between trust and domination social contracts between humans and animals


This article was downloaded by: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin]
On: 5 May 2010
Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 921567760]
Publisher Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-
41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
World Archaeology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713699333
Between trust and domination: social contracts between humans and
animals
Kristin Armstrong Oma a
a
University of Oslo, Department of Archaeology, Conservation and History,
Online publication date: 23 April 2010
To cite this Article Armstrong Oma, Kristin(2010) 'Between trust and domination: social contracts between humans and
animals', World Archaeology, 42: 2, 175  187
To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/00438241003672724
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00438241003672724
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf
This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses
should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly
or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Between trust and domination: social
contracts between humans and animals
Kristin Armstrong Oma
Abstract
Tim Ingold s seminal article  From trust to domination introduces a hypothesis in which there is a
shift from hunter-gatherer cultures to agro-pastoral cultures regarding perceptions of, and
engagements with, animals. Whereas hunters regard prey as kindred brothers, farmers regard, and
treat, their domestic livestock as slaves. On the basis of this hypothesis, archaeologists frequently
take this to be a universal given. In this article, Ingold s hypothesis is critiqued via an in-depth
discussion of the concepts of trust, reciprocity and intimacy. The author suggests an alternative
model to the dualism of either trust or domination, namely the notion of a social contract between
humans and animals. The uses of this model are explored through Bronze Age case studies from
southern Scandinavia.
Keywords
 From trust to domination ; human-animal relationships; social contract; intra-action; Bronze Age;
Scandinavia.
Introduction
The environmental archaeologist Dincauze (2000: 506) has suggested that  [a]nimals
provide an array of resources, including food, raw materials, work, energy, and
companionship. Ideologies define the terms in which we evaluate our roles in the
biosphere: dominance or kinship. This implies that there are various ways to perceive and
construct human-animal relationships. In this article I want to muse upon various
perceptions of kinds of relationships, and changes in terms of engagements (Ingold 2000).
In particular I want to critique Tim Ingold s hypothesis that domesticated animals per se
are slaves and wild (prey) animals are seen as brothers or equals to hunter-gatherer
societies (2000: 73 4). This hypothesis is widely adopted by archaeologists regarding how
roles of animals in past societies are envisioned. I suggest that such a one-sided view of this
World Archaeology Vol. 42(2): 175 187 Humans and Animals
ª 2010 Taylor & Francis ISSN 0043-8243 print/1470-1375 online
DOI: 10.1080/00438241003672724
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
176 Kristin Armstrong Oma
relationship is over-simplified, and does not take into account the relationship and trust
between humans and domestic animals embedded in the process of caring for animals
(Knight 2005). Alternatively, I follow the philosopher Mary Midgley s (1983) proposal of
human-animal social contracts. This idea will be explored in depth concerning domestic
animals, and the potential of such contracts will be investigated using Scandinavian
Bronze Age case studies.
From trust to domination?
Ingold s paper  From trust to domination (2000), addresses changing perceptions of the
relationship between humans and animals in different historical situations. He claims that
hunters, of hunter-gatherer groups, share a view of nature fundamentally based on trust
and reciprocity (2000: 69 72), where people live in the environment in the same way as
animals do (2000: 62). In societies depending upon pastoralism and husbandry, the
attitude towards animals is different: they are  slaves ,  subject to the authority of their
human master , restricted and controlled by  the whip, spur, harness or hobble . Domestic
animals are thought unable to reciprocate, dominated by humans who wish to control and
exert force over them (2000: 72 4). The process of domestication thereby constitutes a
transition in which the terms of engagement between humans and animals become
fundamentally changed (2000: 75).
Important to note is Ingold s emphasis on an engagement between humans and animals,
and animals ability to be perceived contextually as having different statuses of being. I do
not want to question the validity of anthropological studies that demonstrate a sense of
kinship and trust between hunter and prey and the potential complexity therein (see, for
example, McNiven 2010). However, I argue that Ingold s narrative is untenable as a
universal model. It cannot be applied directly to prehistory. Based on modern
anthropology and historical sources (for example, Marx), it does not consider the
particular conditions of prehistoric societies.
Ingold s ideas have become a cornerstone for theoretical frameworks in studies of
human-animal relationships in archaeology. They are frequently employed when talking
about the domination over domesticated animals (e.g. Denham and White 2007: 9 11;
Wengrow 2006: 81; for a more reflexive view, see Dransart 2002: 5 7; Jones and Richards
2003: 50; Whittle 2003: 80). However, using this cornerstone, the framework stands on
murky grounds.
