A concept of 'critical period' for language acquisition


A concept of 'critical period' for language acquisition
--- Its implication for adult language learning ---

Katsumi NAGAI
Osaka Nougei High School, Evening Course

‚Í‚¶‚ß‚É

`Šú‰pŚę‹łˆç‚Ě•K—v«‚Ş‹©‚΂ęCŹ¬ŠwŤZ‚ʼnpŚę‹łˆç‚ŞŽŽŤs‚ł‚ę‚éˆę•űC'莞§Ť‚ŤZ‚ʼnpŚę‚đŠw‚Ԑ¬l¶“k‚©‚碂±‚̍΂ŌęŠw‚̕׋­‚đ‚µ‚Ä‚ŕ-ł`Ę‚Ĺ‚Í‚Č‚˘‚©Ł‚Ć‚˘‚¤‹^-â‚ŞŹo‚ł‚ę‚éD-{Ź¬˜_‚Ĺ‚Í‚±‚ę‚ç‚Ě‹c˜_‚ĚŚ®‚ƂȂ錾ŚęŠwŹK‚Ě—ŐŠEŠú‰Ľŕ‚ɂ‚˘‚Ă̐ćŤsŚ¤‹†‚đ'˛Ť¸‚µ‚˝DŚ‹˜_‚͢—ŐŠEŠú‚Ě`¶ŤÝ‚͉Ľŕ‚̈ć‚đŹo‚Č‚˘Ł‚Ĺ‚ ‚é‚ŞC`ć‚QŚľŚę‚Ě•¶-@Šl“ľ‚Ɖą‰CŠl“ľ‚đŤ¬“Ż‚·‚é‚̂͊댯‚Ȃ悤‚Ĺ‚ ‚éD

1. Teachability and Learnability

Every time adult learners see a child who speaks a foreign language fluently, the learners regret not having started learning the foreign language earlier because the learners' speech necessarily involves a foreign accent. Does their regret have a rationale? Is learning foreign language impossible for adult learners? It is widely believed that the earlier people start learning a second language, the more successful they will be. If there is 'a period during which language can be acquired more easily than at any other time (Richards, Platt and Platt 1992:92),' language learning after that period should be more difficult. Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH henceforth) affects the educational policy-making of foreign language teaching. If the period falls before the age of twelve or thirteen, at which the age Japanese students begin to learn English, the Japanese learners of English are in no way to blame for their foreign accents.

Apart from a question of what the word 'native' means, the CPH provides a good excuse to the people who fail to achieve native-like fluency in foreign languages. The conclusion of this chapter is, unfortunately, that the CPH will remain a hypothesis for ever unless further research clarifies the nature of language acquisition itself. However, the results of experimental studies have two important implications for adult second language learning. One is that children's acquisition of a foreign language is different from that of adults'. The other is that acquisition of pronunciation and grammar is also different because it involves a problem of physiologic aging process. These results do not contradict a widespread belief that adults can learn the grammar of a new language more easily and rapidly than children but that they retain foreign accents.

2. Evidence of the Critical Period Hypothesis

2.1. The Critical Period Hypothesis

Originally the phrase 'critical period' was used for the ethologists' study of species-specific behavior. It is the period when imprinting is observed in certain species such as young birds and rats. For example, geese isolated from their parent birds since the hatching react to and follow the moving object they see first. This kind of behavior takes place only for a short period of time after hatching (Lorenz 1961, quoted in Clark & Clark 1977:520). Another example is a 'crystallization' of chaffinch whose songs are highly intricate. If a young bird does not hear an adult singing within a certain period, the bird will never sing a full song. The CPH posits this kind of a short period for the acquisition of language, which is decided by the learners' behavior.

Flege (1987) lists four characteristics of the imprinting as below.

1. It tends to appear under well-defined developmental conditions.
2. It cannot be forgotten or revised once it has been established.
3. It involves the recognition of species' characteristics rather than individual characteristics.
4. It may be learned long before it is manifested.

