Promotion of creativity (divergent productions) and
convergent productions by systematic-relaxation
exercises: empirical evidence from five experimental
studies with children, young adults, and elderly
GUÈNTER KRAMPEN*
UniversitaÈt of Trier, Germany
Abstract
Presented are the results of five experimental studies on the effects of systematic-
relaxation exercises on divergent as well as convergent performance. Subjects were 40
elementary school children, 120 college students, and 52 elderly. In Experiments 1, 2, 4,
and 5, subjects were assigned at random to either an experimental group (participating
in a short systematic-relaxation exercise without pre-experience or exercising
autogenic training which subjects had learned at least 6 months before) or to a
control group (with unspecific relaxation instructions). In Experiment 3, subjects with
against without experience of autogenic training were matched for age, gender, years of
study, and creativity pretest scores. Before and after systematic against unsystematic
relaxation, subjects were tested for divergent performances (word fluency,
associational fluency, expressional fluency, and ideational fluency) and convergent
performances (short-term memory, concentration, and general intelligence). Results of
all experiments consistently show significant improvements of divergent andÐto a
lesser extentÐconvergent performances in the experimental groups after systematic-
relaxation exercises. These short-term effects of systematic-relaxation exercises are
discussed with respect to characteristics and problems of divergent problem solving
processes in individuals and groups. & 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
INTRODUCTION
Individual as well as group problem solving and performance sometimes is hindered
or even stopped by exhaustion and tiredness orÐthe oppositeÐtoo much excitement
and task involvement with its dangers for circular thinking and mental
perseveration. This has been known at least since the results of Yerkes and
CCC 0890±2070/97/020083±17$17.50
Received 31 May 1996
& 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Accepted 6 December 1996
European Journal of Personality, Vol. 11, 83±99 (1997)
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to GuÈnter Krampen, University of Trier,
Department of Psychology, D-54286 Trier, Germany. E-Mail: krampen@uni-trier.de
Dodson (1908), first obtained in animal experimentation. The `Yerkes±Dodson Law'
became one of the basics of (achievement) motivation theory including the
moderating status of task difficulty in its applications to humans (see e.g.
Atkinson and Raynor, 1974; Heckhausen, 1967). Under-motivation can result in
individuals' passivity, over-motivation can result in over-excitement and distress.
However, both motivational states can refer to feelings of not progressing and of
being in impasse, to circular thinking and compulsive mental perseveration, and to
giving up. These relations were confirmed for different achievement domains (e.g.,
academic achievement in schools and colleges, job performance, experiments on
complex problem solving, etc) in several studies (for an overview, see Atkinson and
Raynor, 1974; Heckhausen, 1967). Therefore, one focus of applied motivation
psychology, educational psychology, and occupational psychology is the
development and evaluation of intervention techniques and teaching methods to
change those under- and over-motivational states (or even habits) in favour of
motivational and emotional states (or habits) that are optimal for performance.
However, most research on the Yerkes±Dodson Law and modern motivation
theory has been done with respect to convergent problem solving; less has been done
with respect to divergent thinking and production. Surely, this partly results from
the unresolved problems in the measurement of creativity (in comparison with the
assessment of convergent performances; see e.g. Hocevar, 1981). Another reason
may be that historical (e.g. from Gestalt Psychology; BuÈhler, 1907; Wertheimer,
1943) as well as modern (see e.g. Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Olton, 1979)
phenomenological descriptions and analyses of divergent problem solving
processes include phases of motivational±emotional calmness, rest, peace, and
passivity (which sometimes even result in subjective feelings of not progressing and
of being in impasse) as well as phases of motivational±emotional excitement, high
task involvement, and activity (which sometimesÐat bestÐresult in totally
unexpected ideas and solutions; e.g. `aha experiences', BuÈhler, 1907). Indeed,
there is a strong hypothesis that divergent problem solving processes not only
include both forms of motivational and emotional states, but rather the continuous
alternation of the two types of state is one of the main characteristics of divergent
problem solving processes and creativity. Referring to childhood play behaviour,
Heckhausen (1964) described these fluctuations from over- to under-arousal as the
`cycle of activation', in which the individual swings around the optimal motivational
level but never stays at this level for a longer time. Thus, the cycle of activation
describes a continuous ebb and flow of tension and relaxation. Therefore, phases of
subjective passivity and feelings of not progressing and of being in impasse, as well
as phases of high excitement, task involvement, and activity, belong to the creativity
process. However, they must be prevented from resulting in circular thinking and in
ineffective, compulsive perserveration, in total exhaustion, and in giving up. To
assure this, several creativity techniques and creativity training programmes have
been developed and empirically evaluated (see e.g. Osborn, 1963; Rose and Lin,
1984). In short-term applications, those techniques focus mainly on the
optimization of the individual's or group's production and transformation of
ideas (i.e. fluency and redefinition) by the systematic, technically induced activation
of individuals or groups. By comparison, the phases of motivational±emotional
calmness, rest, peace, and passivity have rarely been taken into account, at least not
in empirical studies.
84
G. Krampen
At present, there exist several conceptual proposals to integrate relaxation
techniques (such as progressive relaxation, autogenic training, meditation, or
biofeedback) into creativity promotion programmes or classroom strategies to
initiate and to foster divergent thinking and action (see e.g. Brown and Wolf, 1986;
Councill, 1988; Zelina, 1982), but until now only the effects of long-term relaxation
training programmes on divergent production have been tested empirically. Domino
(1977) found no effects on creativity of a relaxation-response programme and a
transcendental meditation programme which both lasted for six months. Only the
group participating in a psychology-of-creativity course (including exercises with
creativity-enhancing techniques) improved significantly in five creativity measures.
