds me of
myself 0.95
0.46
0.79
0.43 0.25 0.68 0.28 0.84
1.07
0.47
reassures me
there's life
after death
1.26
0.72 0.68 0.9
0.62
0.49 0.97 0.24 0.08
0.27
monster is
compassionate 1.33 0.28
2.25
0.4 0.28 1.2 0.59
1.08
2.5
0.33
monster is
pure evil
1.72
2.54
0.66
2.5 2.75
1.31
2.56 2.12
0.54 1.73
monster never
ages or dies
2.48
2.19 0.79 2.7 2.47
1.93
2.47
0.68 0.85
1.33
monster has
own subculture
2.39
1.24 0.54 1.1
1.19
1.56 1.22 1.44 0.92
2.13
monster has a
sensitive side
1.97
0.49
2.25
0.4 0.42
1.74
0.78
1.68 3
0.6
Monster has a
sense of
humor 1.5
2.17
1.61 0.75
0.38
1.22
2.19
1.96 1.9
0.33
monster is not
inhibited or
morally
constrained
1.9
2.12
1.18 0.95
1.97
1.54
2.06 2.28
1.77 1.5
monster
enjoys killing
1.69 2.35
0.64
2.55 2.53 1.8
2.53 2.44
0.62 1.47
Monster is
sexy, charming
1.86
0.26 0.07 0.2 0.5
0.41 0.22 1.12 0.62
0.33
like different
ways monster
kills people
0.96
1.81
0.39
2.1 1.56
1.05 1.31 1.48
0.46 1.44
monster kills
lots of people
1.53
2.28
0.61
2.85 2.64 1.78
2.37
1.76 1.1
1.6
kills deserving
teeage
females 0.76
1.26
0.14
1.5
0.97 0.68 0.84 0.32 0.23
0.33
kills deserving
teeage males
0.72
1.25
0.39
1.45
1.06 0.71 0.81 0.4 0.46
0.33
superhuman
strength
2.36
2.15 1.79 2.55 2.47 2.32 1.91 0.84 2.15
2.4
monster can
become
invisible 1.19
1.25
0.5 1.1
0.5
0.41
0.47 0.4 0
0.27
helps us
understand
evil 1.77
1.25
1.5
1.8 1.41
1.22
1.62
2.08
1.1 1.2
helps us
understand
insanity 1.28
1.81
1.32 1.95
2.13
0.88
1.97
2.24
0.62 0.87
movie monsters
23
shows us
where science
and
technology can
go wrong
0.78
0.87
2.14
0.6 0.56
2.02
1.44 0.64
1.23
1.94
I enjoy being
frightened and
this monster
really frightens
me 1.46
1.96
0.86 1.8
2.25
1.32
1.94
1.6 0.85
2.1
monster has
serious
psychological
problems
1.32
2.29
1.21 2.2
2.75
0.78
2.43 2.36
0.62 0.6
monster kills
good people
1.72 1.9
1.03
2.25 2.15 1.5
2.37
1.6 0.77
1.82
monster
consumes
human
flesh/blood
2.33
0.91 0.29 0.6
0.28 1.02 0.56 2.56
0.23 1.13
monster can
alter his/her
body shape
2.23 1.76
0.36 0.45
0.13
0.66
1.19 0.36 0 1.8
never know
who monster is
going to kill
1.67
1.97
1.03
2.35
1.81 1.56 2.19 1.52 1.15
2.47
looks
realistically
horrifying 1.27
2.43
1.59 1.95
2.1
1.88 2.16 0.84 1.54
2.93
monster is an
outcast
1.84 2.13
2.21
1.8
2.41
1.88 2.22 1.46 2.15
0.73
can't control
his violence
1.84
1.09
2.1
1.25 1.41 2.1 1.41 1.16
1.54
0.87
monster is
misunderstood
by society
1.78
0.85
2.11
1.1 1.41
1.71
0.94
1.54
2.85
0.8
monster acts
out of self-
protection or
rage 1.57
1.62
1.66 2.1
1.69
2.38
1.97 1.44
2.37
1.47
monster can
disguise its evil
ways
2.1
1.26 0.43 1.15
0.84
0.8
2.25 2.24
0.38 0.6
can have sex
whenever
he/she wants
1.69
0.72 0.43 0.4
0.09
0.5 1.1 0.63 0
0.27
shows us dark
side of human
nature
2.28
1.99 1.43 1.85
2.66
1.24 1.94 2.36
1.31 1.1
experienced it
first as a child
0.5 0.41 0.71
0.3 0.28 0.83 0.47 0.52 0.77
0.27
reflects ancient
myths 1.8
0.66
0.57
0.95
0.66 1.17 1.03 0.48 1.15
0.47
very intelligent
2.55
1.74 1.1 1.81
1.69 1.46 2.25 2.76
1.69
2.33
monster can
take control of
victim's minds
2.26 2.18
0.32 1.1
0.34
0.59
1.88 1.88 0
0.47
monster can
fly or levitate
2.19
1.07 0.18 0.35
0.06
0.76 0.53 0.2 0
0.47
movie monsters
24
monster can
read a
person's mind
1.54 1.65