He assumes that only the first characteristic above is applicable to the human language acquisition. Is it applicable to second language learners too? A pioneering work by Lenneberg (1967) explores the CPH and says:

Between the ages of two and three years language emerges by an interaction of maturation and self-programmed learning. Between the ages of three and the early teens the possibility for primary language acquisition constitutes to be good; the individual appears to be most sensitive to stimuli at this time and to preserve some innate flexibility for the organization of brain functions to carry out the compex integration of sub-processes necessary for the smooth elaboration of speech and language. After puberty, the ability for self-organization and adjustment to the physiological demands of verbal behavior quickly declines. The brain behaves as if it had become set in its ways and primary, basic skills not acquired by that time usually remain deficient for life (Lenneberg 1967:158).

His book has been widely accepted because of its clear explanation for difficulties in second language learning after puberty. It explains well our experience of seeing small children speak foreign languages quite naturally, and the experience of hearing unnatural foreign accents in the speech of adult learners.

2.2. Studies of a Human Brain and its Lateralization

If a language develops with the increase of the brain cell connections, the development of language and brain should bear some relation to each other. Physiological functions of neuronal cells, circuits, or connections themselves have not been identified clearly so far, but some specified fields of linguistic functions have been reported. Geschwind (1980:62-63) claims:

Although the content, that is, the specific language learned, is completely determined by the environment, the capacity to acquire language is biologically determined. Indeed, one should not speak about a contrast between language and general cognitive ability.

Earlier studies show that the brain seems to have special-purpose computers for limited functions, and there is at present no evidence of any all-purpose computer for any general cognition. If a language is acquired in parallel with the development of the human brain as children grow, it is reasonable to postulate some language function in the brain. Consequently, the critical period for language learning is considered to be the biologically determined period in which the brain keeps its plasticity for acquisition of any language.

At first it was expected that the function of human language acquisition was clarified by exploring a special structure, which all other animal brains lack. The brain of human beings consists of a left hemisphere and a right hemisphere, and different functions are said to develop gradually in different parts of the brain as children grow older. The parts of brain which control a language are placed in the left hemisphere after its lateralization. One of the parts is called the Broca's area and the other part is called the Wernicke's area. The study of aphasia has revealed that the Broca's area controls spoken language. The Wernicke's area is, on the other hand, said to be the center of language understanding.

Lenneberg posits that the development of language is the result of brain maturation. Although both hemispheres of the brain are equal at birth, the function of language gradually settles in the dominant left hemisphere of the brain after biological maturation or the critical period. That is, the critical period for language learning has been considered to agree with the period of the lateralization of brain. Research on people who have suffered brain damage also supplies the evidence for the lateralization of brains. Lenneberg viewed clinical data and claimed that children younger than nine years old had a higher incidence of right hemisphere lesions causing aphasia than adults (Villiers & Villiers 1978:211). Another support of lateralization is an experimental report of speed and accuracy in language acquisition by Kimura (1973). He examined speech sounds heard in each ear. His result is that speech sound through the right ear is processed more quickly than through the left ear because the left hemisphere is connected to a right ear.

A brain is said to lose its plasticity after the lateralization, and some case studies are reported on the impaired brains before and after the critical period. Adults who have suffered brain damage in their left hemisphere fail to recover their language if they don't recover in five months, but children show an ability to recover over a longer period, and have sometimes made a full recovery if they were very young at the time of damage (Crystal 1987:263). Even total removal of the left hemisphere did not preclude children's reacquisition of language. Lenneberg's argument is based on this period of lateralization, of which completion means the end of the critical period.

2.3. Studies of Feral Children

Supports most frequently quoted for the CPH are the case studies of the children who had been isolated from language and who tried to acquire the language before and after their critical period. Unlike the cases of Amala and Kamala, who were said to be reared by wolves and found in 1920, studies of teaching a language are reported in the following three cases.

The first case was a deaf mute child named Isabelle, who was found at the age of six and half. She spent alone in a darkened room before being found, but she succeeded in her language learning because she was at the age of six and half. Brown (1958: 192, cited in Aitchison 1989:85) recorded:

Isabelle passed through the usual stage of linguistic development at a greatly accelerated rate. She covered in two years the learning that ordinarily occupies six years. By the age of eight and one half Isabelle was not easily distinguishable from ordinary children of her age.