Cowger and Torrance (1982) compared the creativity-enhancing effects of a Zazen
meditation programme and a relaxation training programme. Pretest±posttest
comparisons favoured the meditation training programme, but the relaxation
training improved sensory experiences, synthesis, and unusual visualizations (while
central creativity variables such as verbal and figural fluency as well as originality
dropped). To date, only one study has shown consistent positive effects of a longer
relaxation training programme on creativity: Hershey and Kearns (1979) analysed
the effects of a series of eight half-hour relaxation-guided fantasy sessions (in
comparison to a similar series of arithmetical exercise sessions) in gifted fourth to
sixth graders. Subjects who had participated in the relaxation-guided fantasy
training performed better at posttest on verbal fluency, flexibility, and originality
measures.
This brief overview shows that the effects of long-term relaxation training
programmes on creativity are inconsistent. However, all reported studies focused
mainly on long-term creativity (trait) effects of training programmes in the domain
of creativity (i.e. personality) development. Short-term effects of relaxation
exercisesÐeither trained previously or practised spontaneouslyÐon divergent
productions have not yet been tested empirically. With reference to the cycle of
activation in divergent problem solving processes described above, it can be
hypothesized that relaxation exercises in current problem solving situations enhance
problem solving. Such exercises create motivational±emotional states (i.e. personal
preconditions) that are a prerequisite for the optimal realization of one's own
creative abilities (trait) in the current problem situation. However, this has been
proved up to now only for convergent production (e.g. retention, memory
performance, and concentration; see Budohoska and Orlowska, 1970; Krampen,
1992; Siersch, 1989), and not for divergent production.
There exist many systematic-relaxation methods (mostly applied as additional
treatment methods in clinical psychology) which exceed individual non-systematic
relaxation and resting strategies by their (greater or lesser) training (resulting in their
broader applicability and effectiveness, even when the individual, non-systematic
methods fail) and empirical foundation. There are systematic-relaxation training
methods (e.g. progressive relaxation, Jacobson, 1924, and autogenic training,
Schultz, 1926; see also Luthe, 1969±1973) as well as systematic short-term relaxation
exercises which can be spontaneously realized after instructions without need for
longer training. I therefore investigated, in five experimental studies, the impact of a
systematic-relaxation training technique after training (i.e. autogenic training) and
that of spontaneous systematic-relaxation exercises (not training) on current
divergent productions of children as well as of adults. In addition to creativity
Creativity and relaxation
85
measures, test indicators of convergent productions (i.e., short-term memory
performance, concentration, general intelligence) were included to (i) reduce
subjects' focus on creativity and the transparency of the experiment and (ii) to test
the short-term effects of systematic-relaxation exercises on these variables. The
general hypothesis of the experiments predicts an interaction effect: it is expected
that groups performing systematic relaxation show significantly higher gains in
pretest±posttest comparisons than groups performing unsystematic resting on their
own (i.e., interaction effect of group by pretest±posttest).
EXPERIMENT 1: RELAXATION EXERCISE AGAINST RESTING IN
ELEMENTARY SCHOOL CHILDREN
Method
Subjects
Participants of Experiment 1 were 40 elementary school children aged 8±10 years (22
females and 18 males; age, M=8.8; SD=1.7) of grades 3 and 4. The children had no
pre-experiences with systematic-relaxation exercises and participated in the
experiment within their regular school education. Children were motivated by pre-
information about performing some playful tasks in small groups (while their class-
mates would stay in the classroom). They participated voluntarily and without
refusals.
Procedure
After randomization (controlling for grade and gender) and pretest, subjects in
Group A (n=20) participated in small groups (n=5 each) in systematic-relaxation
exercises. After a short introduction to the exercise, taking a relaxed sitting posture
and closing the eyes, the relaxation was induced by a short non-dynamic relaxation-
guided fantasy story (The Journey of a Leaf ). The story was read aloud with a calm
but not suggestive intonation and long speech pauses. It lasted for 10 min and
included the topics of self-attention, peace, rest, and passivity by repeated insertions
of feelings of peace (referring to passive self-attention and a calm, abdominal
breathing), heaviness (referring to muscle relaxation), and warmth (referring to
vascular dilation in the arms, hands, and legs). Subjects in group B (n=20 in four
small groups) were asked to be quiet for a while (10 min) and to rest in a subjectively
preferred manner. After relaxation/resting children reactivated by flexing their arms
vigorously and breathing deeply.
The whole procedure was implemented by four student teachers. All were
especially trained for test applications and two of them were especially trained in the
systematic-relaxation exercise used in Group A. Each experimenter tested either
Group A or Group B. They knew neither the other experimental condition nor the
hypothesis under study, but all were instructed to carry out fair testings, giving each
individual child good socio-emotional test conditions. Thus, the experiment was
implemented under double-blind conditions making expectancy effects (with respect
to the subjects as well as to the experimenters) unlikely.
86
G. Krampen
Measures
Before and immediately after the small-group exercises (systematic-relaxation
exercise in Group A versus unsystematic resting in Control Group B), all subjects
were tested with parallelized tests of short-term memory performance (the German
adaptation of the subtest `Auditory Digit Span' of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children, WISC), general intelligence (Subsets A and B of the Standard Progressive
Matrices, SPM), and verbal fluency (ideational and associational fluency, subtests
`Consequences (obvious)' and `Similarities'; TDK 4-6, a German creativity test for
children; Mainberger, 1977). All tests were administered in the small-group settings
with intensive instructions and test materials for each child. The time limit for each
test was 5 min (following test instructions with slight variations depending on task
number within the tests).