0.14 0.75
0.19
0.27
0.47 1.36 0
0.33
like way
monster uses
humans for
reproduction
0.69 0.35
0.27
0.35 0.09 0.29 0.63 0.28 0
1.53
I like what the
monster wears
1.97
1.22 0.25 1.15
1.81
0.54 1.44 0.79 0.33
0
The highest mean reason scores in each row are printed in bold. Suffice it to say
here that on matters concerning killing dimensions, slasher monsters generally score
highest, something already seen in the presentation of results regarding Scale Scores,
especially Scale 6.
Using highest scores in each column to provide a thumbnail characterization of
each monster’s most salient characteristics which contributed to their being a sample
favorite, interpretation of results suggest the following:
1. Vampires engage viewers most because of their intelligence and because they
never die or age. They are also the sexiest of all monsters, with Hannibal Lecter coming
in second. Vampires share another commonality with Hannibal, their taste for humans
although Hannibal is noted for sins of the flesh while Vampires drink -- not wine, but
blood.
2. Freddy Krueger, one of the slasher monsters, is principally highlighted as
“pure evil,” but a close second is that he is “realistically horrifying.”
3. Frankenstein scores high on compassion and sensitivity and the fact of is
being both an outcast and an example of where science can go wrong.
4. The most outstanding feature of slasher Jason Voorhees, of Friday the 13
th
is
that he is an unstoppable killing machine. His cornicopic feats of slicing and dicing a
seemingly endless number of adolescents and the occasional adult is impressive to his
fans. He scores the highest of all 10 monsters on all relevant killing variables comprising
Scale 6, with a mean score of 13.52. His closest rival is Freddy Krueger scores only
12.29 on this Dimensions of Killing variable. Considering that his body count does not
compare with those of Godzilla or the creature from Alien, it is an impressive
accomplishment to be effectively anointed the King of Killers. Jason’s audience appeal
movie monsters
25
is further abetted by his immortality, his apparent enjoyment of killing and by his
superhuman strength.
5. Michael Myers, of Halloween fame, is, like Jason Voorhees, a slasher monster
extraordinaire. While also the embodiment of pure evil, he also stands apart from others
in his highest score for serious psychological problems, M = 2.75. Michael’s closest
rivals are Chucky, M = 2.53 and Hannibal, M = 2.36. Why Michael is seen as more
troubled than Jason is not discernible from the data. That it may be related to his
witnessing his sister having sex in the introductory episode of this franchise, which
putatively led to his psychotic reaction and murder of his sister, is one possibility.
6. Godzilla’s most dominant features are that it acts out of self-protection or rage
and that it has superhuman strength and that, a result of atomic testing in the Pacific, is a
product of science and technology going very wrong. Its destruction of cities and its
inhabitants seems “understandable” in this light given her favorite status. Godzilla scores
third lowest in penchant for killing, higher only than King Kong and Frankenstein.
