2004 Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek From Clitics to Affixes Palgrave Studies in Language History and Language Change

background image

Variation and

Morphosyntactic Change

in Greek

From Clitics to Affixes

Panayiotis A. Pappas

background image

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

background image

Palgrave Studies in Language History and Language Change
Series Standing Order ISBN 0–333–99009–9
(outside North America only)

You can receive future titles in this series as they are published by placing a standing order.
Please contact your bookseller or, in case of difficulty, write to us at the address below with
your name and address, the title of the series and the ISBN quoted above.

Customer Services Department, Macmillan Distribution Ltd, Houndmills, Basingstoke,
Hampshire RG21 6XS, England

Palgrave Studies in Language History and Language Change

Series Editor: Professor Charles Jones

This new monograph series will present scholarly work in an increasingly
active area of linguistic research. It will deal with a worldwide range of language
types and will present both descriptive and theoretically-orientated accounts of
language change through time. Aimed at the general theoretician as well as the
historical specialist, the series will seek to be a meeting ground for a wide range
of different styles and methods in historical linguistics.

Titles include:

Panayiotis A. Pappas
VARIATION AND MORPHOSYNTACTIC CHANGE IN GREEK
From Clitics to Affixes

Ghil‘ad Zuckermann
LANGUAGE CONTACT AND LEXICAL ENRICHMENT IN ISRAELI HEBREW

Forthcoming titles:

Betty S. Phillips
WORD FREQUENCY AND LEXICAL DIFFUSION

background image

Variation and
Morphosyntactic Change
in Greek

From Clitics to Affixes

Panayiotis A. Pappas

background image

© Panayiotis A. Pappas 2004

All rights reserved. No reproduction, copy or transmission of this
publication may be made without written permission.

No paragraph of this publication may be reproduced, copied or transmitted
save with written permission or in accordance with the provisions of the
Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, or under the terms of any licence
permitting limited copying issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency, 90
Tottenham Court Road, London W1T 4LP.

Any person who does any unauthorised act in relation to this publication
may be liable to criminal prosecution and civil claims for damages.

The author has asserted his right to be identified as the author of this work
in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.

First published 2004 by
PALGRAVE MACMILLAN
Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire RG21 6XS and
175 Fifth Avenue, New York, N. Y. 10010
Companies and representatives throughout the world

PALGRAVE MACMILLAN is the global academic imprint of the Palgrave
Macmillan division of St. Martin’s Press, LLC and of Palgrave Macmillan Ltd.
Macmillan® is a registered trademark in the United States, United Kingdom
and other countries. Palgrave is a registered trademark in the European
Union and other countries.

ISBN 1–4039–1334–X

This book is printed on paper suitable for recycling and made from fully
managed and sustained forest sources.

A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Pappas, Panayiotis A., 1971–

Variation and morphosyntactic change in Greek / Panayiotis A. Pappas.

p. cm. — (Palgrave studies in language history and language change)

Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and index.
ISBN 1–4039–1334–X

1. Greek language, Medieval and late—Pronoun. 2. Greek language,

Medieval and late—Syntax. 3. Greek language, Medieval and late–
–Clitics. 4. Greek language, Medieval and late—Affixes. I. Title. II. Series.

PA1085.P36 2003
487’.3—dc21

2003054874

10

9

8

7

6

5

4

3

2

1

13

12

11

10

09

08

07

06

05

04

Printed and bound in Great Britain by
Antony Rowe Ltd, Chippenham and Eastbourne

background image

v

πáντα ε, οδèν δè μéνει

Heracleitus (Fr. 20)

background image

This page intentionally left blank

background image

vii

Contents

List of Figures
List of Tables
Preface
List of Abbreviations

1.

Introduction

Preliminaries

Summary of the placement of unstressed elements throughout
the history of Greek
Weak object pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek
texts

Previous descriptions

Rollo (1989)
Mackridge (1993, 1995)

Contribution of the study

2.

Methodology

Linguistic usage vs. linguistic competence
Compiling a corpus of texts
Accountability and the linguistic variable in morphosyntactic
research

Defining the set of variants

The possessive pronouns
The definite article and relative pronouns

Conclusion

3.

Data Analysis

Raw data
Data analysis

Postverbal placement of pronouns
Preverbal placement of pronouns
Comparing postverbal and preverbal pronoun placement

Conclusion

4.

Linguistic Parameters

The distinction between

ο and ν ο

Differentiation within the factor reduplicated object
The effect of emphasis on pronoun placement

x

xi

xiii

xvii

1
1

4

6
8
8
9

12

15
15
18

22
26
26
27
29

30
30
34
35
38
41
43

44
44
51
55

background image

viii

Contents

The effect of discourse constraints on pronoun placement
Weak object pronoun placement with non-finite forms of the
verb

Participles

Present Active
Perfect Passive

Gerund
Infinitives

Articular Infinitive
Infinitive as the complement of a verb

Imperative

Conclusion

5.

Non-linguistic Parameters

Metrical constraints and weak object pronoun placement in
LMG texts

The interaction between accent and pronoun placement
The effect of the caesura

Pronoun placement according to chronology and geography
Conclusion

6.

Previous Proposals

Previous explanations for the LMG facts

Horrocks (1990)
Philippaki-Warburton (1995)
Horrocks (1997)
Condoravdi and Kiparsky (2001)

Syntactic approaches to similar phenomena in other languages

Weak object pronoun placement in Old Spanish

Wanner (1991)
Fontana (1993)

Weak object pronoun placement in Bulgarian and Old French
Weak object pronoun placement in some Modern Greek dia-
lects

Other general approaches
Conclusion

7.

A Diachronic Perspective

Pronoun placement in 17

th

century Greek: poetry vs. prose

Examining the pattern of variation from a diachronic perspec-
tive

Further implications for Later Medieval Greek

Conclusion

57

60
61
61
62
63
63
64
64
70
72

73

73
75
81
83
89

92
92
93
93
97
99

104
104
104
106
111

113
114
116

117
117

125
132
135

background image

Contents

ix

8.

Theoretical Implications

Are the weak object pronouns of LMG clitics or affixes?
The development of ‘atypical affixes’ in LMG and the theory of
‘grammaticalization’
Concrete contexts of linguistic change
Grammars with less-than-perfect-generalizations
Conclusion

Appendix: Tables showing variation by text

Notes
References
Index

136
137

141
143
146
150

152

166
170
183

background image

x

List of Figures

Figure 1.1

Figure 3.1
Figure 3.2
Figure 3.3
Figure 3.4
Figure 3.5
Figure 4.1

Figure 4.2

Figure 4.3
Figure 5.1

Figure 5.2

Scale of weak object pronoun placement in LMG according
to Mackridge (1993)
Postverbal pronoun placement for Group 1
Preverbal pronoun placement for Group 2
Preverbal pronoun placement for Group 3
Preverbal pronoun placement by factor
Diagrammatic ranking of pronoun placement
Comparing factors in Group 1 but excluding tokens with
λος from the factor reduplicated object
Four-part analogy schema for the change in pronoun
placement in

λος constructions

Comparing pronominal vs. nominal subjects
Comparison of the effect of metrical requirements on factors
fronted constituent vs. subject and temporal expression
Diagrammatic comparison of the initial and final description
of weak object pronoun placement in LMG