Results
Means and standard deviations of all variables for pretest and posttest measures are
reported for both groups in Table 1. Mean comparisons for all pretest measures
indicated that the randomization procedure resulted in comparable groups
(t(38)=0.87, n.s.).
The interaction hypothesis was tested in two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
with the grouping factor groups (A, B) and the repeated measurement factor time
(pretest, posttest). Table 1 shows the F-ratios that indicate differences in
performance gain between the groups and, additionally, the effect sizes d (Cohen,
1977) for simple pretest±posttest comparisons within the groups. Experimental
Group A improved significantly more than Control Group B in short-term memory
performance as well as in the tests of ideational and associational verbal fluency.
Effect sizes (d ) for these variables were large (according to the procedure and criteria
of Cohen (1997)) and, consequently, the effects are of practical relevance. A
corresponding gain from the systematic-relaxation exercise in general intelligence
performance was not observed.
Creativity and relaxation
87
Table 1. Pretest and posttest scores for Experimental Group A and Control Group B in
Study 1 (40 elementary school children), significance of the group6time interaction, and effect
sizes (d) for changes within groups
Pretest
Posttest
Group6time
Variable
Group
M
SD
M
SD
F(1,36)
d
Memory performance
A
1.8
1.2
3.9
1.3
5.12*
1.7
(WISC: digit span)
B
1.9
1.1
2.0
1.2
0.1
General intelligence
A
6.4
2.2
6.6
2.3
1.02
0.1
(SPM)
B
6.3
2.1
6.6
2.0
0.1
Ideational fluency
A
6.8
1.9
9.7
1.8
7.69**
1.6
(TDK: consequences)
B
6.9
1.8
6.8
1.9
0.1
Associational fluency
A
5.7
1.4
9.2
1.6
8.20**
2.3
(TDK: similarities)
B
5.5
1.3
6.0
1.5
0.4
*p50.05, **p50.01.
Additional analyses of variance on possible moderator effects of grade (third
versus fourth graders), age, and gender (included as additional grouping factors in
three-way ANOVAs) did not result in any significant main effect or any further
significant interaction terms. Thus, systematic-relaxation exercises improved short-
term memory performance and verbal fluency independent of grade, age, and gender
of the elementary school children under study.
EXPERIMENT 2: RELAXATION EXERCISE AGAINST RESTING IN
COLLEGE STUDENTS
Method
Subjects
Subjects of Experiment 2 were 48 undergraduate students of psychology (28 females
and 20 males; age, M=22.4, SD=3.4) participating voluntarily without
compensation. The subjects had no pre-experiences with systematic-relaxation
techniques. They were recruited in a lecture by a call for experimental subjects for an
investigation concerning test construction and validation.
Procedure
After randomization (controlling for years of study, age, and gender) and pretest,
subjects in Group A (n=24) participated in small groups (n=12 each) in
systematic-relaxation exercises. After a short introduction to the exercise, taking
a relaxed sitting posture, and closing the eyes, the 10 min relaxation was induced.
The instructions included passive self-attention, peace, heaviness (referring to
muscle relaxation), warmth (referring to vascular dilation in the arms, hands, and
legs), and calm, abdominal breathing. Subjects in Group B (n=24 in two groups)
were asked after pretest to rest for 10 min in a subjectively preferred manner. After
relaxation/resting, subjects reactivated by flexing their arms vigorously and
breathing deeply.
The whole procedure was implemented by four graduate students of psychology
with especial psychodiagnostic training. Again, two of them were especially trained
for the implementation of the systematic-relaxation exercise in Group A. As in
Experiment 1, each experimenter tested either subjects of Group A or Group B. They
knew neither the other experimental condition nor the hypothesis under study, but
all were instructed to carry out fair testings giving each subject good socio-emotional
test conditions. Thus, the experiment was implemented under double-blind
conditions making expectancy effects (with respect to the subjects as well as to the
experimenters) unlikely.
Measures
Before and immediately after the exercises (systematic-relaxation exercise in
Group A versus unsystematic resting in Control Group B), all subjects were tested
with parallelized tests on short-term memory performance (the German
adaptation of subtest `Auditory Digit Span' of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
88
G. Krampen
Scale, WAIS), concentration (`Test d2', a German concentration test;
Brickenkamp, 1975), and verbal fluency (word and expressional fluency,
subtests `Prefixes' and `Expressional Fluency'; V-K-T, a German creativity test
for adults; Schoppe, 1975). All tests were administered in the small-group settings
with verbal instructions and test materials for each subject. Time limits followed
standard test instructions.
Results
Means and standard deviations of all variables for pretest and posttest measures are
reported for both groups in Table 2. Mean comparisons among all pretest measures
indicated that the randomization procedure resulted in comparable groups
(t(46)=1.22, n.s.).
Data analyses were performed as in Experiment 1. The results of the ANOVA
tests on the interaction hypothesis and the effect sizes d for simple pretest±
posttest comparisons within the groups are presented in Table 2. Whereas there
was no significant pretest±posttest difference in Control Group B, Experimental
Group A improved significantly more in short-term memory and concentration
performance as well as in the two tests of verbal fluency (see Table 2). In
comparison to the results of Experiment 1, the effect sizes are somewhat lower.
According to the criteria of Cohen (1977), however, effect sizes again reach large
values.
Additional analyses of variance on possible moderator effects of years of
university study, age, and gender (used as additional grouping factors in ANOVAs)
did not result in any significant main effect or further significant interaction terms.