7. Another slasher monster, Chucky, the demon doll from the Child’s Play series,
was, it may be recalled, picked three times as often by females as males. Enjoying
killing, having serious psychological problems, and embodying pure evil are his principal
virtues for being a favorite. But, like some other monsters, such as Vampires, Hannibal
and Alien, Chucky is considered quite intelligent and appealing for that reason.
8. Hannibal Lecter, the most recently minted of the Top 10 monsters, lacks
totally any supernatural “gifts,” but his intellectual appeal is the highest of all, M = 2.76.
Likewise, because of his non-supernatural status, he provides his admirers with an
appreciation of the workings of an insane mind. His mean score on this reason of helping
us understand insanity is 2.24. Lecter’s closest rival on this variable was Michael Myers
whose mean score on this reason is 2.13. And, top scorer again, Hannibal is viewed as
the monster who is least morally inhibited or constrained. Thus, Hannibal’s
supernaturally unadulterated, sardonic cannibalism makes him a righteous target for
judgment about human failings. At the same time, such failings seem to be part and
parcel of his audience appeal.
9. King Kong is the king of the sensitivity and the recipient of the most pity. He
is rated as having the most Sensitive Side of all the 10 monsters, M = 3.0 is the Most
movie monsters
26
Misunderstood, M = 2.85 and the Most Compassionate, M = 2.5. He virtually ties with
Godzilla as having his violence justified because he acts out of self-protection and rage.
10. Finally, the creature from Alien is a favorite in large measure because one
never knew the monster was going to kill (“everybody” was usually a safe bet) and
because it was so horrifying looking, M = 2.93. Freddy Krueger’s score on this reason,
M = 2.43, was a distant second. Intelligence was also a strong point. So, intelligence,
unpredictability and sheer physical horrific all teamed up to place the lizard mother from
Alien in the current pantheon of movie monsters.
DISCUSSION
Previous research has shown that females are less likely than males to prefer
movies that show violence and gore. The present study found no evidence for consistent
and systematic differences between the genders in terms of monster choices. Therefore,
no support for our third hypothesis could be confirmed regarding greater or lesser
preferences for murderous monsters. Nevertheless, the study did find that females were
less likely to have a favorite monster than males. It may be that females, particularly the
younger women and those who did choose a monster and enjoy horror films, are just as
"blood thirsty" a cast of viewers as their male counterparts. Further research might
examine if other factors influence the responses of female subjects such as the presence
of a self-reliant, briefly victorious, female protagonist in the horror films chosen such as
Sigourney Weaver’s Ripley in the Alien series and the women who live to scream another
day in another film, in so many slasher horror films, women such as Jamie Lee Curtis in
Halloween I and II.
In terms of favorite monsters, most emphatically the fictional vampire species
in general, and Bela Lugosi’s Dracula in particular, is the king of the netherworld of
film monsters. This may be due to the timeless nature of the story as well as the
countless vampire remakes that have flooded cinema over the years and the popularity of
imitating Bela Lugosi. Moreover, F. W. Murnau’s Bram Stoker rip-off, Nosferatu,
notwithstanding, Dracula has most often been played by very attractive men who serve to
increase the sexual power of the character in prowess and romance. This has served to
boost the monster’s appeal for both men and women, although women, understandably
rated vampires significantly more sexy than did males, M
male
= 1.29, M
female
= 2.11,
movie monsters
27
t(49.2) =3.03 p < .001. In the Top 10 list of monsters, for all respondents, vampires were
rated as the sexiest (M = 1.86), followed a distant second by Hannibal Lecter (M = 1.13).
With high ratings on looks and brains, and an appeal to both men and women, it is
perhaps no wonder that the vampire (and Dracula especially) is the most popular movie
monster.
Why is a serial murderer like Dracula considered sexy and attractive?
Psychological research has reaffirmed conventional wisdom repeatedly in studies
showing that we are more ready to empathize with and excuse handsome or beautiful
people when they commit crimes than is our disposition when it comes to the non-
handsome, the non-beautiful defendants. (Stewart, 1980). We also tend to attribute more
positive personality characteristics to attractive people (Tesser, 1995). Even in
“monsterland,” it seems, it pays to be beautiful or handsome --and sympathetic! Recent
films on the Ann Rice literary creation, Vampire Lestat, prompt both sexual and
sympathetic responses from fans of her books and the derivative movies.