10
36
39
40
42
43

54

55
57

78

90

background image

xi

List of Tables

Table 1.1
Table 1.2
Table 3.1
Table 5.1
Table 5.2

Table 5.3

Table 6.1
Table 7.1

Table 7.2
Table 7.3
Table A.1

Table A.2
Table A.3

Table A.4

Table A.5

Table A.6

Table A.7

Table A.8

Table A.9

Table A.10

Table A.11

Table A.12

Forms of the weak pronouns in Later Medieval Greek texts
Forms of the full pronouns in Medieval Greek
Results of token count based on Mackridge’s classification
Pronoun placement in the Cypriot chronicles
Pronoun placement in western texts according to factor
initial
Pronoun placement in western texts according to factor
coordinating conjunction
Possible verb positions in Later Medieval Greek
Pronoun placement by factor in prose texts from different
areas in 17

th

century Greek

Pronoun placement by factor in 17

th

century Cypriot texts

Pronoun placement by factor in 17

th

century Cretan poetry

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors initial and
coordinating conjunction
Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors

διóτι and τι

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors

ο, δé and

reduplicated object
Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors relative pro-
noun
, negation, and interrogative pronoun
Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors

νá, να, and

ς
Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors tempo-
ral
/comparative conjunction,

áν, ν, and πẃς

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors object,
prepositional phrase, and non-temporal adverb
Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors subject, and
temporal expression
Raw counts of pronoun placement in the contexts

ν ο

and

ο μ

Raw counts concerning the interaction between the pres-
ence of

λος and pronoun placement

Raw counts concerning the interaction between

θéλω peri-

phrastic constructions and pronoun placement
Raw counts concerning the interaction between imperative
verb-forms and pronoun placement

2
3

33
85

88

89

109

118
119
121

152
153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

background image

xii

List of Tables

Table A.13

Table A.14

The effects of metrical requirements on preverbal place-
ment for factors fronted constituent vs. subject and tempo-
ral expression
Verb position by text in Later Medieval Greek

164
165

background image

xiii

Preface

The language of popular Greek literature of the late Byzantine period (12

th

-16

th

century) displays an ordering phenomenon that can only be described as con-
founding. The weak pronominal forms that serve as arguments to a verb must be
adjacent to it, but may appear either before it or after it, as shown in examples
(1) and (2).

(1)

πáλε σς

λαλ

palE

sas

lalo

again

you–IOpl WP

say–1sg Pres

‘Again I say to you’ (Moreas, 715)

(2)

πáλιν

λéγω

σας

palin

lEVo

sas

again

say–1sg Pres

you–IOpl WP

‘Again I say to you’ (Digene

¤

s, 1750)

The present study is a reevaluation of this phenomenon that challenges not

only the analyses that have been proposed so far, but also some important as-
pects of the description of the facts. Up until the last ten years or so, this phe-
nomenon was noticed mainly by those working on critical editions of manu-
scripts, and they only offered descriptive lists for the particular text at hand. In
1989, Antonio Rollo published an article, which took a closer look at the phe-
nomenon and noted certain similarities between the variation in Later Medieval
Greek (LMG) and what has become known as the Tobler-Mussafia ‘Law’ in the
Romance languages. In 1990, a brief description of the facts and a tentative pro-
posal to account for them appeared in Horrocks’ survey of clitic position in the
history of Greek. It was, however, Mackridge’s (1993, 1995) work that brought
the phenomenon to prominence by providing a detailed examination of the
variation and proposing a set of ‘rules’ to account for it. Since then, Mackridge’s
description of the phenomenon has been accepted as the standard.

The factual challenges presented in this book arise from the use of a different

methodology, which combines the traditional belief that ‘every attestation
counts’ with the use of statistical tools to determine which patterns of variation
are significant. The crucial change in perspective is that although the belief in the
validity of under-represented constructions is not totally abandoned, it is also
acknowledged that the evidence in texts represents linguistic performance—not
linguistic competence—and may include ungrammatical or less-than-grammati-

background image

xiv

Preface

cal constructions (recognizing that grammaticality is not necessarily a binary
phenomenon). Arriving at an accurate description is thus a delicate exercise in
weighing the relative importance of various observations; the researcher’s intui-
tion and the power of statistical analysis, in the ideal case, work together to pro-
vide the most illuminating results.

Furthermore, this research goes beyond the work of Mackridge in that it pre-

sents the facts for non-finite forms (the gerund and the infinitive), as well as for
the imperative; these verb-forms are only briefly mentioned in his work. In addi-
tion, several subparts to the general problem of weak object pronoun placement,
which had up to now puzzled researchers, are explained in a principled way that
takes into account not only the synchronic data but also the diachronic develop-
ment of such constructions. Another first for this project is the serious considera-
tion given to the possibility that metrical requirements may influence the place-
ment of the pronoun, a critical observation since much of the evidence comes
from poetic texts. Finally, it is shown that, contrary to standard opinion, there is
indeed some dialectal difference regarding this phenomenon.

Besides establishing a well-documented description of the facts of pronoun

placement variation in LMG texts, which could serve as a credible reference for
any future researchers that become interested, this study also provides an as-
sessment of analyses previously proposed as possible explanations for the phe-
nomenon. In doing this, it reaches beyond the narrow realm of explanations pro-
posed strictly for LMG, and examines viable accounts that have been put for-
ward for other languages which display a similar variation in weak object pro-
noun placement, most notably in Old Romance languages (Old French, Old
Spanish, Old Italian). Most of these attempts have been framed within the Prin-
ciples and Parameters program of research into Universal Grammar and depend
crucially on the mechanism of ‘verb movement’.

However, none of these approaches can explain the entire pattern of variation.

The discussion provided here shows clearly that although these analyses can
account for the basic facts, they break down in the face of complex variation, and
either propose ad hoc solutions or diminish the importance of some exceptions to
the predictions that they make. Moreover, the key problematic aspects of the
variation are identified and it is shown that the very nature of the problem is such
that it cannot be explained in the ways promoted so far. On the basis of evidence
from the 17

th

century, where a clear distinction can be seen between pronoun

placement in poetry and pronoun placement in prose, it is argued that the stabil-
ity of the pattern of variation is the result of a stylistic effect which formalized a
pattern of ongoing change. Thus, it is further argued that, although this phe-
nomenon is perplexing from a synchronic perspective, it can be accounted for
diachronically.

The account developed here takes into consideration the fact that weak object

pronouns in the Koiné and Early Medieval Greek period appear rather systemati-
cally after the head of the phrase (noun or verb) and proposes that the change to

background image

Preface

xv

preverbal placement evident in LMG began when a particular preverbal marker
was reinterpreted as the head of the verbal phrase. The spread of the innovative
pattern is explained as a gradual process based upon the linear order relation-
ships between certain elements and the verb. Once preverbal pronoun placement
was established in this core group of contexts, the spread may have halted for a
period (as evidenced in the data from Cyprus) but eventually came to cover all
instances of finite (non-imperative) verb-forms. As a result of this ongoing
change, grammars emerged in which there is no generalization that captures all
the facts about weak object pronoun placement in LMG, an unexpected and sig-
nificant conclusion.