Ingold s hypothesis under attack
Knight (2005: 4 5) questions the basis of Ingold s argument: the trust and reciprocity
between the hunter and his prey is not described in terms of the actual individual hunt of
individual animals, but rather as generic hunting of the animal category. When Ingold
likens hunters getting to know animals to people getting to know people, a crucial slippage
is exposed. Several problems with Ingold s argument are seen upon closer examination. For
instance, the hunters get to know people in particular (as individuals), whereas they get to
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
Between trust and domination 177
know animals in general (as a generic animal category);  [h]uman sociality is based on a
recognition of other human beings as individual persons, whereas hunter sociality with prey
seems to be based on a view of empirical animals as substitutable tokens in a class (Knight
2005: 4 5). Further, whether intimacy with prey animals is at all possible is doubtful, since
encounters between hunters and prey are episodic and unrepeated; the prey is killed on the
spot, provided that the hunt is successful. Also, hunter-prey interactions are serially
repetitive, but cannot be repeated with the same animal, therefore hunters lack familiarity
with individual animals, even though they might have a generic familiarity with the animal
species (Knight 2005: 5). This also implies that animals are not necessarily a coherent
category; different kinds of engagements can exist with different animals simultaneously.
This critique shows that the foundation for creating a relationship based upon trust is
missing in the hunting of wild animals. Moreover, I suggest that, by attempting to ascribe
perceptions of animal personhood to the hunters, Ingold is in fact exposing the hunter s
sense of guilt and psychological defence mechanisms to lessen the feelings of guilt (Serpell
2005; but see Nadasdy 2007 for a different view). Although the hunter might trust the
animal, from the animal s viewpoint this trust is not reciprocated; the prey flees in the
presence of humans and exhibits no trust.
Alternatively, in farming and pastoralist societies, relations between individual humans
and individual animals are definitely formed:  domestication does provide the temporal
and spatial conditions for human-animal intimacy to emerge (Knight 2005: 5). Living and
working with domesticated animals demands a continuous daily association with these
animals which generates a high level of mutual familiarity.
The intimacy that is formed in the proximity between humans and domestic animals is
highly dependent upon trust. Humans trust animals to be docile and cooperative, while
animals trust humans to protect them, feed them and care for them.  All creatures which
have been successfully domesticated . . . were originally social. They have transferred to
human beings the trust and docility which, in the wild state, they would have developed
towards their parents, and, in adulthood, towards the leaders of their pack or herd
(Midgley 1983: 112). Fundamental trust forms the basis for animals to give up their
autonomy, and for humans to claim responsibility. Even small domesticates like sheep and
goats are potentially dangerous animals, but the in presence of trust harmful situations
(both for humans and animals) are avoided. This demonstrates that Ingold s idea of a
universal transfer from trust to domination is over-simplified. It does not take into
account the relationship and trust between humans and animals embedded in the process
of caring for animals (Knight 2005). Thus, as a rhetorical device, I turn Ingold s (2000)
argument around: the development from hunting to husbandry signified a shift from
domination to trust.
A social contract between humans and animals
I suggest an alternative understanding of the human-animal relationship, namely that it
can be perceived as a social contract (Larre` re and Larre` re 2000; Lund et al. 2004a). The
idea of a human-animal contract differs fundamentally from a black-and-white worldview
of either a relationship of trust or one of domination. Rather, the notions of trust and
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
178 Kristin Armstrong Oma
reciprocity are at the core of the social contract. Theodossopoulos describes farmers
relationship to their animals in a small, traditional island community in Greece thus:
 [d]omestic animals, through their inclusion in the household economy, are seen as
forming close relationships with their owners. They exist within a clearly defined and
reciprocal system of order and care, one which consists of rules, duties and rights (2005:
16). The idea of a human-animal contract thus has contextually specific ramifications for
how the relationship is articulated.
The idea of a  pact between man and beast, sought by each for their mutual aid (Larre` re
and Larre` re 2000: 55) goes back to antiquity (Larre` re and Larre` re 2000; Lund et al. 2004a:
37), from the Epicureans to Lucretius, Montaigne and Hobbes. A social contract defines
the terms of engagement and the duties, responsibilities and rights of the parts involved
(Lund et al. 2004a: 35). The notion of a human-animal contract is found in gender
research, where the notion of a  gender contract indicates the type of compromise made
about the gender division of labour, both at work and, by implication, at home (e.g.
Gottfried 2000; Kapustina 2005). The gender contract is a useful tool given that gender
categories and relationships between them are fluid and negotiable. It is also found in
organic farming (Larre` re and Larre` re 2000; Lund 2002, 2005; Lund et al. 2004a) and in
biology:  [b]oth the cat and the dog are animals with which we humans have entered into a
solemn contract. We made an unwritten, unspoken pact with their wild ancestors, offering
food and drink and protection in exchange for the performance of certain duties (Morris
1986: 3).