It is reasonable to consider that she was able to acquire her language because she started learning before the critical period came to an end.

The second case was Genie, who was found at the age of about fourteen (Curtiss, Fromkin, Krashen, Rigler, and Rigler 1974). Because she started learning a language after the critical period, her progress was slower than other children. For example, her two-word stage, at which every child goes though uttering two words at a time like 'Want milk' and 'Mummy play,' lasted much longer. Genie used this type of primitive form and its negation such as 'No want milk' for a longer period. Her ability to learn vocabulary was superior to other children. However, her grammatical development was much slower and unsuccessful, because her critical period had passed already. Since she started learning a language after she was already pubescent, Genie had to take quite a long time to acquire a language.

The third case was Chelsea, who started to learn language in her early thirties (Curtiss 1988). She showed poor grammatical ability like Genie, but her vocabulary was better. It was recorded that her syntax created sentences such as 'the woman is bus the going' and 'banana the eat.'

All these cases of children reared in isolated environments reveal the difficulties of learning a language after the critical period.

2.4. Criticism of the Critical Period Hypothesis

2.4.1. Critical Period Hypothesis reexamined

As medical science has progressed, some points in Lenneberg (1967) are under criticism. Firstly, his claim that the CPH could be supported by the study of Down's syndrome cases is attacked. According to his argument, the development of children with Downfs syndrome is so slow that they pass their critical period for language learning. However, a recent survey disputes his claim because children with Down's syndrome have a build-in endpoint to their ability (Gleitman 1984).

Secondly, the theory of brain's lateralization at the age of two, with the critical period set by Lenneberg (1967), is also criticized. He claimed that children before their critical period were less severely impaired by brain damage. However, Krashen (1973) reexamined the data used in Lenneberg (1967) and found all the cases of complete recovery from aphasia were under the age of five. Surprisingly, the number of cases of recovery at the age of more than five or over was nearly the same number as the adults'. Kinsbourne (1975) pointed out the difficulty in deciding whether only half of the hemisphere was injured or not. Another piece of counter evidence is seen in MacKain et. al. (1983). Their experiment with babies of six months or less shows that lateralization begins much earlier than two years old. If so, lateralization cannot be the evidence of the critical period. Reports of dichotic listening, experiments in which different stimuli are presented simultaneously to the two ears, also show that language functions are lateralized much earlier than the critical period. Regrettably, no conclusive evidence for right ear advantage based on the lateralization has not been reported.

Thirdly, the case studies of three linguistically isolated children have some problems. Because the early research in Isabelle's case was not written by a specialist in language (Aitchson 1989:86), her speech may have been less sufficient than reported. There is also a report that Genie's left hemisphere was atrophied because of the brain damage. This implies that she used only the right hemisphere of the brain, the part which is said to have little function in language processing. Curtiss, et. al. (1974) writes:

The result of the dichotic tests using environmental sounds also show a left-ear advantage, but only to a degree found in normal subjects. This 'normal' result shows that Genie is not simply one of those rare individuals with reversed dominance, but instead is one in whom all auditory processing currently appears to be taking place in the right hemisphere (Curtiss, Fromkin, Krashen, Rigler, and Rigler 1974:542).

Isabelle was deaf and Chelsea had hearing problems too (Aitchson 1989:87). A difference between deaf and normal children should have been taken into consideration in the case studies. Since no further case of feral children will be reported, the data have been reanalyzed. All of these criticisms of the past studies show how difficult it is to connect brain functions and language acquisition.

Moreover, the different methods for the research on the CPH have also resulted in varieties of inconsistencies. For example, native speakers were asked to rate the performance or naturalness of the utterances of learners in one study (Scovel 1981, cited in Ellis 1984:485), whereas grammatical judgments made by learners of different ages were examined in another study (Coppetiers 1987). Birdsong (1992) designed a grammaticality judgment test for twenty native English speakers who spoke French with native-like fluency in order to examine Long's question (1990:281) of 'whether the very best learners actually have native-like competence.'