Thus systematic-relaxation exercises improved short-term memory performance,
concentration, and verbal fluency independent of years of study, age, and gender of
the college students.
Creativity and relaxation
89
Table 2. Pretest and posttest scores for Experimental Group A and Control Group B in
Study 2 (48 college students), significance of the group6time interaction, and effect sizes (d)
for changes within groups
Pretest
Posttest
Group6time
Variable
Group
M
SD
M
SD
F(1,44)
d
Memory performance
A
3.0
1.9
4.6
2.1
4.63*
0.8
(WAIS: digit span)
B
3.1
2.2
3.3
2.2
0.0
Concentration
A
23.4
5.3
28.0
6.7
4.33*
0.7
(Test d2)
B
24.0
5.7
25.0
6.0
0.2
Word fluency
A
24.0
6.1
29.6
5.9
8.09**
0.9
(V-K-T: prefixes)
B
24.8
6.4
25.9
6.2
0.2
Expressional fluency
A
11.3
2.7
13.8
2.4
8.51**
1.0
(V-K-T)
B
10.9
3.1
11.4
2.9
0.2
*p50.05, **p50.01.
EXPERIMENT 3: AUTOGENIC TRAINING AGAINST RESTING IN
COLLEGE STUDENTS
Method
Subjects
Subjects of Experiment 3 were 42 undergraduate students of psychology (24 females
and 18 males; age, M=23.7, SD=3.7) participating voluntarily without other
benefits. One half of the subjects (Group A) had learned and practised autogenic
training with good success and transfer to everyday life for at least 6 months (known
from follow-up data from a study on learning autogenic training). The other half of
the subjects (Group B) had no pre-experiences with systematic-relaxation techniques
(including autogenic training). Subjects in Group B were matched to those of Group
A for age, gender, years of study, and pretest creativity performance. Pretest
creativity scores had to be considered in the matching procedure because of
increased pretest scores of those students having learned and practised autogenic
training. This may be an effect of self-selection of students learning and practising
autogenic training, or an effect of long-term practice of autogenic training. Data
available do not allow us to test these hypotheses. However, pretest creativity scores
had to be considered additionally in matching. Thus subjects in Group B (n=21)
were selected from a larger sample of college students (N=58) and matched to the
subjects in Group A according to their pretest creativity scores, age, gender, and
years of study. Subjects of Group A were recruited by personal request with
reference to their participation in courses on autogenic training, which had been
finished at least 6 months previously. Subjects of Group B were recruited in a lecture
by a call for experimental subjects for an investigation concerning test construction
and validation (this pre-information was also given to the subjects of Group A).
Procedure
One week after pretests and matching procedure, subjects in Group A (n=21) did
their well known and consolidated autogenic training exercise in (two) small-group
settings. After greeting and information about the experimental session, subjects
practised the simple sitting posture of autogenic training with eyes closed and under
passive self-attention for 10 min. They used the seven mental standard formulas
(auto-suggestions of peace, heaviness, warmth, calm breath, calm heartbeat,
abdominal warmth, and cool forehead), following the autogenic training standard
procedure (see e.g. Schultz and Luthe, 1969, p. 15). After greeting and information
about the experimental session, subjects in Group B (n=21 in two groups) were
asked to rest for 10 min in a subjectively preferred manner. After relaxation/resting
subjects reactivated by flexing their arms vigorously and breathing deeply.
As in Experiment 2, the whole procedure was implemented by four graduate
students of psychology with special psychodiagnostic training. Again, two of them
were especially trained in the implementation of the systematic-relaxation exercise in
Group A. As in Experiments 1 and 2 each experimenter tested either subjects of
Group A or Group B. They knew neither the other experimental condition nor the
hypothesis under study, but all were instructed to carry out fair testing giving each
subject good socio-emotional test conditions. Thus, the experiment was implemented
90
G. Krampen
under double-blind conditions making expectancy effects (with reference to the
experimental subjects as well as to the experimenters) unlikely.
Measures
At pretest and immediately after the exercises (autogenic training in Group A versus
unsystematic resting in Control Group B), all subjects were tested with the same
measures as applied in Experiment 2 (parallelized tests on short-term memory
performance, WAIS-Subtest Digit Span, and concentration test d2, as well as V-K-T
subtests Word and Expressional Fluency).
Results
Means and standard deviations of all variables for pretest and posttest measures are
summarized in Table 3. Mean comparisons between all pretest measures indicated
that the randomization procedure resulted in comparable groups (t(40)40.79,
p40.05). Comparisons of pretest means (see Table 3) with norm data from German
standardization samples as well as with pretest results obtained in Experiment 2
(studying the same college student population as in Experiment 3; see Table 2) show
that the present sample is described by slightly increased short-term memory and
concentration performances as well as increased verbal fluency. Available data do
not allow a decision on whether this is an effect of self-selection of autogenic training
applicators or an effect of long-term practice of autogenic training (and matching for
pretest creativity scores of subjects in Group B).
Data analyses were run as in Experiments 1 and 2. Whereas there was no
significant pretest±posttest difference in Control Group B, Experimental Group A
improved significantly more in short-term memory and concentration performance
as well as in the two tests of verbal fluency (see Table 3). Effect sizes (d ) reached
large values.