In the social contract there is, unlike in judicial contracts, no independent third party
with the power to enforce the contract, and those who enter a contract are bound solely by
their trust in the other party. Midgley (1983: 84) suggests that the contract should stand
for an ideal area of unspoken trust and agreement. The idea of a social contract stems
from Hobbes Leviathan (1985 [1651]) where the push and pull of fear persuade the people
to give away their freedom to be protected by the sovereign to whom they have an
 artificial trust (Weil 1986). Such a contract is between each individual and the sovereign.
The contract is asymmetrical, it does not involve equal partners; rather it creates a bond
between unequal partners to secure the rights and duties of both parties. Such social
contracts may also involve animals (cf. Rowlands 2006). According to Hobbes materialist
philosophy, there is no real (ontological) difference between humans and animals
(Martinich 2005: 38; Russell 2005 [1946]: 503). Following Hobbes broader philosophical
discourse, the social contracts between a sovereign and an animal cannot be ruled out.
In organic farming an ethical  eco-contract between humans and animals is found (see
Lund et al. 2004a: 38 43 for an outline of this contract), to ensure that animals are given
ethically sound living conditions and treatment (Larre` re and Larre` re 2000; Lund et al.
2004a). It presupposes that humans and animals are partners in the agro-ecosystem, and
seeks a means to regulate human-animal relations to ensure that both parties benefits from
it (Lund et al. 2004a: 35). Here fundamental notions such as a holistic view of nature,
ecological sustainability and a focus on natural animal behaviour are instrumental to
animal welfare (Alrłe et al. 2001; Lund 2005; Lund et al. 2004b; Lund and Rocklinsberg
¨
2001: 409). Implicitly the human-animal  eco-contract acknowledges animals as
participants in contractual relations, where they have rights and obligations towards
each other.
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
Between trust and domination 179
The notion of contract has wider implications for considering human-animal relation-
ships both today and in prehistory. First, in the archaeological discourse, considering the
human-animal relationship as a social contract allows for factors that are normally not
discussed together, such as the role of animals in both economic strategies and
cosmological institutions. After all, the same animals are both symbolic transmitters
and subsistence, and can therefore be incorporated within the same framework of the
social contract (Armstrong Oma 2007, 2008). As participants in the social contract,
animals are given multiple roles and can simultaneously participate in different arenas. As
such, the social contract is a tool to examine the animals in a community where they have
a variety of roles and obligations, rather than seeing them as segregated, for example, as
either calorific value or cosmological vehicles.
Second, each contract has its own, particular ramifications, and thus a much wider
scope than as a universal catch-all, and goes beyond the idea of symbiosis. Rather, it is
historically and contextually specific. Similar to the gender contract, the human-animal
contract is negotiable. This means that it can take many forms, and today s organic  eco-
contract probably does not correspond with contracts that existed in the past.
Third, the agents involved are in asymmetrical relationships. The contract does not
imply that humans and animals must be equal partners (Larre` re and Larre` re 2000: 55),
any more than that between the sovereign and the people in Leviathan, or that between
different gender categories. It is because humans and animals are perceived as unequal
partners in modern society that an  eco-contract is meant to assure a fair and ethically
sound treatment of animals  the weaker part in the relationship. It presupposes that there
can be some communication between humans and animals, an exchange of affect, of
emotion and information interpretable by both parties (Larre` re and Larre` re 2000: 56).
However, Lund et al. point out that,  although the relation is essentially one between
unequal contributors, both humans and farm animals are equal in the sense that they are
members of the agroecological community, and their interdependence will at a closer look
turn out to be greater than commonly assumed (2004a: 36).
Animal agency and human-animal intra-action
The idea of a social contract acknowledges that the relationship between living animals
and humans is multi-layered and beyond a one-way communication. I hold that animals
are agents by way of their ability to act upon the world as living, sentient beings (see also
Argent forthcoming). Intra-action denotes how humans and animals are engaged in
mutual decision-making, a co-creation of behaviour, termed a mutual becoming (Birke
et al. 2004: 174). Mutual becoming happens at the meeting point between living animals
and humans, where animals behaviour is formed by social interactions with humans and
vice versa, leading to a fundamental reciprocity. Mutual becoming is a discursive process
that operates between human/animal conjunctions, meaning that humans and animals
adjust themselves to each other in a field of intra-agency (Birke et al. 2004: 168). Such an
inclusive understanding of animals is more useful when examining the role of animals in
the lives of humans. The agency of animals means that they are a doing or a becoming,
formed by social interactions (Birke et al. 2004: 169). For example, baby animals such as
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
180 Kristin Armstrong Oma
newborn puppies must socialize themselves in relation to humans, as well as to non-human
animals (Birke et al. 2004: 175). As sentient beings, animals are not essentialist objects,
neither fixed nor malleable, but living beings with agentive powers. Thus, the human-
animal relationship as a social process is learnt by both humans and animals (Grandin and
Johnson 2005: 32 3), and intra-action springs from this process.