The final chapter presents a discussion of the theoretical implications of this

study. It is argued that the data presented here and their analysis provide further
evidence that the commonly made three-way distinction among ‘word’, ‘clitic’
and ‘affix’ is uninformative and that a bipartite categorization of ‘word’ vs. ‘af-
fix’ (albeit with atypical members in each category) provides better insight into
the subtleties of the morphosyntactic continuum. Furthermore, it is argued that
under this analysis a change from typical to atypical affix is suggested for the
period of Medieval Greek, providing yet another counterexample to the ‘unidi-
rectionality’ hypothesis of recent ‘grammaticalization’ proposals. In addition, it
is demonstrated that the change from postverbal to preverbal pronoun placement
in Later Medieval and Early Modern Greek provides a robust counterexample to
Kroch’s (1989) claim that the contexts involved in syntactic change are abstract.
Finally, the fact that there is no generalization that covers LMG pronoun place-
ment is discussed from the synchronic perspective, and some thoughts are of-
fered about the possibility of writing grammars with less-than-perfect generali-
zations.

A note about the style of the thesis is also in order. The potential readers of

this work may have various interests and, thus, different backgrounds. Some may
read it out of a simple interest in the Greek language, others may approach it as a
study of language change, while others still may be interested in the particulars
of the variationist analysis or the more general morphosyntactic issues. The aim
of the expository style is to be accessible to those with little or no training in the
technical aspects of the analysis, while at the same time providing a detailed
description of the facts for the satisfaction of the more technically oriented read-
ers. The result is a compromise with a number of clarifications which may seem
redundant to some, as well as a level of technical discussion and terminology
that may present difficulty for others; it is my hope, though, that this compro-
mise will prove successful in helping this work reach a wider audience.

Finally, I would like to thank those whose expertise and support have made

this book possible. To professor Brian Joseph, who patiently and diligently read
through several drafts of this study providing references, pointing out problems,
and offering possible solutions, who has led me by the example of his own work,
I will never be able to fully express my gratitude.

background image

xvi

Preface

I would also like to thank professors Don Winford and Carl Pollard—the

members of my dissertation committee—for their helpful comments, as well as
professor Keith Johnson, who helped me better understand the results of the
statistical analysis. In addition, professors Peter Mackridge and Geoffrey Hor-
rocks were more than willing to communicate via e-mail, and provided me with
some very important information and encouragement. An anonymous reviewer
offered some very helpful suggestions. I, of course, am solely responsible for
any errors. Much of this research was conducted under a National Science Foun-
dation fellowship, a Foreign Language and Areal Studies fellowship or The Ohio
State University Presidential fellowship, all of which I appreciate greatly.

Thanks also go to Steve Hartman Keiser and Jennifer Muller for their support

as fellow doctoral candidates, the Changelings reading group for providing a
forum to test new ideas, and the OSU Linguistics Department for promoting
graduate student scholarship in all possible ways. At Simon Fraser University,
the Department of Linguistics and the Chair in Hellenic Studies have been won-
derful in helping me adjust and in encouraging me to pursue my academic goals.
I am indebted to my editor, Jill Lake, for supporting the project, and for her in-
valuable advice about publishing. Finally, I thank my parents and the rest of my
family on both sides of the Atlantic for their emotional and financial support,
especially during my tour of duty with the Hellenic Navy; and Robin, whose
relentless quest for knowledge is a constant source of inspiration.

P. A. P.

background image

xvii

List of Abbreviations

Acc

accusative case

Dat

dative case

DO

direct object

fem

feminine

Fut

future marker

Gen

genitive case

Impf

imperfective past tense

Impv

imperative

Infin

infinitive

IO

indirect object

msc

masculine

neut

neuter

Nom

nominative case

Part

particle

Pass

passive

Past

perfective past tense

Perf Pass Prcle (PPP)

perfect passive participle

pl

plural

Pres

present tense

Pres Act Prcle

present active participle

PS

possessive pronoun

Rel prn

relative pronoun

sg

singular

Subjun

subjunctive marker

WP

weak pronoun

background image

1

1

Introduction

In this chapter, I provide some background information that will help
readers understand the problem of Later Medieval Greek weak object
pronoun placement and its significance for linguistic theory in general.
First, I discuss the reason behind the use of the term ‘weak object pro-
noun’ as opposed to the more common term ‘clitic’, and I give an over-
view of the development of these forms in the history of Greek. In the
second section, I present the details of two previous descriptions of the
phenomenon, and discuss the discrepancies between them, which sug-
gest that a more rigorous evaluation of the phenomenon is needed. Fi-
nally, I discuss why the results of this study have implications that go
beyond Greek, affecting our understanding of such issues as the nature
of clitics and the scope of generalizations in linguistic theory.

Preliminaries

The term ‘weak object pronouns’ is used in this study to refer to a sys-
tem of forms associated with a corresponding system of personal pro-
nouns. These forms are referred to as ‘weak’ because they do not carry
independent stress as the full pronouns do, but are instead always pho-
nologically dependent on an adjacent word which either precedes or
follows them. Forms like these are usually referred to as ‘clitics’ (as is
indeed the case for most of the literature on this specific set of forms),
but that term is avoided here, at least in the description of the phenome-
non, because it is not theory-neutral. Instead, ‘clitic’ has been used in the
literature as a theoretical term, which means that for many readers its use
would lead them to expect a certain pattern of behavior for these ele-

background image

2

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

ments. However, the various uses of the term in the literature are many
times discordant with each other, a fact that has deprived the term of any
meaning and has rendered it a point of confusion instead of illumination
(cf. Zwicky 1994). The term ‘weak pronoun’, on the other hand, does
not carry such theoretical baggage, and appears to be the best term to
employ for the description of the variation. The weak pronouns used in
the corpus of texts as objects of the verb are presented in Table 1.1.

Table 1.1

Forms of the weak pronouns in Later Medieval Greek texts

First

Second

Third (msc–fem–neut)

Singular

Acc

μé

σé

τóν-τν-τó

mE

sE

ton

–tin–to

Gen

μο

σο

το-τς-το

mu

su

tu

–tis–tu

Plural

Acc

μς

σς

τοúς-τíς, τéς, τáς-τá

mas

sas

tus

–tis, tEs, tas–ta

Gen

μς

σς

τν

mas

sas

ton

These are the most commonly found forms. However, in Cypriot and

Cretan texts, one can also find the forms

τσ /tsi/ for feminine genitive

singular and

τσí /tsi/ for the accusative plural in all genders. Also, in

some texts the form

τως /tos/ is found for the genitive plural of the third

person. Dative forms of the pronouns are extremely rare, but a few ex-
amples of the dative case are found in the corpus, mostly

μοι /mi/ (first

person singular) and

σοι /si/ (second person singular). It should also be

noted here that, according to Jannaris (1968: §538, 561), the variants

τíς,

τéς, τáς that one finds in the third person feminine accusative plural
were created by analogy to the feminine accusative plural forms of the
definite article.