Again, additionally computed analyses (ANOVAs) on possible moderator effects
of years of university study, age, gender, and pretest creativity scores (included as
Creativity and relaxation
91
Table 3. Pretest and posttest scores for Experimental Group A and Control Group B in
Study 3 (42 college students), significance of the group6time interaction, and effect sizes (d)
for changes within groups
Pretest
Posttest
Group6time
Variable
Group
M
SD
M
SD
F(1,38)
d
Memory performance
A
4.1
1.8
6.6
1.6
5.27*
1.5
(WAIS: digit span)
B
4.2
1.7
4.3
1.5
0.1
Concentration
A
26.0
5.4
32.1
5.1
4.39*
1.2
(Test d2)
B
26.2
5.3
27.0
5.5
0.1
Word fluency
A
26.3
6.0
33.2
5.6
7.78**
1.2
(V-K-T: prefixes)
B
27.2
5.9
27.0
6.0
0.0
Expressional fluency
A
13.2
2.5
17.9
2.1
8.43**
2.0
(V-K-T)
B
12.9
2.3
13.1
2.5
0.1
*p50.05, **p50.01.
further grouping factors in ANOVAs) did not result in any significant main effect or
any further significant interaction terms. Thus, autogenic training practised in the
current situation improved short-term memory performance, concentration, and
verbal fluency independent of years of study, age, gender, and pretest creativity
performance of the college students.
EXPERIMENT 4: AUTOGENIC TRAINING AGAINST RESTING AND
HEARING MUSIC IN COLLEGE STUDENTS
Method
Subjects
Subjects of Experiment 4 were 30 undergraduate students of psychology (18 females
and 12 males; age, M=23.6, SD=3.5) participating voluntarily without other
benefits. Known from follow-up data of a study on the learning of autogenic
training, all subjects had learned and practised autogenic training with good success
and transfer to everyday life for at least 6 months. All subjects were recruited by
personal request with reference to their participation in the courses on autogenic
training, which had been finished at least 6 months previously. Written invitations to
a reunion meeting of the course included information about data collection within a
study on test construction and validation. Subjects were accustomed to testing
because the courses on autogenic training included pretest, process, and several
posttest measures concerning the indication and effectiveness of autogenic training
(e.g. a symptom checklist, a biographical data sheet, and personality inventories).
These earlier data collections within the autogenic training courses did not include
any tests on divergent or convergent production.
Procedure
All subjects were asked to bring to the experimental session a Walkman or CD
player and some pieces of instrumental music (either pop or classical music with slow
rhythms and no singing) that they preferred in rest and relaxation situations. After
pretest and randomization (controlling for age, gender, and years of study), subjects
in Group A (n=15) were asked to practise their well known and consolidated
autogenic training exercise for 10 min in a group setting (procedure as in Experiment
3). After pretest, subjects in Group B (N=15) were asked to rest in a subjectively
preferred manner for 10 min hearing their most preferred rest and relaxation music
without practising autogenic training or other systematic-relaxation techniques. This
was done because of the significance of music for resting in the everyday life of many
people. After autogenic training, or resting and hearing music, subjects reactivated
by flexing their arms vigorously and breathing deeply. Posttests followed
immediately. In individual post-experimental interviews, all subjects in Group B
confirmed that they had not used a systematic-relaxation technique during the
experiment.
After subjects' greeting by the former instructor of the courses on autogenic
training, the whole experimental procedure was implemented by four graduate
students of psychology with especial psychodiagnostic training. Two of them had
92
G. Krampen
had special training as instructors for autogenic training (Group A). As in the
experiments before, each experimenter tested either subjects of Group A or Group B.
They knew neither the other experimental condition nor the hypothesis under study,
but all were instructed to carry out fair testings giving each subject good socio-
emotional test conditions. Thus, the experiment was implemented under double-
blind conditions making expectancy effects (with reference to the experimental
subjects as well as to the experimenters) unlikely.
Measures
At pretest and posttest all subjects were tested with the same parallelized tests as in
Experiments 2 and 3 following standard test procedures.
Results
Means and standard deviations of all variables for pretest and posttest measures are
summarized in Table 4. Mean comparisons between all pretest measures indicated
that the randomization procedure resulted in comparable groups (t(28) 41.12,
p40.05). Overall, pretest means were in good accordance with those of Group A in
Experiment 3 (see Table 3) which had been sampled from the same population of
college students experienced with and practising autogenic training in their everyday
life. In comparison to norm data from German standardization samples as well as to
pretest scores obtained from subjects without autogenic training experience, the
present sample is characterized by better short-term memory and concentration
performance as well as higher verbal fluency. Again, available data do not allow a
decision on whether this is an effect of self-selection of autogenic training applicators
or an effect of long-term practice of autogenic training.
The hypothesis of a group6time interaction was confirmed for short-term
memory and concentration performance only, and not for the two creativity
measures of verbal fluency (see Table 4). Significant (overall) main effects for the
repeated measurement factor time (pretest±posttest) showed, however, that subjects
Creativity and relaxation
93
Table 4. Pretest and posttest scores for Experimental Group A and Control Group B in
Study 4 (30 college students), significance of the group6time interaction, and effect sizes (d)
for changes within groups
Pretest
Posttest
Group6time
Variable
Group
M
SD
M
SD
F(1,26)
d
Memory performance
A
4.4
1.6
6.9
1.7
6.84*
1.5
(WAIS: digit span)
B
4.3
1.4
4.7
1.5
0.3
Concentration
A
25.7
4.9
31.8
4.7
5.01*
1.3
(Test d2)
B
26.0
5.2
27.3
4.6
0.3
Word fluency
A
27.1
5.6
32.1
5.2
0.89
0.9
(V-K-T: prefixes)
B
26.7
5.5
30.5
6.2
0.6
Expressional fluency
A
12.6
2.1
18.1
2.4
1.15
2.4
(V-K-T)
B
13.0
2.4
16.3
2.1
0.4
*p50.05.
of both groups improved significantly in word fluency (F(1,26)=6.77, p50.05) and
expressional fluency (F(1,26)=8.85, p50.01). Effect sizes of the within-group
changes reached moderate (Group B, word fluency) to large values (Group A, all
dependent variables; Group B, expressional fluency).