2.4.2. The Sensitive Period and the Multiple Periods

The CPH is under criticism, with a large quantity of counter evidence. Nevertheless, no other hypothesis will explain the truth better that language learning by matured students and the brain damaged patients' recovery of language become more difficult with aging.

Because it is difficult to fix the exact span of years during which language learning can take place naturally, some researchers have presented a revised version of the CPH. They use the term 'sensitive period,' rather than 'critical period,' for second language acquisition. The distinction between the CPH and the sensitive period hypothesis is whether acquisition is 'possible only within the definite span of age' or 'easier within the period.' Oyama (1979:88) says that sensitive periods are preceded and followed by less responsive periods. Seliger's proposal (1978) is that there may be multiple critical or sensitive periods for different aspects of language. The period 'during which a native accent is easily acquirable' appears to end earlier than the period governing the acquisition of a native grammar.

2.5. Implications of the CPH for Adult Second Language Learning

2.5.1. The Implication from the Variety of Results

What implications does the argument of the CPH for adult second language learning have? As there are such divergent results on the CPH, the classification by Ellis (1994: 485) is useful to find a point of departure. It is especially so if the priority is given to phonological development, which necessarily involves articulatory motor control.

1. What effect does age have on the rate of L2 learning?
2. What effect does age have on learners' levels of L2 achievement of native-speaker levels of proficiency?
3. What effect does age have on learners' levels of L2 achievement (in those learners who do not reach native-speaker proficiency)?
4. What effect does age have on the processes of L2 learning?

Ellis (1994:493) says, 'if adults substitute inductive cognitive learning strategies for the language acquisition device used by children, differences in the process of acquisition might be expected to occur.' Recent studies of the second language acquisition, however, fail to report consistent results to support his view. Singleton (1989:137) writes:

Concerning the hypothesis that those who begin learning a second language in childhood in the long run generally achieve higher levels of proficiency than those who begin in later life, one can say that there is some good supportive evidence and that there is no actual counter evidence.

Does this claim have the rationale? The following are examples of the studies concerning the CPH.

Harley (1986) shows greater overall control of verb system by older group who studied French in Canada. The result is not supportive of the CPH because, as Ellis (1994:489) writes, formal learning environments do not provide learners with the amount of exposure needed for the emergence of young learners' age advantage. Cummins and Nakajima (1987) conducted a survey of 273 Japanese children in Canada. They found that the older the students were on arrival in Canada, the more they had reading skills and writing skills in English. They claimed that the older learners benefited from prior literacy experience in Japanese. In Neufeld (1978), twenty adult native speakers of English were asked to do an imitation test after they had eighteen hours of intensive instruction in Chinese and Japanese. Their naturalness was judged by native speakers on a five-point scale. The result suggests that adults can achieve native-like ability in pronunciation. His experiment was criticized later on the grounds that the subjects in the study were the elites who produced unnatural rehearsed data (Long 1990). Overall, as in Krashen, Long and Scarcella's argument (1979), adults learn faster, especially so in the case of formal learning of grammar than pronunciation.

The most important reason for the divergent results of the past research is, excepting the differences in the methods themselves, disregard of the natural learning environment. Child learners, playing with other native young children, can enjoy their language interaction. They can speak loudly, they can ask anytime and anywhere, and they do not have to worry about making mistakes. Adult learners, on the other hand, have difficulties in participating in natural language interaction when they learn a foreign language. This affective factor plays an important role in artificial environments. In a classroom, however, adult learners can use mature cognitive skills to compensate for their declining memory and motor control. Adult learners' inductive and explicative processing abilities are in optimal condition (Steinburg 1982:179). This enables them to do better than children in classrooms. Ellis (1994:491) writes:

Adult learners have an initial advantage where rate of learning is concerned, particularly in grammar. They will eventually be overtaken by child learners who receive enough exposure to the L2. This is less likely to happen in instructional than in naturalistic settings because the critical amount of exposure is usually not available in the former.