Direct objects in Later Medieval Greek usually appear in the accusa-

tive, although there are certain verbs (

νθυμομαι /EnTimumE/ ‘I re-

member’,

διαφéρω /DiafEro/ ‘I differ’, ντρéπομαι /EndrEpomE/ ‘I am

ashamed’) that take a direct object in the genitive case in Ancient and
Hellenistic Greek, and do so to some extent in LMG as well. In Standard
Modern Greek, these verbs take objects in the accusative, which has be-
come the default direct object case, via an accusative-for-genitive
change that had already begun in Medieval times—cf. Jannaris (1968:

background image

Introduction

3

§1242-1299). Indirect objects, which appeared in the dative case in An-
cient Greek appear either in the genitive or the accusative in LMG. (In
Standard Modern Greek only genitive forms are accepted as indirect
objects, although in the north-eastern dialects the accusative is used as
well—cf. Newton 1972, Browning 1983). Thus it is safe to say that in
Later Medieval Greek the forms

μο, σο, το-τς-το, τν are geni-

tive in form, but function as both direct and indirect object (henceforth
DO and IO), while the forms

μé, σé, τóν-τν-τó are accusative in form

but function both as DO and IO also. Finally, the plural form

τους is

accusative in form but functions as a DO, IO or as a possessive pronoun
(PS).

Table 1.2

Forms of the full pronouns in Medieval Greek (after Browning

1983: 62-63)

First

Second

Third (msc–fem–neut)

Singular

Nom

γẃ

σú

ατóς-ατ-ατó

EVo

Esi

aftos, afti, afto

Acc

μé!να"

σé!να" ατóν-ατν-ατó

EmE[na]

EsE[na]

afton

–aftin–afto

Gen

μο

σο

ατο-ατς-ατο

Emu

Esu

aftu

–aftis–aftu

Plural

Nom

μες

σες

ατοí-αταí-ατá

Emis

Esis

afti

–aftE–afta

Acc

μς

σς

ατοúς-ατéς ατáς-ατá

Emas

Esas

aftus

–aftEs aftas–afta

Gen

μν

σν

ατν

Emon

Eson

afton

The full pronouns, which are associated with the forms in Table 1.1

were rare in use, and were employed mainly for emphasis (Jannaris
1968: §525 ff. see also Table 1.2). This system of full personal pronouns
emerged in the Early Medieval Greek stages with the extension of /E/
from the first person singular to the second (

σú /sy/ in Hellenistic

Greek), then to second plural, and finally to first plural (replacing forms
such as

σé /sE/ second singular accusative, #μς /ymas/ second plural

accusative, and

$μς /imas/ first plural accusative—Horrocks 1997:

126-127). In addition, the weak forms of the third person are a creation
belonging entirely to this period; the full forms had alternants without /f/
(e.g.,

%τóν /aton/) from which—it is usually claimed (cf. Browning

background image

4

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

1983, Horrocks 1997)—the weak pronouns developed via aphaeresis of
pretonic initial vowels, a general phonological change of the time. Ac-
cording to Dressler (1966: 57), this hypothesis is untenable because, in
his research of the relevant textual evidence, aphaeresis is common with
prepositions and relative pronouns but not with third person pronouns.
Joseph (in preparation) further points out that aphaeresis with initial /a/
was sporadic and, therefore, could not have played a seminal role in this
development. Instead, he suggests that it was the Koiné and Ancient
Greek pronominal usages of the article

τóν—a carryover from its Ho-

meric usage and Indo-European origin—together with the emergence of
the third person relative pronoun

τó that were the key factors in the de-

velopment of the weak pronoun system for the third person.

Summary of the placement of unstressed elements throughout the
history of Greek

The position of unstressed elements in the clause has been extensively
researched for Ancient Greek (Homeric and Classical), as interest was
generated by the statement of the well-known ‘law’ of Wackernagel
(1892), which stated that, in Indo-European languages, clitic elements
occupied the second position in a clause. Recent research has demon-
strated that in some languages this position can also be defined as the
second daughter in a clause (Halpern 1995). Horrocks (1990) considers
the position of clitic elements in Greek from a diachronic perspective.
He claims that these elements have moved from second position in the
earliest attested Greek (9

th

-6

th

century BC, example 1), to a ‘mixed’

situation in Classical Greek (5

th

-3

rd

century BC), where clitics show both

an affinity for second position and a tendency to attach to the head con-
stituents that select them as arguments (see examples 2 and 3 below).

(1)

καì σφι

ε'δε

(πασι

τéκνα

kai

sphi

EidE

hapasi

tEkna

and

he–Dat pl

see–3sg Past

all–Dat pl

child–Acc pl

‘And (he) saw children (born) for all of them’ (Hdt. I, 30, 4—from Hor-
rocks 1990: 36)

(2)

πυρετοì

παρηκολοúθουν

μοι

συνεχες

pyretoi

pare…kolu…thu…n

moi

sunekhe…s

fevers

follow–3pl Pres

I–Datsg

continual–Nom pl

‘But continual fevers hounded me’ (Dem. 54.11—ibid: 40)

background image

Introduction

5

(3)

καì μοι

λéγε

kai

moi

lege

and

I–Dat sg

tell–2sg Impv

‘And tell me’ (Dem. 37.17—ibid: 40)

During the Koiné and Early Medieval Greek period (2

nd

century BC-

10

th

century AD, cf. example 4) ‘most clitic pronouns follow immedi-

ately after the verbs or nouns that govern them …’ (Horrocks 1990: 44).
In Later Medieval Greek, weak pronoun placement depends on the type
of phrase. In noun-phrases the pronouns are placed after the noun (as in
example 5) almost categorically.

1

In verb-phrases, on the other hand,

they appear mostly before the verb (example 6), yet the number of cases
in which the pronouns appear after the verb is by no means negligible
(example 7).

(4)

το ο+νου

ο.

/γραψéς

μοι

tu… ynu

u

EVrapsEs

my

the wine–Gen sg

which–Gen sg

write–2sg Past

I–Dat sg

‘The wine of which you wrote me’ (P. Oxy. 1220—ibid: 44)

(5)

γυρεúουσι

τà ταíρια

τους

VirEvusi

ta tErja

tus

search–3pl Pres

the mates–DOpl

they–PSpl WP

‘They search for their mates’ (Katalogia, 708)

(6)

πáλε σς

λαλ

palE

sas

lalo

again

you–IOpl WP

say–1sg Pres

‘Again I say to you’ (Moreas, 715)

(7)

πáλιν

λéγω

σας

palin

lEVo

sas

again

say–1sg Pres

you–IOpl WP

‘Again I say to you’ (Digene¤s, 1750)

Finally, in Standard Modern Greek the position of pronouns in verb-

phrases becomes fixed as pronouns appear before the verb (example 8)
except when the verb is of imperative (example 9) or gerund form (ex-

background image

6

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

ample 10). For the sake of consistency, I use the polytonic system of
orthography throughout the book.

(8)

μς

μιλ

mas

mila

we–IOpl WP

talk–3sg Pres

‘S/He is talking to us’

(9)

μíλα

μας

mila

mas

talk–2sg Impv

we–IOpl WP

‘Talk to us!’

(10)

μιλẃντας

μας

milondas

mas

talk–Gerund

we–IOpl WP

‘Talking to us’

Weak object pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek texts

This section provides an initial description of the facts concerning the
position of weak object pronouns in LMG texts. The first observation
one makes is that the pronouns must appear adjacent to the verb. There
are a few counterexamples to this (example 11) but they are considered
archaic formulations, reflecting the freer position of pronouns in earlier
periods of the language (cf. Mackridge 1993).