Birke et al. (2004: 172 3) take the lab rat as an example of how even animals that are
perceived strictly as data and exist for scientific uses partake in the intra-action between
humans and animals. Lab rats are embedded in the whole industry of activity and
institution, and humans perform according to the rat s biological and social needs. The rat
is thereby an agent in a process. This example illustrates that not only domestic animals
and prey are formative to human lives and societies; all animals, from guinea pigs to
elephants, have agency and the ability to act upon humans.
The encounter between humans and animals is a discursive process that forms the
relationship, and a field of intra-action is created by mutual action, becoming and
performing. Such encounters are found in spatial locations where humans and animals
meet, arenas where mutual becomings are generated. Human-animal meeting points
therefore become a potential area of study of human-animal relationships in archaeology.
Riding the centaur
The idea of human-animal intra-action is aptly illustrated by the special bond that is formed
when humans and horses ride together (Argent forthcoming; Game 2001; see also Grandin
and Johnson 2005: 5 6). The human-horse relationship is founded on interconnectedness
where a joint participation in the world leads to a state of humans and horses being mixed.
Inherently, species that live together come to attune to each other, and a platform of
communication based upon sympathetic responses in each other is established. Game (2001:
2) suggests that, when riding, we experience ourselves as creatures (i.e. mixed beings), and
the mythical centaur is one example of such an interconnectedness which portrays the
human-horse relationship. The metaphor of the centaur allows riding to become an
imagining of the seamlessness between horse, human and cosmos. Although this has a
mysterious ring to it, these experiences are real within spatial and temporal dimensions.
This seamless relationship needs to be entrained. Game (2001:3; see also Hall 1983)
explains this as  learning to come in tune with , and it demands openness and receptiveness
of the other (i.e. the animal). Humans and horses entrain riding together in a horse-human
rhythm, in which they learn how to tune into one another. In this process the human and
the horse are in tune together, the relationship is what matters and species are forgotten.
 What horses and riders entrain with is the relation, the rhythm between, the transporting
flow, the riding (Game 2001: 5). Riding well demands that one forgets the human separate
self, as riding is  absorbing horse, taking horse into our body (Game 2001: 9). When one
learns and embodies a motion, the motion is inhabited (Bachelard 1969), thus, through
rhythm, horse and rider come to inhabit riding. Consequently, thinking of riding in
relational terms moves the focus from the rider as carrying out an action to an
understanding of the rider and horse as simultaneously carrying and being carried by each
other. Fundamental to this particular form of intra-action is mutual trust.
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
Between trust and domination 181
However, regarding both horses and other animals, most scholars adhere to the
domination model. Noske (2005) criticizes this as a tendency in zoological studies that
look at animals as passive vehicles for stimulus input and response output. She claims that
such research is controlled by knowledge based on domination, and not openness, towards
the other. The image of animals as neutral is fundamentally flawed. Rather, animals
construct their own world, and select the characteristics of the world that have meaning to
them, and act accordingly (Grandin and Johnson 2005; Noske 1997, 2005). For example,
horse trainers successfully draw upon the horse s own experiences, sociality and language in
handling them (Blake 1975; Game 2001; Hearne 1987; Patton 2003; Roberts 1996), thus
accentuating the two-way communication between horses and humans (Noske 2005: 47).
This nature of the human-animal relationship is seen in other instances. A mutual rhythm
and trust must also be created when milking a cow, when shearing a sheep, when using a bull
for traction, when herding or hunting with a dog and when rounding animals up to move
them from one field to another.
Alternatively, as history has shown, animals are dominated and sometimes treated
brutally, abused for the sake of entertainment and most domestic animals are slaughtered.
How does this accord with the idea of trust and social contract between humans and
domestic animals? Each relationship is unique, and the asymmetrical nature of human-
animal relationships leaves animals vulnerable to brutality and abuse of power (see
Lindstrłm this volume). Different relationships can exist at the same time with different
animals, for example, bullfighters on horseback have a relationship of mutual trust with their
horse, while simultaneously antagonizing the bull. However, I have two arguments in favour
of trust: first, it makes sense in economic terms. Following Hearne (1987; see also Argent
forthcoming and 2010), the objection towards brutality is that it does not work. Maximizing
gains, be they secondary products, riding, driving, herding or emotional and aesthetic
pleasure, is achieved more quickly, with more pleasure and with a quantitatively and
qualitatively better outcome when trust is present. A productive relationship is dependent
upon cooperation and mutual trust. Second, while life is there, death is absent. The farmer
grooming his cattle enjoys their presence in the here and now. Richard Benson portrays this
in the story of his family on a traditional farm in modern times in the Yorkshire Wolds:
I thought about Guy [Benson s brother] and Onkus [their boar]. . . . After all, how
could you possibly claim an affection between men and the animals they bred to
kill? . . . Guy would just say,  That s how it is  if someone weren t going to eat them,
I d never get to look after them , and that sounded like a brutalized feeling, a sensitivity
warped, deformed or severed, if you had never seen him talking into a boar s ear, or
looking over his shoulder at a trailer carrying one of his pigs out of the yard.