Research on the CPH suggests that adult learners' developed cognitive skills help them learn a foreign language. This results in an initial advantage over children in the artificial environment such as in a classroom though it does not guarantee their native-level achievement.

While some adult learners of foreign languages can speak foreign languages quite naturally even if they started learning after puberty, most learners fail to reach native-speaker's level. Should adult learners give up their aim at achievement of native-speaker levels? Can they outperform the learners who started learning a second language before puberty? Four studies are considered below.

Coppieters (1987) examined twenty-one adult French speakers who had begun learning French as a second language. When their grammatical performance was compared with native speakers, it was impossible to distinguish the non-native speakers by the number of mistakes and inappropriate wordings. The judgment of grammaticality was, however, different between native and non-native speakers. Coppieters claimed that the divergence between the two groups was more marked in functional distinctions. Birdsong (1992), on the other hand, concludes contrastively. He gave a grammatical test to twenty learners of French, who spoke like native speakers but whose native language was English. In comparison with native speakers of French, no significant difference was found within the theoretical frame of 'Universal Grammar.' His study reassures the existence of competent learners of foreign languages who started learning a second language after puberty. The third example is in Thompson's study (1991). He reported that the learners who arrived in the United States before they became ten years old succeeded in learning more natural English than those who arrived at later age. The two subjects who came from Russia at the age of four failed to achieve native-like pronunciation because of, Thompson claims, their high proficiency in Russian. This hints at the dominance of the first language. The fourth is Scovel's research (1981). He divided the subjects into four groups, i.e. adult native, child native, adult non-native, and child non-native speakers, and asked them to distinguish differences of spoken and written accents. The result showed that even the most advanced non-native speakers could not achieve native-like accuracy (73 percent, compared with 95 percent by the native speakers). Studies by Coppieter and Scovel conclude that even very advanced learners can not perform perfectly.

It is unclear whether the achievement of native-speaker level fluency is possible for adult learners because all research cited above produce mixed results. Children are more likely to achieve higher levels in both pronunciation and grammar than adults, but some adults who started learning their second language after puberty can achieve native-like levels. This inconclusiveness is a warning to teachers and parents of language learners. However early they start teaching and whatever a desirable environment they have, these conditions can not guarantee the native-like performance. It should be also noted that it is impossible to decide whether learners of foreign languages have a real 'native-like' competence without defining the word 'native.'

2.5.2. The Different Acquisition of Pronunciation and Grammar

One important thing obtained from reviewing the past studies above is the fact that the critical period of pronunciation seems to be different from that of grammar. Studies of anarthric children have also revealed that their perception of phonological contrasts is normal. Lenneberg (1967) and Slobin (1971:60) illustrate that anarthric people can acquire a language. For example, they report the case of a speechless child who was unable to articulate speech. The boy's articulation was abnormal, but he was able to understand complete English sounds and syntax. Genie's case (Curtiss, Fromkin, Krashen, Rigler, and Rigler 1974:534) is not simple because she had to learn to control her articulatory organs. The researchers write: Genie can pronounce many sound sequences in imitation which she does not use in spontaneous speech. It is clear that her output is more constrained by her own phonological 'realization' rules than by her inability to articulate the sounds and sound sequences of English.

This shows that, even in an abnormal case of language acquisition, children's phonological competence and their phonetic performance need to be differentiated carefully. One comprehensible example frequently quoted is in Miller (1963). He reports:

Recently a three year old child told me her name was Litha. I answered:
eLitha?'
eNo, Litha.'
eOh, Lisa.'
eYes, Litha.'

She was not able to articulate the difference between th and s, but was able to recognize the auditory difference. This datum shows that children's phonological rule and production of the speech sound does not develop at the same time. The notion of multiple critical periods also hints at the need of distinguishing acquisition of phonology and phonetics.