(11)

καì μè

1 νος

σéβασεν

kE

mE

o nus

EsEvasEn

and

I–DOsg WP

the mind–Nom sg

put–3sg Past

‘And (my) mind put me’ (Thre¤nos, 1000)

Second, if both the direct and the indirect object of the verb have been

pronominalized, both pronouns appear on the same side of the verb with
the indirect object pronoun preceding the direct object pronoun (see ex-
amples 12 and 13).

(12)

3σàν

σè

τò

%φηγομαι

osan

sE

to

afiVumE

as

you–IOsg WP

it–DOsg WP

narrate–1sg Pres

‘As I am narrating it to you’ (Moreas, 630)

background image

Introduction

7

(13)

λ4

%φηγθη

τς

τα

ola

afiViTi

tis

ta

all–DOpl

narrate–1sg Past

she–IOsg WP

they–DOpl WP

‘All things, I narrated them to her’ (Lybistros, 1847)

The position, however, of the pronouns with respect to the verb (pre-

verbal or postverbal) is not fixed, and at first glance seems to be uncon-
strained. For example, in sentences (14) and (15) the pronouns are pre-
verbal and postverbal respectively despite the fact that the syntactic en-
vironment and even the content of the clauses are the same. The same is
true for examples (6) and (7) in the previous section, and in examples
(12) and (13) just above.

(14)

1 δοùξ

τοùς

%ποδéχθηκεν

o Duks

tus

apoDExTikEn

the duke–Nom sg

they–DOpl WP

receive–3sg Past

‘The duke received them’ (Phlo¤rios, 304)

(15)

κι 1 βασιλεùς

δéχθην

τους

kj o vasilEfs

EDExTin

tus

and the king–Nom sg

receive–3sg Past

they–DOpl WP

‘And the king received them’ (Phlo¤rios, 939)

This pattern of variation does not seem to be affected by the type of

verb-form that is used in the construction. Even if an imperative verb-
form or a participle is used, both preverbal and postverbal pronoun
placement are possible (examples 16 and 17, and 18 and 19 respec-
tively).

(16)

6γíα τ7ν

ε8πé

aVia

tin

ipE

holy

she–DOsg WP

call–Impv sg

‘Call her holy’ (Thre¤nos, 35)

(17)

ε8ς τ7ν καρδíα σου

θéς

το

is tin karDia su

TEs

to

in the heart your

place–Impv sg

it–DOsg WP

‘Place it in your heart’ (Spaneas, 135)

background image

8

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(18)

καì σον

σè

φυλáττων

kE soon

sE

filaton

and safe–Acc sg

you–DOsg WP

keep–Pres Act Prcle

‘And keeping you safe’ (Glykas, 341)

(19)

τòν

καταφλéξαντá

σε

ton

kataflEksanda

sE

the–Acc sg

burn thoroughly–Pres Act Prcle

you–DOsg WP

‘The one who burnt you thoroughly’ (Achilleid, 1410)

Despite this first impression, however, some strong tendencies for the

placement of these pronouns can be detected. The issue was briefly dis-
cussed by Joseph (1978/1990) and Gemert (1980), while Horrocks
(1990) also dealt with it in his review of clitic placement throughout the
history of Greek. However, it was Rollo (1989) and Mackridge (1993,
1995) who gave the first detailed descriptions of the phenomenon and
who posited the hypothesis that the placement of the pronoun is influ-
enced by the nature of the element which immediately precedes the
verb–pronoun (or pronoun–verb) complex. In fact, Mackridge’s descrip-
tion is accepted as the standard for understanding this phenomenon (cf.
Philippaki-Warburton 1995, Janse 1994 and 1998, Horrocks 1997, Jans-
sen 1998), so in a sense it falls under the rubric of general knowledge
concerning the placement of the pronouns. In the next section I provide
summaries of the descriptions given by Rollo and Mackridge.

Previous descriptions

Rollo (1989)

In this brief paper, Rollo draws comparisons between the ‘use of enclisis
in vulgar Greek’ and the ‘law’ of Tobler-Mussafia, which describes the
pattern of preverbal and postverbal pronoun placement in Old Romance
(Old French, Old Italian, Old Spanish, and their dialectal varieties).
Rollo describes the Late Byzantine pattern thus: The pronoun is placed
after the verb when the immediately preceding element is a subject, a
‘declarative conjunction’ such as

τι /oti/ or διóτι /Dioti/, a verbal argu-

ment whose information is repeated by a ‘pleonastic’ pronoun, an ad-
verb, a verbal argument, the negative marker

ο /u/, or the conjuntion

καí /kE/ meaning ‘also’. The pronoun is placed preverbally when the
immediately preceding element is a ‘subordinating conjunction’, the

background image

Introduction

9

‘particle’

νá /na/, the hortative ς /as/, any of the relative and interroga-

tive pronouns, or negatives other than

ο. These descriptions are stated

in absolute terms although Rollo offers several cases for which there are
exceptions, most notably, for preceding subjects, complements and ad-
verbs where one finds some examples of preverbal pronoun placement.

Mackridge (1993, 1995)

Working without knowledge of Rollo’s research, Mackridge (1993,
1995) presents a much more detailed look at weak pronoun (which he
calls ‘clitic pronoun’) placement, mainly in the Digene¤s text, although he
discusses a few examples from other texts as well. He successfully
shows that this position is not a ‘free for all’, as was previously assumed
by editors who would change the position of a weak pronoun in order to
satisfy metrical requirements. Instead, he demonstrates that there is a
strong correspondence between the nature of the element that immedi-
ately precedes the verb–pronoun complex and the pronoun’s position.
He identifies these correspondences as lists of environments, which enter
into rule formulations as parameters determining the placement of the
pronoun. These rules describe a scale of pronoun placement, which
ranges from obligatory postverbal order to obligatory preverbal order.

The environments that he associates with ‘more or less’ obligatory

postverbal pronoun placement are: a) when the verb is in clause-initial
position, or b) if the verb is immediately preceded by one of the follow-
ing elements: a coordinating conjunction (

καí /kE/ ‘and’, %λλá /ala/, μá

/ma/,

%μ /ami/ ‘but’, οδé /uDE/, μηδé /miDE/ ‘neither’, 9 /i/ ‘or’), a

reduplicated object,

2

the negative marker

ο(κ) /uk/

3

‘not’, the comple-

mentizer

τι /oti/ ‘that’, the causal conjunction διóτι /Dioti/ ‘because’,

and the conditional conjunction

ε8 /i/ ‘if’.