Vampires have an additional virtue … of sorts. As a western society, to a likely
neurotic degree, we fear aging and death and the Vampire character is ideally tempting in
both regards. He never ages and never dies completely. He also has supernatural powers
that may be appealing to those who feel powerless.
The results of our study support the hypotheses that younger moviegoers prefer
more recent horror film monsters and are far more partial to slasher monsters than are
older moviegoers. Results also support our hypothesis that younger viewers prefer a
newer generation of horror monster and, it may be surmised, a cinematic style and
storyline drenched in the sensational and novel forms of bloodletting and mortal dispatch
favored by the likes of Freddy, Michael, Jason and their brethren of evil. Slasher -
monster storylines place the acts of murder in the foreground. What little there might be
in terms of rationale for the seemingly endless orgies of death in which these films
commerce, is unceremoniously relegated to the background, as if to say, “Why ask why?”
Contemporary monsters might easily be considered psychopathic in their
bloodlust as they eschew even a scintilla of remorse. In other words, older monsters
struggled with their stature as deviants and killed for survival (Dracula), out of fear-
induced rage (Frankenstein), search for a loved one (Karloff’s Mummy) or, as with the
movie monsters
28
Wolf Man, bestial possession. This monster quartet, in their original film appearances,
often yearned for the deliverance of death. Indeed, according to Crane (1994), it was
Lugosi’s 1931 Dracula who uttered the plaintive lines “To die. To really be dead. That
must be glorious.”
But the modus operandi of contemporary monsters? Contemporary monsters
seem to kill because…they kill. Even though brief psychological explanations are given
in various modern slasher films, such as revenge against their original aggressors, they do
not seem to stand as important during the subsequent installments. These psychological
motivations, mentioned in the premier episodes of the series films seem obligatory rather
than substantive and frequently exercises no palpable influence on character motivation
or behavior. It is not surprising then, that in subsequent iterations of the movie
(sometimes called sequels), motivation for murder all but disappears.
These changes in film styles (quick, bloodless dispatch vs. explicit slaughter) and
reinvention of the formulae for character motivation (existential despair vs. sheer
nihilism) may reflect altering value trends in popular culture. Or, they may bespeak a
culture which itself was or is still reflecting social and political nightmares in
contemporary society, as has been argued by Crane (1994), Pinedo (1997) and Waller
(1987).
Yet, whether legendary horrormeisters such as George Romero, Tobe Hooper or
Wes Craven were (and are) speaking for a post-Viet Nam war, politically cynical
generation, as they have claimed in film interviews, or are merely people of a generation,
is a moot point. But the explicitness of their filmic violence in the 70s seemed to be
echoed in the 80s and 90s in other horror franchises series like Scream and the Hannibal
Lecter oeuvre, and in the explicitness of music lyrics, body adornments, piercings, and
the clothing styles for the culture of youth. Lecter continues to mimic the prevailing
culture of America, both civilized and savage. He looks, according to David Skal (1996),
very much like us. A monster for the millennium, Lecter wears his evil on the inside not
on his face. His disfigurement is spiritual, not physical. He is Jeffrey Dahmer meets
Norman Bates with a seductive panache of Wall Street’s Gordon Gekko.
For once, it seems, Hollywood got it right. The respondents in our survey saw
Lecter as pure evil; not evil in looks, but in deed and conscience. Thus, evil is the
movie monsters
29
charming gentleman next door, not the freak in the circus or the drooling psychotic off
his meds. In the tradition of Pogo, we’ve come to know the monster and the monster is
very often, us.
Lecter still carries on the tradition of Freddy and Jason. He kills for pique and
pleasure, gamesmanship, hunger and lust, not for moral outrage, self-protection or
persecution. He is remorseless and asks the viewer to share, or at least overlook his
peculiar tastes. Charm, Hollywood would have us believe, excuses almost everything.