All of these points suggest that language teachers should distinguish teaching of pronunciation and grammar, and that they should take different teaching approaches when necessary. According to Flege (1987):

Speech is 'material' in the sense that it is realized motorically by means of a mechanism with specific biomechanical constraints, and perceptually processed through sensory mechanisms having specific limits of resolving power. Flege (1981) analyzes voice onset times (VOT) values in English and French to illustrate accented English spoken by French Canadians. He comes out with his own phonological translation hypothesis and says:

I would like to propose that a tendency by mature speakers to interpret sounds occurring in a foreign language in terms of sounds found in their native language may be a more important cause of foreign accent than any limitation on phonetic learning imposed by neurophysiological maturation.

Aitchison's classification (1989:67) of some 'hallmarks' based on Lenneberg (1967) also draws a line between phonetic and grammatical acquisition. Aitchison says that biologically controlled behavior such as pronunciation has the following characteristics:

1. The behavior emerges before it is necessary.
2. Its appearance is not the result of a conscious decision.
3. Its emergence is not triggered by external events (though the surrounding environment must be sufficiently 'rich' for it to develop adequately).
4. Direct teaching and intensive practice have relatively little effect.
5. There is a regular sequence of 'milestones' as the behavior develops, and these can usually be correlated with age and other aspects of development.

It sounds reasonable for language teachers to examine the five points above when they are designing a language course. The characteristics look similar to the behaviorist approach since they claim the 'priority of behavior' and 'exclusiveness of consciousness.' The remarkable feature lies in the evaluation of 'rich' quantitative and qualitative environments.

2.5.3. The Different Acquisition by Children and Adults

A large number of the past studies conclude that the acquisition of pronunciation is different from that of grammar. Here the other pieces of research are summarized in view of phonological acquisition. They all show that language acquisition by adults and children are also somewhat different.

Bailey, Madden and Krashen (1974) reveals that there is no difference in the order of acquisition of English morphemes between children and adults. They do not support the CPH because ages do not appear to affect the morphological developmental patterns. Burstall's contrastive survey (1985) of English speakers who started learning French at the age of eight and eleven shows that older learners are consistently superior to younger learners. He found that the only exception was in the field of listening because the younger learners' scores of the listening test were better.

Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohole (1978) traced English speakers' learning of Dutch. The subjects were eight to ten-year-old children, twelve to fifteen-year-old students, and adults. Their achievements were measured in three months, six months, and ten months respectively after the period of learning. The results of morphology and syntax tests showed that twelve to fifteen year old adolescents scored the best, the adults came second, and children less than ten years old ranked the last. On the other hand, their differences in pronunciation were small, and the children less than ten years old caught up later in their grammar. Olsen and Samuels (1973) is also an experimental study in which adults outperformed children. They made an experiment on German pronunciation by English speakers in the United States and found that adults did significantly better than children after ten sessions.

Conversely, frequently cited research with Japanese children and adults shows that adults did not get better results than children (Cochrane 1980). Subjects were asked to distinguish English /r/ and /l/ sounds after exposure of 245 hours for the adults and 193 hours for the children. The children outperformed the adults, but after the adults were taught the phonemic distinction in the follow-up experiment, the adult got better scores, while children did not.

Sato's study (1987) suggests that open (CV) syllable preference in Japanese is not evident in second language learners' pronunciation when they began learning before the age of twelve. But Tarone's study (1987) about the learners beginning after twelve years old, shows a difference. The acquisition of pronunciation is the field in which adult learners cannot perform better than children in many cases because speaking and listening are heavily based on natural learning environments outside classroom.

Oyama's study (1976) includes two adult native speakers' judgment of naturalness of accents. She investigated 60 male residents in the United States who had come there aged six to twenty and reported that the age of arrived in the States had a strong effect. But the number of years of residence in the States had a negligible effect in cases where they started learning languages after puberty. Her data suggests that it is important for 'young' learners to be exposed to their target language sufficiently to acquire a natural pronunciation. To sum up, above-mentioned reports support the CPH on listening and speaking despite some social and psychological variances.