Next, Mackridge states that when the verb complex is immediately

preceded by a temporal adverb, the placement of the pronoun can vary
freely between preverbal and postverbal position. The position of the
pronoun is more likely to be preverbal when the preceding element is a
subject. Preceding ‘semantically emphasized’ constituents (such as an
object, a non-temporal adverb or a predicative argument) are even more
strongly associated with preverbal pronoun placement. Mackridge calls
this ordering ‘almost obligatory’. Finally, he describes the position of the
pronoun as ‘more or less obligatorily’ preverbal in those cases where the
complex is immediately preceded by any one of the following: the sub-
junctive marker

νá /na/, the hortative particle ς /as/, the future marker

θá /Ta/, the negative markers μ /mi/, μηδéν /miDEn/, δéν /DEn/, οδéν

background image

10

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

/uDEn/ ‘not’, the interrogative pronouns and adverbs (

τíς /tis/, ποιóς

/pios/ ‘who’,

+ντα /inda/ ‘what’, πς /pos/ ‘how’, διατí/γιατí /Djati/

/jati/ ‘why’,

πο /pu/ ‘where’, πóσος /posos/ ‘how much’), the relative

pronouns (

που /opu/, 1ποú /opu/, ποú /pu/ ‘which’, ς /os/, τóν /ton/

‘who’

,σος /osos/ ‘which amount’, στις/ποιος/ε+τις /ostis/ /opjos/

/itis/ ‘whoever’), the complementizer

πẃς /pos/ ‘that’, the temporal and

comparative conjunctions (

πεí /Epi/, ποτε /opotE/, ταν/ντε/ντας

/otan/ /ondE/ /ondas/ all meaning ‘when’,

προτο /protu/ ‘before’, πρíν

/prin/ ‘before’,

%φο/%φóτου/%φẃν /afu/ /afotu/ /afon/ ‘since’, (μα

/ama/ ‘when’,

3ς /os/, 3σáν /osan/, σáν /san/ all meaning ‘when, as’

καθẃς /kaTos/ ‘as’), the final conjunction να /ina/ ‘in order to, to’, and
the conditional conjunctions

ν /an/, áν /Ean/ ‘if’. Figure 1.1 below

provides a diagrammatic summary of Mackridge’s description of the
variation.

preverbal

almost preverbal

normally preverbal

free variation

postverbal

Figure 1.1

Scale of weak object pronoun placement in LMG according to

Mackridge (1993)

Obviously, Mackridge’s description is much more detailed and intri-

cate than that of Rollo, which would lead us to believe that it is also
more accurate. On the other hand, since Mackridge mostly concentrates
on one text (the epic of Digene¤s) and Rollo utilizes examples from many
different texts, the discrepancies between the two descriptions may be
the result of differing databases. This, on its own, emphasizes the need
for a quantitative study of the phenomenon which will include as many
of the available texts, and will draw tokens from each text in as repre-

background image

Introduction

11

sentative a way as possible. Another aspect of Mackridge’s description
that invites the use of a quantificational analysis is that he recognizes
differences in the effect that each environment has on pronoun place-
ment. For example, preverbal pronouns are ‘almost obligatory’ when the
complex is preceded by an object, but such placement is only ‘normal’
when the complex is preceded by a subject. But what exactly do these
terms mean? Certainly, we will gain greater insight into the pattern of
variation if we are able to quantify these rankings.

Whether or not these differences are confirmed, the emerging pattern

of variation presents us with a linguistic phenomenon that requires ex-
planation. What are the structural reasons for this variation? According
to Mackridge (1993: 329), the explanation is two-fold. Part of the place-
ment pattern can be explained in ‘purely syntactical terms’: ‘there are
certain words or classes of words that are followed by V + P … while
there are others that are followed by P + V.’ But how can a descriptive
list such as the one that he provides be a ‘purely syntactical’ explana-
tion? One wonders whether it is possible to state a generalization that
covers all these separate words and word classes in a principled way,
instead of simply listing them. Most linguists would expect the former,
but if the latter proves true, then that in itself is interesting and impor-
tant. Even if it is so, however, one should still try to explain why it is this
particular set of words that is associated with preverbal pronoun place-
ment, and not another. In essence, Mackridge’s rules are mostly stipula-
tive, and do not help us understand the variation.

The other part of the placement pattern is explained as ‘cases where

the pronoun precedes the verb because some constituent is placed before
the verb-phrase for reasons of emphasis.’ Indeed, Mackridge attributes
great significance to semantically emphasized elements ‘attracting’ the
pronouns to their position. In fact, he maintains that the distinction
among the classes of elements seen in Figure 1.1 is based on the fact that
preverbal objects are more emphatic than preverbal subjects which in
turn are more emphatic than preverbal temporal adverbs. However, he
does not go into detail about the nature of emphasis or the mechanism of
‘attraction’. Given the weight that both of these terms are given in the
exposition it is unfortunate that they are not explained more fully.

These questions show that despite Mackridge’s contribution, the phe-

nomenon of weak object pronoun placement variation in Later Medieval
Greek has not been thoroughly explained. At most, it has been ade-
quately described. In the subsequent chapters, I will propose a method-

background image

12

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

ology to be employed in the investigation of the variation, and then seek
empirical confirmation of Mackridge’s description. Once the facts have
been disclosed in detail, I will attempt to explain the pattern of variation.

Contribution of the study

The effort to arrive at a better understanding of weak object pronoun
placement in Later Medieval Greek has implications that go beyond
solving a puzzle for a brief period in the history of a single language.
Rather, the results of this study expand our knowledge in several ways
which impact the field of Greek Linguistics, research on clitics, as well
as our views about the nature of generalizations.

Although Greek is a language that has been researched in depth, our

knowledge of Medieval Greek is particularly limited, especially since
there is a genuine lack of texts or other documents that record the ver-
nacular of the time. This is indeed unfortunate because many of the more
interesting constructions in Modern Greek—such as the replacement of
infinitival complementation by finite subordinate clauses, the use of a
periphrastic future and perfect, and the placement of weak pronouns (as
verbal and nominal arguments)—have their origin, or become estab-
lished in Medieval Greek. As will be demonstrated in a later chapter,
there is considerable amount of interaction among these constructions,
and the topic at hand—weak object pronoun placement—is the least
understood of the three.

4

Apart from providing an important missing

piece of Medieval Greek syntax, an account of this phenomenon also
plays a major role in understanding the eventual development of Greek
dialects, which are differentiated on the basis of pronoun position, as
was clearly demonstrated in Condoravdi and Kiparsky (2001). Although
I disagree with the specifics of their proposal for LMG pronoun place-
ment (cf. Chapter 6), I concur that these facts are pivotal to our under-
standing of the eventual division of Greek into dialects with (mostly)
preverbal pronoun placement on the one hand, and dialects with postver-
bal pronouns on the other.

The results of this study also contribute to the general knowledge

about clitics. Despite the vast amount of research that has been con-
ducted on these nefarious elements in the past 110 years, it seems that
the amount of variation within this category (if indeed it is a separate
category) is inexhaustible. Thus, after the initial classification of clitics
into simple clitics, special clitics, and bound words by Zwicky (1977),
subsequent research proliferated the different types of elements that be-

background image

Introduction

13

long to each of these classes, especially for special clitics. Halpern
(1998), for example, subdivides special clitics into second-position
clitics which must appear second in a domain that is differently deter-
mined for individual languages, and verbal clitics, which must appear
adjacent to a verb. Moreover, he separates this latter group into verbal
clitics, which behave as inflectional affixes (these have a fixed position
with relation to the verb), and verbal clitics which resemble second-po-
sition clitics in that they sometimes appear before the verb and some-
times after it, depending on the nature of the element that precedes them.