Lecter’s insouciant airs make him a monster for the the 21
st
century and beyond—Id
incarnate. Greed is good and murder can be fun…and filling too. As Crane (1994)
would have it, “violence in the contemporary shocker is never redemptive, revelatory,
logical, or climactic (it does not resolve conflicts.)” (p. 4). Violence simply is.
There is clearly a cultural as well as generational gap between those under 25 and
those over 40. The horror films and favorite monsters reflect this gap. So did results
from research by Fischoff and his students on favorite film quotes (Fischoff et al, 2000),
which indicated that young people favored more violent and vengeful quotes from
movies than did older respondents.
CONCLUSIONS
In general, different monsters are adored for different reasons but, overall,
characteristics such as superhuman strength, intelligence, luxuriating in the joy of being
evil and being unfettered by moral restraints, are some of the most popular reasons
favored by the sample. Moreover, monsters are admired for holding a mirror up to our
darker sides and assisting us in understanding evil. Perhaps it is the evil that we fear
lurks in all of us, the evil that, in reality, dares not show its face or speak its name. But it
is an evil that does dare parade itself across the movie screen for our vicarious enjoyment
and delectation.
Beyond what a monster may show us about ourselves and our darker side, our
results indicate that what monsters must do above all is behave horrifically and evoke in
us extreme emotions, especially the adrenalized emotion of fear. Looking scary is useful
as well. Moviegoers also relish their monsters displaying such positive traits as
compassion, sensitivity, humor, and intelligence. Regardless of age, members of all age
groups in this study, in varying degrees, liked characters who were sympathetic because
movie monsters
30
of their afflictions and torments. Moreover, the supernatural powers that the monster
possesses are attractive. Our modern and classic literature and legends show that we
humans fantasize about having powers beyond the normal. Whether we’re rooting for
Superman or Dracula, good or evil, superhuman powers are an audience favorite.
It is worth noting that over 90% of the people who cited classic monsters who
were reprised in modern remakes, specified their favorites to be the original, not the
remakes. Remakes tend to disappoint. Remakes of films such as Godzilla, The Thing
and King Kong, for example, were each singled out for particular rejection by
respondents. The myriad of actors portraying Dracula over the decades once Bela
Lugosi’s star went into decline, including such notables as Jack Palance, Christopher Lee,
Frank Langella and, most recently, Gary Oldman, seemed to carry on the tradition of the
romantic vampire, but Lugosi’s Dracula was still the most frequently mentioned
incarnation.
A closing thought about the monster preferences of the young versus the older
viewer. Younger viewers do celebrate the riot of blood and dismemberment unleashed
by contemporary film monsters. But it must be noted that the more classic film monsters
have appeal across generations - an appeal far broader than the appeal of later monsters.
Modern respondents clearly like classic monsters. They like them almost as much as do
older respondents and, as evidence shows, for many of the same reasons: outsider,
misunderstood, sympathetic, frightened, and compassionate. Perhaps those qualities are
most exquisitely represented in the monster who is taken from his home, placed in an
environment he doesn’t understand and is brought to his iconic demise because of the
love for but not of a woman—King Kong. Kong is a monster with whom people of all
generations can identify and sympathize. And the youth of today is no exception.
Remarkably, though, it would appear that younger movie goers have another set
of criteria that they invoke for the modern movie monsters, the Freddys, the Michaels, the
Jasons: who they kill, how they kill, and how often they kill counts for a lot, and the
bloodier, the better.
This mass murderer dimension of monster appreciation is largely absent from the
metrics and aesthetics employed by older respondents. This may reflect a co-existing set
of preferences in younger minds that they handle easily, a set of tastes that straddle
movie monsters
31
generations of popular culture and film monsters. Jenkins (2000) offers the suggestion
that violent entertainment like this serves four functions for young people including
fantasies of empowerment, of transgression, intensification of emotional experience, and
acknowledgement that the world is not always a safe, friendly place. This youthful
juggling act, this plasticity of filmic preference, may both astonish and offend older
people but it’s one that younger people have come to find rather normal. Whether it
means something deeper and more disturbing about real life tolerances for rape and
murder and real life appetites of younger viewers for death sports and snuff films, is open
to speculation.