(Benson 2005: 125)
Human-animal rhythms in Bronze Age Scandinavia
Regarding human-animal rhythm, archaeological case studies vary profoundly through-
out time and space, even though the rhythm of the human-animal relationship most likely
took place in the domestic arena in farming societies. The repetitive nature of intra-action
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
182 Kristin Armstrong Oma
and mutual becoming sets apart trust as a potential area of study in farming societies. To
exemplify, I wish to draw upon case studies of well-preserved Scandinavian Bronze Age
households. In general, the household arena was made up of one or more two- or three-
aisled longhouses (e.g. Gothberg et al. 1995; Tesch 1992, 1993), in addition to the
¨
surrounding land that was used for grazing and farming.
Longhouses used for habitation are particularly salient when considering human-
domestic animal relationships. The architectural choices in Scandinavia not only facilitate
the human-animal rhythm, but it is fundamentally embedded in type 2 houses (see Fig. 1).
These are medium-sized longhouses with a clear division into two sections (Tesch 1993:
162) where humans and domestic animals lived their lives together, engaging in daily intra-
action (Armstrong Oma 2007: 135 50).
The shared life-space would facilitate caring for the animals, such as grooming, milking
and feeding them, and assisting during difficult births. The presence of strainers testifies to
milking (Armstrong Oma 2007: 249 51; Borna-Ahlkvist et al. 1998: 139; Stjernquist 1969:
31). Milking is a daily repetitive practice which requires not only physical proximity, but
also a mutual rhythm and trust. It is important to create a calm and comfortable
atmosphere to encourage the animals to relax and let down their milk, otherwise people
will get kicked and the animal will withhold its milk. Animals are normally milked in the
morning and evening, after they are fed. Milking during dusk and dawn assures a tranquil
setting, in between the hustle and bustle of everyday life. A stress-free environment creates
the ultimate conditions to achieve for both humans and animals. The most important
factor to accomplish this is mutual trust. Both animal and human need to know that they
will be safe in each other s presence; therefore the mood of the person who is milking
should be calm, not angry, and the animal should be approached gently and kindly with as
much physical contact as possible. The more one physically connects oneself to an animal,
the greater the degree of mutual trust and commitment accrued.
To milk sheep and goats one would normally straddle them, facing their tails, and let the
milk flow into a pail. Goats are particularly vivacious, and would easily get distracted, so
creating a calm atmosphere was crucial, otherwise the goats would spill the milk. To milk
a cow one would sit on a low stool beside the udder and hold the pail between the knees,
and after washing the udder with warm water and moistening the hands, one would pull
the fingers down the teats to start the milk flow. Then two by two teats would get milked,
the cow and the person falling into a mutual rhythm. The following quote from Thomas
Figure 1 Example of a longhouse of type 2. This is the late Bronze Age house Kopinge B14:VIII,
¨
where animals are presumed to have lived in the eastern part and humans in the western.
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
Between trust and domination 183
Hardy captures the required peaceful setting:  Nothing in the picture moved but Old
Pretty s tail and Tess s pink hands, the latter so gently as to be a rhythmic pulsation only,
as if they were obeying a reflex stimulus, like a beating heart (1979 [1912]: 229). Resting
the head towards the flank of the cow has a calming effect (also a habit sported by Tess
and the other milk maids, see Hardy 1979 [1912]: 228).
Animals follow their own yearly rhythms. Wool has to be shorn once a year. A
particular rhythm is necessary for sheep shearing to be at all a feasible task. Older sheep
are found in faunal assemblages from Scandinavia, and woollen clothes are found in
burials (Harding 2000; Kristiansen 1998). Bronze Age sheep herders must have developed
a rhythm which enabled them to shear sheep successfully, consecutively, year after year.
Another yearly event would be the rutting season. The sheep go with the ram in the
autumn and lambs are born in spring. Shepherds keep an eye on the females to see when
they come in heat and make sure the ram does his job. Often it is necessary to interfere and
give the animals a push in the right direction (e.g. Kennard 2004). Securing the life force of
the flock would be vital for a Bronze Age farm, therefore it was imperative to understand
and interact with the natural rhythm of the animals. Upholding the numbers in the flock
would, as a result, mean that not only was an intimate knowledge of the flock, the species
and the individual animals essential to be successful, it was also crucial to act according to
that knowledge. Thus, practices that were developed from the particular situation of the
shared life-space inside type 2 longhouses and in the household arena came into being.