At first glance, this seems to be the exact same situation that we have

in LMG, except that, as will be argued later, LMG weak pronouns are
always phonologically attached to the verb, either as enclitics or pro-
clitics, whereas the ‘Tobler-Mussafia Law clitics’ have been character-
ized as enclitics. These elements thus present us with a new case of spe-
cial clitic, and a detailed investigation of their behavior undoubtedly will
have significant theoretical implications. For example, Condoravdi and
Kiparsky’s (2001) typological study of clitic pronouns in Post-Hellenis-
tic Greek brought them to the conclusion that, on the one hand, the dis-
tinction between clitics and affixes is not a gradient one (so Janse 1998),
but, on the other, the bipartite classification of X

max

and X

0

clitics pro-

posed by Halpern and Fontana (1994) is not sufficient either. Instead
they argue for a three-way classification which includes X

max

clitics, X

0

lexical clitics but also X

0

syntactic clitics. The detailed investigation of

LMG weak pronouns presented here will provide the necessary data to
assess this new proposal. It is interesting to note here that while Zwicky
(1977) claims that a typology of clitics provides insight into historical
change, studies such as the ones cited above and the one presented in
this book prove that examinations of the historical development of clitics
are equally informative about the nature of these elements.

Perhaps the most significant issue addressed by the results of this

study is the role of generalizations in linguistic theory. It is not an exag-
geration to state that discovering the generalizations that govern linguis-
tic knowledge has been the holy grail of linguistics. In using this meta-
phor, I am aware of and wish to allude to all its implications, since there
has not yet been a description of a language that depends solely on gen-
eralizations. Instead, many patterns have to be specified for particular
classes of lexemes or even individual ones. Nonetheless, linguists still
hold on to the belief that these generalizations exist, that we just lack the
theoretical tools to state them, and they are optimistic that the formula-

background image

14

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

tion of such a theory cannot be too far away. In this light, the results of
this study are surprising since they lead to the conclusion that no gener-
alization can capture the entirety of the phenomenon. As will be demon-
strated, this failure to generalize over the data is brought about by the
fact that the position of the pronoun in Later Medieval Greek depends on
the surface characteristics of the element that immediately precedes the
verb and not the structural ones. In fact, many of the elements that affect
the placement of the pronoun in a similar way do not fall under any natu-
ral classification, while others, which would be expected to pattern in the
same way, do not. Consequently, this study joins a number of other re-
search efforts that have arrived at less-than-perfect generalizations and
explores the alternative ways in which we can model linguistic knowl-
edge.

background image

Introduction

15

1

When the noun is preceded by an adjective the pronoun may appear after the

adjective and before the noun, a pattern that Horrocks views as a residual of the

‘clitic second’ tendency transferred to the phrasal level.

2

Mackridge’s exact formulation is ‘when the verb-phrase is preceded by an

object with the same referent as that of the clitic pronoun (i.e., when the pronoun

is resumptive or doubling).’ However, by verb-phrase Mackridge means the

verb-pronoun complex, not the VP of generative syntax. It must also be noted

that, according to Haberland and van der Auwera (1993) doubling and

resumptive clitics (pronouns) are not the same.

They reserve the term

resumptive for those pronouns that appear in relativized clauses representing the

antecedent, and use the term doubling for the situation that Mackridge describes.

3

The velar stop is retained only before vowels and becomes a velar fricative if in

Classical Greek the following vowel was aspirated. Later Medieval Greek

orthography still reflects this change (

ουηχ ευΗριςσκω τον, Achilleid 1616)

even though the vowels are no longer aspirated.

4

For the loss of the infinitive see Joseph (1983a), while for the future and perfect

periphrases see Horrocks (1997) and Joseph and Pappas (2002).

background image

15

2

Methodology

This chapter deals with methodological issues concerning the investiga-
tion of morphosyntactic phenomena when the only available evidence
comes to us from texts. It is argued that even though such data constitute
observations of performance, a statistical analysis can allow one to de-
duce linguistic competence from them if a truly representative sample of
texts is used. At the same time, it is cautioned that in cases such as this
where the body of texts has been arbitrarily culled by the passage of
time, one should pay careful attention even to singular occurrences. An-
other major methodological concern addressed in this chapter is whether
a variationist analysis of morphosyntactic phenomena is even possible as
well as the definition of the particular linguistic variable that is investi-
gated in this study. A detailed list of the works used to complile the da-
tabase is also presented.

Linguistic usage vs. linguistic competence

Analyzing syntactic phenomena of an earlier stage of a language where
the evidence comes overwhelmingly from documents such as texts pre-
sents a challenge that should not be underestimated. Texts give us exam-
ples of language usage, whereas the prevalent way of analyzing syntactic
structures is to test our intuitions (Chomsky 1965, 1995), especially
about ungrammatical sentences, which are rarely available from texts in
the typical case. It would seem then impossible to work in historical
syntax within the generative framework, without finding some method
of deducing competence by observing performance. Sometimes we are
helped in this respect by grammarians of the period who describe the

background image

16

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

language of their time. For example, Browning (1983: 52-53) reports on
the observations of the Atticist lexicographer Phrynichus and his com-
ments on the ‘errors’ of his students who were using Koiné constructions
and vocabulary instead of the Classical ones. In Early Modern Greek we
even find grammars of the spoken language (e.g., the 1555 grammar of
Sofianos (Papadopoulos 1977), or the early 17

th

century grammars of

Germano (1622, in Pernot 1907) and Portius (1638, in Meyer 1889)).
However, there are no contemporary descriptions of the Later Medieval
Greek (i.e., before 1550) stage of the language (Joseph 1978/1990: 4).
So this is a case where only the texts themselves can provide information
about the grammar.

However, deducing grammaticality judgements from textual evidence

is not an easy task. As Lightfoot (1979: 5) has stated:

Usually [in diachronic syntax] one has no knowledge of ungrammati-
cal ‘sentences’, except in the rare instances where a contemporary
grammarian may report that certain forms and constructions are not
used; one is thus in the position of a child acquiring its first language,
who hears sentences being uttered but does not know whether certain
other hypothetical sentences are not uttered because they would be
grammatically deviant in some principled way or because they have
not occurred in his experience as a function of chance.

Lightfoot offers two methodological suggestions that may help over-
come this inherent problem in the collection of data for diachronic syn-
tax. The first one is that one must examine as many texts as possible
covering as many genres as possible, in order to make sure that what we
are describing is not the grammar of a particular individual or of a single
literary style. Lightfoot’s second suggestion is that the ‘… linguist [must
be] prepared to use his own intuitions where obvious generalizations can
be made’ (1979: 6). So, even in cases where certain constructions are not
attested, we should not immediately assume that they are ungrammatical,
but question ourselves if their non-occurrence is simply a matter of
chance. One has to be very cautious since not all intuitions about earlier
stages will be the same, or even valid.

Another method to remedy the paucity of negative data in diachronic

syntax is offered by Joseph (1978/1990: 2): ‘when there is sufficient
attestation, textual evidence allows for the use of statistical counts in
making judgements of acceptability. This is especially so when a dia-

background image

Methodology

17

chronic change is reflected in a significant variation in the ratio of the
use of one surface configuration to another.’ This is precisely the goal of
variationist studies, a program spear-headed by Labov (1963) in order to
understand how sociolinguistic variation may lead to linguistic change.
The methodology of this program was originally employed in studies of
phonological and morphophonological variation, and with the work of
Kroch (1982/1989) it has also been employed in understanding syntactic
variation and syntactic change. The basic assumption as stated by Taylor
(1994: 19) is that there is ‘… a fairly direct connection between gram-
mar and usage such that the organization of the grammar is reflected in
the patterns of usage.’ The word ‘reflected’ is used because, while gen-
eral usage will follow the syntactic rules of the grammar, there will also
be tokens of usage that do not. One has to ask then, what the import of
such non-conforming examples is, especially when their relative num-
bers are small.