When these younger viewers approach middle age, whether they continue to find
such explicit violence and mayhem as appealing as they do now is another open question.
Research cited earlier suggests that time alters such appetites. But perhaps times have
changed and, like greed on Wall Street, a monster mired in murder, mutilation and
mayhem will remain an allure not to be outgrown but, rather, a timeless source of an
evening’s entertainment for the entire family.
movie monsters
32
REFERENCES
Apter, M. (1992). The psychology of excitement, in The Dangerous Edge, The Free
Press-Macmillan, New York,
Baird, R. (2000).The Startle Effect, Film Quarterly, Spring, p.p.1-18 ,
Cantor, J. (1994). Fright reactions to mass media, in Media effects: Advances in theory
and research, J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Erlbaum Publishing, Hillsdale, NJ,
pp. 213-245,
Cantor, J., & Oliver, M.B. (1996). Developmental differences in responses to horror, in
Horror films: Current research on audience preferences and reactions, J.B.
Weaver III & R. Tamborini, (Eds.), Erlbaum Publishing, Mahwah, NJ,
Cowan, G. & O’Brien, M. (1990). Gender and survival vs. Death in slasher films: A
content analysis, Sex Roles, 23: 3, p.p 187-196.
Crane, J. L. (1994). Terror and everyday life: Singular moments in the history of the
horror film, Sage Publishing,: Thousand Oaks, Ca,
Fischoff , S., Cardenas , E., Hernandez , A. Wyatt, K., Young, J. and Gordon, R. (August
2000). Popular movie quotes: Reflections of a people and a culture, Paper
presented at annual meeting of APA, Washington, D.C, ,.
Fischoff, S. (August, 1998). Favorite Film Choices: Influences of Beholder and the
Beheld, Paper presented at annual meeting of APA Convention, San Francisco.
Fischoff, S., Antonio, J., & Lewis, D. (August, 1997). Favorite films and film genres as a
function of race, age, and gender, Paper presented at annual meeting of APA,
Chicago.
Fischoff, S. (August, 1994). Race and sex differences in patterns of film attendance and
avoidance. Paper presented at annual meeting of APA, Los Angeles.
Jenkins, H. (Winter 2000). Lessons from Littleton: What Congress doesn’t want to hear
about youth and media, Independent School.
http://www.nais.org/pubs/ismag.cfm?file_id=357&ismag_id=14
(accessed
90/19/02).
Kaplan, M. & Kickul, J. (May, 1996). Mood and extent of processing plot and visual
information about movies, Paper presented at annual meeting of Midwestern
Psychological Association, Chicago,.
movie monsters
33
Pinedo, I. (1997). Recreational terror: Women and the pleasures of horror film viewing,
State University of New York Press, Albany, N.Y.
Sparks, G. & Miller, W. (1998), as cited in Purdue News,
http://news.uns.purdue.edu/uns/html4ever/981016.Sparks.films.html,.
Stewart II, J. E. (1980). Defendant’s attractiveness as a factor in the outcome of trials,
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, p.p. 348-361.
Skal, D. (1993). The Monster Show: A cultural history of horror, W. W. Norton & Co.:
New York.
E. Tan, E., (1996). Emotion and the structure of narrative film, Erlbaum Publishing,
Mahwah:, N.J.
Tesser, A. (1995). Advances in social psychology, McGraw-Hill Publishing, New York,
N.Y.
Twitchell, J. B. (1985). Dreadful pleasures: An anatomy of modern horror, Oxford Press:
N.Y.
Waller, G. A. (1987). American Horror: Essays on the modern American horror film,
University of Illinois Press: Urbana, Ill.
Weaver, J.B , & Tamborini, R. (Eds. (1996). Horror film: Current research on audience
preferences and reactions, Erlbaum Publishing, Mahwah: NJ.
Wilkins, K. G. (2000). The role of media in public disengagement from political life,
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 44(4), pp. 569-580.
.Zillmann, D.,Weaver, J. B., Mundorf, N., & Aust, C. F. (1986). Effects on an opposite-
gender companion’s affect to horror on distress, delight, and attraction, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, pp. 586-594.
.