These practices exemplify the effects and outcomes of the proximity manifest in
Scandinavian type 2 longhouses and the household arena. In addition to the shared life-
space of the household, rock art testifies to shepherding and arding, activities where
human-animal relationships are performed and a rhythm needs to be established. Several
rock art panels depict arding, in which a man (identified by a clearly marked penis) steers
the ard that is harnessed to two bulls (see Plate 1; see also Rasmussen 1998).
A panel at Valhaug 1 in Rogaland in south-western Norway depicts a rather uncommon
scene: a shepherd with his arms open drives a flock of four horned animals. A smaller
animal without horns seems to aid the shepherd in rounding the animals up; this is
probably a depiction of a dog (see Plate 2). In modern and historical times, dogs have been
indispensable to shepherds, and one dog can do the work of ten men (beautifully
illustrated in Kennard 2004, 2005).
Plate 1 Ploughing scene on a rock art panel in Aspeberget, Bohusla¨ n in Sweden. After: http://
home.online.no/*wen-mja/ontherocks/index.htm (accessed 8 October 2009).
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
184 Kristin Armstrong Oma
Plate 2 Pastoral rock art scene at Valhaug 1, Rogaland, Norway. Photograph by author.
A deeply intuitive relationship between shepherd and dog is needed, and serves well to
illustrate the expediency of trust. The well-known sheepdog handler and trainer Derek
Scrimgeour puts it like this:  Instead of adding additional pressure on your dog when he is
already confused or worried you must be the one he relies on to make things easier for him
to understand. If you can do this he will learn to trust you. In fact, he will begin to think he
needs you. This is when a really special relationship begins to happen (2002: 24).
The practices that these examples suggest are the results of particular human-animal
social contracts. Such contracts would deepen mutual human-animal relations of trust, and
trust played a crucial role in the contractual development. These social contracts were set
up to facilitate and benefit the dealings that humans had with animals, and ultimately to be
an advantage for household production. The shared life-space of the type 2 longhouses, as
well as rock art panels, testifies to a trust in, and a care for, domestic animals in
Scandinavian Bronze Age agro-pastoral societies. Effort and care is needed to aid the
mutual trust and commitment that took place in these societies. To make the relationship
thrive, openness, receptiveness and sympathy towards what the animal conveys is essential.
This is not, as some might dismissively state, sentimentalizing; rather it is necessary to
create a setting in which domestication and husbandry practices can take place. The notion
of trust is crucial for creating the foundation on which a reciprocated rhythm may happen,
and the enactment of mutual becoming cannot take place without mutual trust. In this
sense, trust is antithetical not to domination, but rather to the less intimate encounter
between hunter and prey. While the hunter sentimentalizes brutality, the herder needs to
develop a mutual relationship based upon trust with her flock. The architectural choices in
Scandinavian type 2 longhouses facilitated the notions of trust, care and openness towards
domestic animals, thereby constructing space for the human-animal rhythm to take place,
and were ultimately places where mutual becomings happened.
The above discussion demonstrates that intra-action comes from both economic strategies
and cosmological institutions, but above all from social practice. Studying these households
through animals reveals the underlying rationale by which the household structured its life-
spaces, demonstrating how animals were embedded as a structuring principle in the
household. The social contract between humans and animals structures the framework for
intra-action. Thus, the underlying rationale of these societies is seen through the social
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
Between trust and domination 185
contract, since it grasps the full complexity of the human-animal relationship and allows for
ritual and economy (and other notions) to be discussed together.
Acknowledgements
I wish to thank Niall Armstrong, Gala Argent and two anonymous reviewers for
constructive feedback.
University of Oslo, Department of Archaeology, Conservation and History
k.a.oma@iakh.uio.no
References
Alrłe, H. F., Vaarst, M. and Kristensen, E. S. 2001. Does organic farming face distinctive livestock
issues? A conceptual analysis. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 14(3): 275 99.
Argent, G. 2010. Do the clothes make the horse? Relationality, roles and statuses in Iron Age Inner
Asia. World Archaeology, 42(2): 157 74.
Argent, G. forthcoming. Toward a privileging of the nonverbal: communication, corporeal
synchrony and transcendence in humans and horses. In Experiencing Animals: Encounters between
Animal and Human Minds. New York: Columbia University Press.
Armstrong Oma, K. 2007. Human-Animal Relationships: Mutual Becomings in the Household of
Scandinavia and Sicily 900 500 BC. OAS. Oslo: Unipub.
Ú
Armstrong Oma, K. 2008. Kropp og sjel: eit skeivt blikk pa bronsealderdiskursen. Nicolay, 104: 67 72.