3. Conclusion

It is difficult to derive a simple conclusion for adult second language acquisition from the complicated results because most results are reached by different methods. The neurological evidence does not provide firm supports of the CPH. The past studies are diachronic, synchronic, or experimental, each varying in the starting ages of language learning. The crucial question of 'whether the difference between child and adult learners can be explained by the difference of their biological mechanisms for language learning' is still open. Lenneberg (1967) has been proved wrong on the theories about lateralization and about Down's syndrome children, but his CPH will be a crucial idea forever. One supporter of the CPH proposes a neurological mental muscle model (Long 1990) claiming that access to the language-specific endowment which remains intact after puberty is progressively impeded to varying degrees with aging, unless the faculty is used daily and kept plastic. Another anti-CPH supporter claims that adults and children appear to behave very much in the same manner, which indicates that activation of certain linguistic development is dependent on the existence of specific environmental factors, rather than on the different cognitive abilities of children and adults (Muhlhauser 1986:266). In Flege's opinion (1987), the CPH is a priori assumption about the basis of adult-child differences rather than a testable hypothesis. He goes so far as to say that acceptation of the CPH may impede the development of specific hypotheses that can be tested.

One of the important reasons for the disagreement of results is that a large number of studies involve learners in a classroom environment. In the artificial situation adults can do better because children take a while to adjust themselves to the new environments and unfamiliar teaching methods. Another reason is a confusion of pronunciation and grammar. Since the acquisition of pronunciation and grammar are different in nature, it seems natural for each of them to have independent critical periods. Phonological acquisition seems to be more sensitive to the critical period than that of grammar. This leads to a theory of the multiple critical periods.

While it is believed that children need to learn a second language before their critical period in order to acquire a natural pronunciation in informal daily learning, it is also true that some children fail to acquire a native-like proficiency under the influence of their first language. Adult learners, on the other hand, can succeed in acquiring a native-like pronunciation with their efforts and artificial instructions. As this chapter explored language learning after critical periods, the results are good motivations for adult English speaking learners of Japanese. Adult learners of a second language should not give up native-like naturalness. Conversely, once learners accept the idea of the critical period, they should not stick to the view that a native-like pronunciation is essential for their communicative skills. Language teachers should also make every effort to arrange the most effective learning environment which enables the students to activate their fully developed cognitive skills in both natural and classroom settings.