Again, ANOVAs on possible moderator effects of years of study, age, and gender
(included as further grouping factors) did not result in any further significant main
effect or any further significant interaction. Thus autogenic training practised in the
current situation improved short-term memory performance, concentration, and
verbal fluency independent of years of study, age, and gender of the college students.
The same was valid for the impact of simple resting and hearing music on verbal
fluency in college students with autogenic training pre-experiences (but not using it
currently), but not for its impact on their short-term memory and concentration
performance.
EXPERIMENT 5: AUTOGENIC TRAINING AGAINST RESTING AND
HEARING MUSIC IN THE AGED
Method
Subjects
Subjects of Experiment 5 were 52 aged persons (32 females and 20 males; age
M=72.1, SD=5.2, range, 67±80 years) participating voluntarily without other
benefits. As known from follow-up data from courses on autogenic training, all
subjects had learned and practised autogenic training with good success and transfer
to everyday life for at least 6 months. They were engaged in the experiment 6 months
after they had regularly finished an introductory course on autogenic training with a
community service programme. Written invitations to reunion meetings of their
course group included information on data collection within a study on test
construction and validation. As in Experiment 4, subjects were accustomed to testing
because the course on autogenic training included pretest, process, and several
posttest measures concerning the indication and effectiveness of autogenic training
(e.g. a symptom checklist, a biographical data sheet, and personality inventories).
These earlier data collections within the autogenic training courses did not include
any test on divergent or convergent production.
Procedure
All subjects were asked to bring to the experimental session a music cassette or a
compact disc with some pieces of instrumental music (either pop or classical music
with slow rhythms and no singing) preferred in rest and relaxation situations.
Walkmen and CD players were put at subjects' disposal. After pretest and
randomization (controlling for age and gender), subjects in Group A (n=26) were
asked to practise their well known and consolidated autogenic training exercise for
10 min in small-group settings (n=13; procedure as in Experiment 3). After pretest,
subjects in Group B (N=26) were asked to rest in a subjectively preferred manner
for 10 min hearing their most preferred rest and relaxation music without practising
autogenic training or other systematic-relaxation techniques. After autogenic
94
G. Krampen
training/resting and hearing music, subjects reactivated by flexing their arms
vigorously and breathing deeply. Posttests followed immediately. In individual post-
experimental interviews, all subjects in Group B confirmed that they had not used a
systematic-relaxation technique during the experiment.
After subjects' greeting by the former instructor of the courses on autogenic
training, the whole experimental procedure was implemented by six graduate
students of psychology with especial psychodiagnosis training. Three of them had
had a special training as instructors for autogenic training. As in the experiments
before, each experimenter tested either subjects of Group A or Group B. They knew
neither the other experimental condition nor the hypothesis under study, but all were
instructed to carry out fair testings giving each subject good socio-emotional test
conditions. Thus, the experiment was implemented under double-blind conditions
making expectancy effects (with reference to the experimental subjects as well as to
the experimenters) unlikely.
Measures
At pretest and posttest all subjects were tested with the same parallelized tests as in
Experiments 2±4, following standard test procedures.
Results
Means and standard deviations of all variables for pretest and posttest measures are
summarized in Table 5. Mean comparisons for all pretest measures indicated that the
randomization procedure resulted in comparable groups (t(50)41.36, p40.05). In
comparison to pretest scores obtained in the college student samples of autogenic
training applicators (Experiments 3 and 4), the present sample of the aged was
characterized by decreased short-term memory and concentration performance as
well as verbal fluency. At first glance, these age differences are consistent with
research results on cognitive development in the elderly. It should be noted, however,
that all pretest means obtained in the present sample of the aged were in relatively
Creativity and relaxation
95
Table 5. Pretest and posttest scores for Experimental Group A and Control Group B in
Study 5 (52 elderly), significance of the group6time interaction, and effect sizes (d) for
changes within groups
Pretest
Posttest
Group6time
Variable
Group
M
SD
M
SD
F(1,48)
d
Memory performance
A
2.9
2.1
4.8
2.3
7.34**
0.9
(WAIS: digit span)
B
3.0
1.9
3.1
2.2
0.1
Concentration
A
23.1
6.1
27.6
6.6
5.83*
0.7
(Test d2)
B
23.5
6.4
24.1
6.7
0.1
Word fluency
A
24.2
5.6
28.7
6.0
1.36
0.8
(V-K-T: prefixes)
B
23.5
5.3
26.8
5.4
0.6
Expressional fluency
A
10.9
1.8
14.2
2.1
0.90
1.7
(V-K-T)
B
11.2
2.0
13.0
2.4
0.8
*p50.05, **p50.01.
good accordance with the pretest means obtained in college students without
experience in systematic-relaxation training (see Table 2). The available data do not
allow a decision on whether this is a positive effect of successful learning and
practising autogenic training in the elderly, because it may also be due to self-
selection of autogenic-training learners and applicators in the aged.
Consistent with the results of Experiment 4, the hypothesis of a group6time
interaction was only confirmed for short-term memory and concentration
performance, and not for the two creativity measures of verbal fluency (see Table
5). Significant (overall) main effects for the repeated measurement factor time
(pretest±posttest) show, again, that subjects of both groups (autogenic training
versus resting and hearing music for 10 min) improved significantly in word fluency
(F(1,48)=7.43, p50.01), and expressional fluency (F(1,48)=8.68, p50.01). Effect
sizes of the within-group changes reached moderate (Group A, concentration;
Group B, word fluency) to large values (Group A, memory and both indicators of
verbal fluency; Group B, expressional fluency).