Bachelard, G. 1969. The Poetics of Space. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Benson, R. 2005. The Farm: The Story of One Family and the English Countryside. London: Hamish
Hamilton.
Birke, L., Bryld, M. and Lykke, N. 2004. Animal performances: an exploration of intersections
between feminist science studies and studies of human/animal relationship. Feminist Theory, 5(2):
167 83.
Blake, H. 1975. Talking with Horses: A Study of Communication between Man and Horse. London:
Souvenir Press.
Borna-Ahlkvist, H., Lindgren-Hertz, L. and StaÚ lbom, U. 1998. Pryssgården: Från stenålder till
medeltid. Arkeologisk slutundersökning RAÄ 166 och 167 Östra Eneby socken Norrköpings kommun
Östergötland. Linkoping: Riksantikvarieambetet.
¨ ¨
Denham, T. and White, P. 2007. The Emergence of Agriculture: A Global View. London and New
York: Routledge.
Dincauze, D. F. 2000. Environmental Archaeology: Principles and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Dransart, P. Z. 2002. Earth, Water, Fleece, and Fabric: An Ethnography and Archaeology of Andean
Camelid Herding. New York: Routledge.
Game, A. 2001. Riding: embodying the centaur. Body and Society, 7(4): 1 12.
Gothberg, H., Kyhlberg, O. and Vinberg, A. (eds) 1995. Hus och gård i det förurbana samhället:
¨
rapport från ett sektorforskningsprosjekt vid Riksantikvarieämbetet. Katalogdel. Stockholm:
Riksantikvariea¨ mbetet.
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
186 Kristin Armstrong Oma
Gottfried, H. 2000. Compromising positions: emergent neo-Fordism and embedded gender
contracts. British Journal of Sociology, 51(2): 235 59.
Grandin, T. and Johnson, C. 2005. Animals in Translation: Using the Mysteries of Autism to Decode
Animal Behaviour. London: Bloomsbury.
Hall, E. T. 1983. The Dance of Life. New York: Anchor.
Harding, A. F. 2000. European Societies in the Bronze Age. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hardy, T. 1979 [1912]. Tess of the d Urbervilles. London: Macmillan Education.
Hearne, V. 1987. Adam s Task: Calling Animals by Name. London: Heinemann.
Hobbes, T. 1985 [1651]. Leviathan. London: Penguin.
Ingold, T. 2000. From trust to domination: an alternative history of human-animal relations. In The
Perception of the Environment: Essays in Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill (ed. T. Ingold). London and
New York: Routledge, pp. 61 76.
Jones, A. and Richards, C. 2003. Animals into ancestors: domestication, food and identity in Late
Neolithic Orkney. In Food, Culture and Identity in the Neolithic and Early Bronze Age (ed. M. Parker
Pearson). Oxford: BAR International Series 1117, pp. 45 52.
Kapustina, M. A. 2005. Towards explaining the modification of gender contract in Norway. Paper
given at EspaNet Young Researchers Workshop, 1 May.
Kennard, D. 2004. A Shepherd s Watch: Through the Seasons with One Man and his Dogs. London:
Headline.
Kennard, D. 2005. The Dogs of Windcutter Down. London: Headline.
Knight, J. 2005. Introduction. In Animals in Person: Cultural Perspectives on Human-Animal
Intimacy (ed. J. Knight). Oxford and New York: Berg, pp. 1 13.
Kristiansen, K. 1998. Europe before History. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Larre` re, C. and Larre` re, R. 2000. Animal rearing as a contract? Journal of Agricultural and
Environmental Ethics, 12(1): 51 8.
Lund, V. 2002. Ethics and animal welfare in organic animal husbandry: an interdisciplinary
approach. Doctoral thesis, Department of Animal Environment and Health/Skara, Swedish
University of Agricultural Sciences.
Lund, V. 2005. The human-animal relationship in organic farming. In The Human-Animal
Relationship: Forever and a Day (eds F. de Jonge and R. van den Bos). Assen: Royal van Gorcum, pp.
231 46.
Lund, V. and Rocklinsberg, H. 2001. Outlining a concept of animal welfare for organic farming
¨
systems. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 14(4): 391 424.
Lund, V., Anthony, R. and Rocklinsberg, H. 2004a. The ethical contract as a tool in organic animal
¨
husbandry. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 17(1): 23 49.
Lund, V., Hemlin, S. and White, J. 2004b. Natural behaviour, animal rights, or making money: a
study of Swedish organic farmers view of animal issues. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental
Ethics, 17(2): 157 79.
Martinich, A. P. 2005. Hobbes. New York and London: Routledge.
McNiven, I. J. 2010. Navigating the human-animal divide: marine mammal hunters and rituals of
sensory allurement. World Archaeology, 42(2): 215 30.