4. References

Aitchison, Jean. 1989. The Articulate mammal: an introduction to psycholinguistics. Routledge.
Birdsong, D.. 1992. 'Ulitimate attainment in second language acquisition'. Language. 68. 706-755.
Brown, E. R.. 1958. Words and Things. The Free Press.
Cochrane, R. McCrae and Sachs, Jacqueline. 1979. 'Phonological learning by children and adults in a laboratory setting.' Language and Speech. 22(2). 145-147.
Coppieters, R. 1987. 'Competence differences between native and near-native speakers'. Language. 63. 544-573.
Crystal, D. 1987. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge University Press.
Curtiss, S. 1977. Genie: a psycholinguistic study of modern day 'wild child'. Academic Press.
Curtiss, S. 1988. 'Abnormal language acquisition and grammar: evidence for the modularity of language.' in Newmeyer (1988), 2.
Curtiss, S., Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Rigler, D., and Rigler, M. 1974. 'The linguistic development of Genie'. Language. 50. 528-554.
Ellis, Rod. 1994. The Study of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford University Press.
Flege, James Emil. 1981. 'The Phonological Basis of Foreign Accent: A hypothesis'. TESOL Quarterly. 15. 4. 443-455.
Flege, James Emil. 1987. 'A critical Period for Learning to Pronounce Foreign Language?' Applied Linguistics. 8. 162-177.
Fokes, Joann., Bond, Z. S., and Steinberg, Marcy. 1985. 'Acquisition of the English voicing contrast by Arab Children.' Language and Speech. 81-92.
Geschwind, N. 1983. 'Biological Foundations of Language and Hemispheric Dominance'. in Studdert-Kennedy, M. (ed.) 1983. 62-68.
Kess, Joseph F. 1992. Psycholinguistics. Benjamins
Klashen, S., Long, M., and Scarcella, R. 1979. 'Age, rate and eventual attainment in second language acquisition'. TESOL Quarterly. 13. 573-582.
Klashen, S. D.. 1981. Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning. Pergamon.
Lenneberg, E. H.. 1967. Biological Foundations of Language. Wiley.
Loewenthal, Kate and Bull, Deborah. 1984. 'Imitation of foreign sounds: What is the effect of age?' Language and Speech. 27. 95-98.
Long, M. 1990. 'Maturational constraints on language development' Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 12. 251-286.
Mackain, K., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., and Stern, D.. 1983. 'Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function'. Science. 219. 1347-1349.
Miller , G. A. 1963. 'Some effects of grammatical transformations on the recall of English sentences.' Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 2. 346-351.
Neufeld, G. 1978. 'On the acquisition of prosodic and articulatory features in adult language learning'. Canadian Modern Language Review. 34. 45-60.
Newmayer, F. J. 1988. Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey 2. Cambridge University Press.
Olsen, L. and Samuels, S. 1973. 'The relationship between age and accuracy of foreign language pronunciation.' Journal of Educational Research. 66. 263-267.
Patkowski, Mark S. 1990. 'Age and Accent in a Second Language: A Reply to James Emil Flege'. Applied Linguistics. 11(1). 73-89.
Richards, Jack C., Platt, John., and Platt Heidi. 1992. Longman Dictionary of Language Teaching and Applied Linguistics. Longman
Scovel, T. 1988. A time to speak: a psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech. Newbury House.
Singleton, D. 1989. Language Acquisition: The Age Factor. Multilingual Matters.
Slobin, Dan I. 1971. Psycholinguistics. Scott, Foresman and Company.
Snow, C. and Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. 1978. 'The critical age for language acquisition: evidence from second language learning'. Child Development. 49. 1114-1128.
Snow, C. and Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. 1982. 'School age second language learners' access to simplified linguistic input'. Language Learning. 32. 411-430.
Sonia, Tahta and Wood, Margaret. 1981. 'Foreign Accents: Factors relating to a second language'. Language and Speech. 24(3). 265-272.
Sonia, Tahta and Wood, Margaret. 1981. 'Age changes in the ability to rreplicate foreign pronunciation and intonation.' Language and Speech 24(4) 363-372.
Steinburg, Danny. 1982. Psycholinguistics. NewYork: Longman.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (ed.) 1983. Psychology of Language. MIT Press.
Thompson, E. 1991. 'Foreign accents revisited: the English pronunciation of Russian immigrants'. Language Learning. 41. 177-204.
Villers, J. J. and Villers, P. A.. 1978. Language Acquisition. London: Harvard University Press.

0x01 graphic

(c) Katsumi NAGAI 1997: Jump to top



Wyszukiwarka

Podobne podstrony:
Han, Z H & Odlin, T Studies of Fossilization in Second Language Acquisition
Han, Z H & Odlin, T Studies of Fossilization in Second Language Acquisition
Han and Odlin Studies of Fossilization in Second Language Acquisition
Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis
Does Critical Period Play a Role in Second language acquisition
A Critical Look at the Concept of Authenticity
Drilling Fluid Yield Stress Measurement Techniques for Improved understanding of critical fluid p
Age and the Acquisition of English As a Foreign Language
Ellis R The study of second language acquisition str 41 72, 299 345
Explaining the first language acquisition overview of theories 4
Johnson, M A Philosophy of Second Language Acquisition 0300100264
Stages of L1 language acquisition IV
ASSESSMENT OF EMPIRICAL EQUATIONS FOR PREDICTING FUNDAMENTAL PERIODS OF TUNNEL FORM BUILDINGS
Year II SLA #3 Theories of First Language Acquisition
British Patent 2,812 Improvements in Methods of and Apparatus for the Generation of Electric Current
The Critical Period Hypothesis; The Unholy Grail of Psycholinguistics
Drilling Fluid Yield Stress Measurement Techniques for Improved understanding of critical fluid p
Age and the Acquisition of English as a Foreign Language (eds M del Pilar Garcia Mayo&M L Garcia Lec

więcej podobnych podstron