Again, ANOVAs on possible moderator effects of age and gender (included as
further grouping factors) did not result in any further significant main effect or any
further significant interaction. Thus autogenic training practised in the current
situation improved short-term memory performance, concentration, and verbal
fluency independent of age and gender of the elderly. The same was valid for the
impact of simple resting and hearing music on verbal fluency in elderly subjects
experienced with but not practising autogenic training in the current situation, but
not for its impact on their short-term memory and concentration performance.
DISCUSSION
The results obtained in all five experiments indicate significant short-term effects of
systematic-relaxation exercises on measures of divergent as well as convergent
performance. This is valid for samples of all age groups under study (elementary
school children, college students, and aged persons), pointing at the age
independency of the positive effects of relaxation exercises on short-term memory
performance and verbal fluency. Furthermore, in all five experimental studies
gender, age, and years of education were not significant moderators of the positive
effects of systematic-relaxation exercises. These results point at a broad applicability
of systematic-relaxation exercises to promote convergent as well as divergent
problem solving in different settings. These exercises (that in none of the experiments
exceeded 10 min) have a high feasibility in various educational and occupational
settings. This is especially true for the systematic-relaxation exercises used in
Experiments 1 and 2, because they do not require longer training and can be
practised spontaneously following instructions. Moreover, due to the double-blind
procedures used in the reported experiments, the internal validity of the results is
high because the probability of expectancy effects either from experimental subjects
or from experimenters is low.
The impact of systematic-relaxation exercisesÐeither performed spontaneously
without training and pre-experience of the subjects or practised on the background
of longer personal relaxation trainingÐon psychometric measures of convergent and
divergent performances exceeded the effects of non-systematic resting (in the
96
G. Krampen
subjectively preferred manner) in all experiments. However, the results of
Experiments 4 and 5 show that resting and hearing subjectively preferred relaxing
instrumental music has effects on divergent (but not on convergent) productions
similar to autogenic training exercises, at least in subjects experienced with autogenic
training but not using it in the current situation. Although control group subjects in
both experiments confirmed post-experimentally that they did not exercise autogenic
training during resting and hearing music, this cannot definitely be excluded.
Perhaps resting and hearing preferred slow-rhythm instrumental music has an effect
similar to autogenic training in the autogenic-training-experienced subjects, and an
effect similar to the relaxation-guided fantasy session practised in Experiment 1 in
elementary school children. Resting and hearing music as well as relaxation-guided
fantasies can induce peripheral relaxation while there is ongoing but slow mental
activation (mentally following the fantasy story or the music in a relaxed, passive
and non-compulsive way). These are, however, the central psychophysiological state
effects during autogenic training (i.e. mentally following the autogenic formulas
under peripheral relaxation) which Schultz (1926; Schultz and Luthe, 1969) named
`autogene Umschaltung' (autogenic switch) including `the fusion of physiological,
cognitive, and behavioral elements into what for some becomes a life-long method of
emotional and physical self-control' (Pikoff, 1984, p. 622).
Whereas divergent productivity (i.e. verbal fluency) is improved by systematic-
relaxation exercises in all experimental groups as well as by resting and hearing music
in autogenic-training-experienced subjects in Experiments 4 and 5, the results on
convergent performances are somewhat different. Short-term memory and
concentration performance are improved by relaxation exercises in all experiments,
but not under the treatment condition `resting and hearing music' in Experiments 4
and 5. This may be due to the mental focusing of instrumental music andÐperhapsÐ
the daydreaming induced by the music. This resulted in the reported positive effects on
divergent, but not on convergent performances. However, focusing relaxation-guided
fantasies in Experiment 1 improved short-term memory performance in elementary
school children, but not the performance in a general intelligence measure (SPM). This
is consistent with the trait view of general intelligence as well as with the relatively
higher dependence of short-term memory performance and divergent thinking on
situation factors (e.g. disturbances and emotional states). Moreover, relaxation-guided
fantasy sessions may be a prototype training session for short-term memory,
concentration, self-attention, and ideational fluency under positive motivational and
emotional conditions. Thus, they may improve short-term memory as well as
divergent thinking in a current situation, but not general intelligence.
It must be emphasized that all reported positive effects of systematic-relaxation
exercises are experimentally obtained short-term and not long-term effects.
According to our results, such exercises improve divergent and convergent
performance in the current situation. The reported results do not allow the
interpretation that systematic-relaxation exercises improve divergent and convergent
problem solving ability (i.e. creativity and intelligence). This hypothesis was not
under study. It can only be concluded that the application of systematic-relaxation
exercises promotes the individual's tendency to make full use and to realize his/her
intellectual abilities as far as possible in the particular situation. This is due to
restfulness and capability following systematic-relaxation exercises and is consistent
with the state-centred phenomenological descriptions of current divergent-problem-
Creativity and relaxation
97
solving processes (i.e. the cycle of activation or flow experiences; see e.g.
Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Heckhausen, 1964). However, future research has to exceed
the narrow experimental approach realized here (featuring psychometric methods of
data gathering and not real life divergent problem situations) in favour of field-
experimental studies analysing coping behaviour with more realistic problems and
including subjects who are in problem solving phases of under- versus over-motivation
with their dangers for problem solving performances of individuals and groups.