Midgley, M. 1983. Animals and Why They Matter: A Journey around the Species Barrier.
Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Morris, D. 1986. Catwatching: The Essential Guide to Cat Behaviour. London: Cape.
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010
Between trust and domination 187
Nadasdy, P. 2007. The gift in the animal: the ontology of hunting and human-animal sociality.
American Ethnologist, 34(1): 25 43.
Noske, B. 1997. Beyond Boundaries: Humans and Animals. Montreal, New York and London: Black
Rose Books.
Noske, B. 2005. Horse image and the human self-image in equine research. In The Human-Animal
Relationship: Forever and a Day (eds F. de Jonge and R. van den Bos). Assen: Royal van Gorcum,
pp. 38 50.
Patton, P. 2003. Language, power and the training of horses. In Zoontologies: The Question of the
Animal (ed. C. Wolfe). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 83 100.
Rasmussen, M. 1998. Livestock without bones: the long house as contributor to the interpretation of
livestock management in the Southern Scandinavian Early Bronze Age. In Settlement and
Landscape. Proceedings of a Conference in Århus, Denmark, 4 7 May 1998 (eds C. Fabech and
Ú
J. Ringtved). Moesgard: Jutland Archaeological Society, pp. 281 90.
Roberts, M. 1996. The Man Who Listens to Horses. New York: Random House.
Rowlands, M. 2006. Out of contract. Times Literary Supplement, 26.
Russell, B. 2005 [1946]. History of Western Philosophy. London and New York: Routledge.
Scrimgeour, D. 2002. Talking Sheepdogs: Training your Working Border Collie. Preston: Good Life
Press.
Serpell, J. A. 2005. Animals and religion: towards a unifying theory. In The Human-Animal Relationship:
Forever and a Day (eds R. van den Bos and F. de Jonge). Assen: Royal van Gorcum, pp. 9 22.
Stjernquist, B. 1969. Beiträge zum Studium von Bronzezeitliche Siedlungen. Lund: CWK Gleerups
Forlag.
¨
Tesch, S. 1992. House, farm and village in the Kopinge area from Early Neolithic to the Early
¨
Middle Ages. In The Archaeology of the Cultural Landscape. Field Work and Research in a South
Swedish Rural Region, Vol. 4 (eds L. Larson, J. Callmer and B. Stja¨ rnquist). Stockholm: Almqvist &
Wiksell, pp. 283 344.
Tesch, S. 1993. Houses, farmsteads and long term change: regional study of prehistoric settlements in
the Kopinge area, in Scania, Southern Sweden. PhD, Uppsala University.
¨
Theodossopoulos, D. 2005. Care, order and usefulness: the context of the human-animal
relationship in a Greek island community. In Animals in Person: Cultural Perspectives on Human-
Animal Intimacy (ed. J. Knight). Oxford and New York: Berg, pp. 15 35.
Weil, F. D. 1986. The stranger, prudence, and trust in Hobbes s theory. Theory and Society, 15(5):
759 88.
Wengrow, D. 2006. The Archaeology of Early Egypt: Social Transformations in North East Africa,
10,000 to 2650 BC. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Whittle, A. 2003. The Archaeology of People: Dimensions of Neolithic Life. London and New York:
Routledge.
Kristin Armstrong Oma is currently holding a postdoctoral fellowship at the University of
Oslo; the working title for her research is  Prehistoric human-animal practices: social life,
land use and economic strategies on the farm and their reciprocal impact upon natural
systems . She works in the cross-disciplinary field of human-animal studies, and her main
research focus is human-animal relationships in past societies. She lives with and trains
Bessie, a border collie, thus having daily first-hand experience of mutual becoming.
Downloaded By: [Armstrong Oma, Kristin] At: 19:56 5 May 2010


Wyszukiwarka

Podobne podstrony:
Culture, Trust, and Social Networks
Transgenic animal production and animal biotechnology
Palmer relation between moral reasoning and agression, and implications for practice
Linux IPCHAINS HOWTO Appendix Differences between ipchains and ipfwadm
Meeting between the Swedish Chairmanship of the Arctic Council and Observers
03 Relationship between electrochemical properties of SOFC cathode and
MEPCB1 Cross reference tables between old and new regulations of MARPOL Annex I
Duality between Electric and Magnetic Black Holes
Simplified modeling of circulating flow of solids between a fluidized bed and a
MEPCB1 Cross reference tables between old and new regulations of MARPOL Annex I
Lumiste Tarski s system of Geometry and Betweenness Geometry with the Group of Movements
Bertrand Russel The Divorce Between Science And Culture
Linux Online Linux IPCHAINS HOWTO Appendix Differences between ipchains and ipfwadm
Genomic differences between C glabrata and S cerevisiea

więcej podobnych podstron