Thus, the results reported here are internally valid for short-term effects of
systematic-relaxation exercises and are in need of external validation. They do not
refer to personality development in the domains of creativity and/or intelligence. To
date, results on long-term effects of longer systematic-relaxation-training
programmes on creativity development are inconsistent (Domino, 1977; Cowger
and Torrance, 1982; Hershey and Kearns, 1979). Some of the results of Experiments
3±5 point at the possibility of such long-term developmental effects of autogenic
training on creativity development in college students and the aged. It must be noted,
however, that these results are obtained neither experimentally nor longitudinally.
The observed cross-sectional differences in pretest measures of creativity between
subjects using autogenic training and subjects not mastering a systematic-relaxation
method may be due either to training effects or to a self-selection of subjects who
decided once before in their life to learn such a relaxation technique and decideÐin a
second stepÐto use it in their everyday life. With reference to the available
experimental data the alternative explanation of self-selection can not be rejected
within the presented investigation. Research using controlled longitudinal designs is
needed to answer this question for the long-term effects of systematic-relaxation
training on creativity development. The results presented are limited to short-term
effects of systematic-relaxation exercisesÐeither trained previously or used
spontaneously following instructions. With these limitations, however, they are
very encouraging to promote individuals' competences to realize their own
intellectual abilities in a more optimal way.
REFERENCES
Atkinson, J. W. and Raynor, J. O. (1974). Motivation and Achievement, Winston, Washington,
DC.
Brickenkamp, R. (1975). Test d2: Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test [Test d2: Attention-
Endurance Test], Hogrefe, GoÈttingen.
Brown, G. W. and Wolf, J. S. (1986). `Development of intuition in the gifted', Journal for the
Education of the Gifted, 9: 157±164.
Budohaska, W. and Orlowska, A. (1970). `Eect of autogenic training on learning and
retention', Studia Psychologiczne, 10: 55±60.
BuÈhler, K. (1907). `Tatsachen und Probleme zu einer Psychologie der DenkvorgaÈnge: I. UÈber
Gedanken' [`Facts and problems for a psychology of thinking processes: I. About
thoughts'], Archiv fuÈr die gesamte Psychologie, 9: 297±365.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn, Academic,
New York.
Councill M. (1988). `Creating inspiration', Journal of Creative Behavior, 22: 123±131.
Cowger, E. L. and Torrance, E. P. (1982). `Further examination of the quality of changes in
creative functioning resulting from meditation (Zazen) training', Creative Child and Adult
Quarterly, 7: 211±217.
98
G. Krampen
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience, Harper and Row,
New York.
Domino, G. (1977). `Transcendental meditation and creativity', Journal of Applied Psychology,
62: 358±362.
Heckhausen, H. (1964). `Entwurf einer Psychologie des Spielens' [Draft of a psychology of
play behavior], Psychologische Forschung, 27: 225±243.
Heckhausen, H. (1967). The anatomy of achievement motivation, Academic, New York.
Hershey, M. and Kearns, P. (1979). `The eect of guided fantasy on the creative thinking and
writing ability of gifted students', Gifted Child Quarterly, 23: 71±77.
Hocevar, D. (1981). `Measurement of creativity: review and critique', Journal of Personality
Assessment, 45: 450±464.
Jacobson, E. (1924). `The technique of progressive relaxation', Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 60: 568±578.
Krampen, G. (1992). `Eekte der GrunduÈbungen des Autogenen Trainings im schulischen
Anwendungskontext' [Eects of basic exercises of autogenic training in schools],
Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 39: 33±41.
Luthe, W. (Ed.) (1969±1973). Autogenic therapy, Vols 1±4, Grune and Stratton, New York.
Mainberger, U. (1977). Test zum divergenten Denken: KreativitaÈt [TDK 4-6; Test on Divergent
Thinking: Creativity], Beltz, Weinheim.
Olton, R. M. (1979). `Experimental studies of incubation: Searching for the elusive', Journal of
Creative Behavior, 13: 9±22.
Osborn, A. F. (1963). Applied imagination: Principles and procedure of creative thinking,
Scribner, New York.
Piko, H. (1984). `A critical review of autogenic training in America', Clinical Psychological
Review, 4: 619±639.
Rose, L. and Lin, H. (1984). `A meta-analysis of long-term creativity training programs',
Journal of Creative Behavior, 18: 11±22.
Schoppe, K.-J. (1975). Verbaler KreativitaÈt-Test [V-K-T; Verbal Creativity Test], Hogrefe,
GoÈttingen.
Schultz, J. H. (1926). `UÈber Narkolyse und autogene OrganuÈbungen' [On narcolysis and
autogenic organ exercises], Medizinische Klinik, 22: 952±954.
Schultz, J. H. and Luthe, W. (1969). `Autogenic methods'. In: Luthe, W. (Ed), Autogenic
Therapy, Vol. 1, Grune and Stratton, New York.
Siersch, K. (1989). `Zum Ein¯uû des Autogenen Trainings auf die Konzentrationsleistung und
auf subjektiv erlebte Beanspruchungsfolgen' [Impact of autogenic training on concentration
and on experienced stress], AÈrztliche Praxis und Psychotherapie, 11: 5±10.
Wertheimer, M. (1943). Productive thinking, Harper and Row, New York.
Yerkes, R. M. and Dodson, J. D. (1908). `The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of
habit-formation', Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 18: 459±482.
Zelina, M. (1982). Program rozvoja tvorivosti ziakov [Students' creativity development
program], Ceskoslovenska Psychologie, 26: 145±155.
Creativity and relaxation
99
Copyright of European Journal of Personality is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.