The first and most obvious answer would be that we do not expect the

grammars that we construct for past stages of a language to have the
same descriptive adequacy as contemporary grammars should have. Af-
ter all, we are examining data of language usage, not of linguistic intui-
tions. Thus, we can explain rare counterexamples to otherwise regular
patterns as simple slips of attention. There is also, however, a parallel
that can be drawn between diachronic and synchronic syntactic studies if
we consider the possibility that the deviations come from an area of the
grammar that is undergoing change (i.e., from postverbal placement of
the pronoun to preverbal placement). It is possible that the ongoing
change creates a certain level of uncertainty within the speaker/writer.
Linguistic intuitions of uncertainty are not uncommon in synchronic
syntax either. In fact there is ample evidence concerning ‘question
mark’

5

judgements of grammaticality where there is hesitation about the

well-formedness of a construction or maybe even disagreement about the
well-formedness of the construction between different speakers. In gen-
eral, if such counterexamples in the texts are rare and do not form sig-
nificant patterns, they can be set aside without much consideration, es-
sentially being treated as curiosities, much as ‘question mark’ construc-
tions are treated in synchronic syntax.

Admittedly, it is much harder to dismiss evidence that turns up in his-

torical research, because we are always operating under the insecurity
that there are other, linguistically non-related reasons for the rarity of a
pattern in a corpus. More important, perhaps, is the recognition that, ex-

background image

18

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

cept in rare cases, everything that occurs in a text is a possible utterance
(or construction) and thus cannot simply be dismissed. As a result, the
historical syntactician is obliged to look at every rare pattern carefully,
and to decide whether or not it should be accounted for as part of the
grammar.

Compiling a corpus of texts

The preceding remarks are general in nature, issues that would be en-
countered in any historical text-based syntactic study. However, in Later
Medieval Greek (indeed, in Medieval Greek in general) we are faced
with an additional problem: the influence of the puristic model of Greek
that many writers adopted in Post-Classical times and which has been
influential in the development of the language up until now. As Joseph
(1978/1990: 5) writes:

Because of the overwhelming cultural influence of Classical Greece,
Greek writers in Post-Classical times felt compelled to emulate the
language and style of the Classical writers. This Atticizing drive re-
sulted in the writing of many Medieval texts, in what can be called a
‘learned’ style, which were virtually indistinguishable linguistically
from the Greek of over 1,000 years before their time.

Another potential source of influence on the language of these texts

came from the Romance languages, especially in the period after the
conquest of Constantinople by the crusaders in 1204, and the establish-
ment of Romance-speaking (mainly Italian and French) feudal lords in
Byzantine territory. This concern is mostly voiced about the language of
romantic poetry where many of the themes are common to both the Byz-
antine and the Romance literature and sometimes the origin of a poem
may be unclear. Some poems may be Romance originals translated into
Greek, raising the possibility that the language of these texts may not
represent the spoken Greek of the time but, instead, show the effects of
translation syntax.

It can also be problematic to place these texts chronologically. As I

assume is usually the case with textual evidence from the Middle Ages,
there exist several manuscript traditions and it is not always easy to as-
certain which manuscript is closest to the original writing or the oral
tradition upon which a particular work is based. This problem is com-
pounded by the liberal copying process (so say both Browning 1983 and

background image

Methodology

19

Horrocks 1997) of Byzantine scribes who felt free to ‘emend’ the texts
they were copying according to their idiom. Joseph (2000) discusses a
similar problem concerning three infinitival constructions of Medieval
Greek, the circumstantial infinitive, the periphrastic future with

θéλω

/thElo/ ‘I want’ plus infinitive and the periphrastic perfect with

/χω /Exo/

‘I have’ plus infinitive whose authenticity has been questioned by other
scholars. Even though Joseph shows that these constructions have a
‘good claim to legitimacy and authenticity’, it is evident that painstaking
philological work is required in the collection of data from Medieval
Greek in order to determine which texts can be used as valid representa-
tives of the spoken language of the time. In the following list, I provide
the names of the texts that make up the database as well as the relevant
philological information about them that I have compiled from the works
of Beck (1993), Browning (1983) and Horrocks (1997), and Joseph
(1978/1990); the texts are listed in the rough chronological order of their
(estimated) composition. If a text was not included in the database in its
entirety, the specific lines examined are indicated in parentheses.

Basileios Digene¤s Akrite¤s. This is an epic poem recounting the ex-

ploits of the hero Digene¤s, a legendary border defender (Akrite¤s) of the
empire, who was the son of a Muslim father and a Christian mother (Di-
gene¤s: Two-race). Despite the fact that many aspects of the manuscript
traditions are disputed, and although there are several questions about
the ultimate source and language of the original, it is generally acknowl-
edged that the language of the manuscript of Escorial (15

th

century) is

the earliest extended version of spoken Greek of the Later Medieval pe-
riod which reflects linguistic behavior of the 11

th

or even 10

th

century.

Pto¤khoprodromika. A collection of satirical poems written in the 12

th

century, attributed to Theo¤doros Prodromos, a member of the Byzantine
court who had fallen out of favor (Pto¤kho/prodromos: Poor/Prodromos).
Not everyone agrees that all the poems have the same author. There is
also some dispute as to whether another short poem, entitled Philosophia
tou Krasopatero
s, which displays many more modern characteristics
should be included in this collection. I did not include it in the database.

Spaneas. Another poem of disputed authorship, in which a father ad-

vises his son. Most scholars agree that it comes to us from the 12

th

or

13

th

century.

Glykas. Best known for his Chronography, a work in the Koiné,

Glykas also penned this largely vernacular poem while he was impris-
oned by the emperor Manuel 1

st

(13

th

century).

background image

20

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Poulologos. An allegorical poem of unknown authorship about a feast

of different birds. Most scholars agree that it has its origin in a Byzantine
court (perhaps from Nicæa) and that it was composed in the 14

th

century.

Physiologos. A list of animal descriptions written in the 14

th

century

based on a much larger work written in the Hellenistic Koiné.

Die¤ge¤sis Paidiophrastos (lines 1-121, 740-1082). Another poem with

anthropomorphized animals belonging to the middle of the 14

th

century.

Die¤ge¤sis Akhilleo¤s (Achilleid). Despite the name of its protagonist

(Achilles) this work has nothing in common with the Iliad. Rather, its
motif is the same as all other romances of the period. It was probably
penned in the 14

th

century.

Khronikon tou Moreo¤s (lines 125-1625). A chronicle of the conquest

of the Moreas province of the Peloponnese by the Franks, attributed to a
Greek-speaking descendant of the conquerors. The version used in this
study was composed, according to most scholars, near the end of the 13

th

century.

Historia tou Belisariou. A poem based on the exploits of a real-life

Byzantine general. Several different manuscripts of this work exist; I
chose the version given by Bakker and Gemert (1988) as the closest ap-
proximation to the original, which was probably composed at the begin-
ning of the 15

th

century.

Kallimakhos kai Khrysorrhoe¤ (lines 500-2000). A story in the chival-

ric tradition written in verse sometime in the 14

th

century. Although it

bears similarities with similar works from the western romance tradition,
scholars are certain that this poem has its ultimate origin in the Byzan-
tine literary tradition.

Lybistros kai Rhodamne¤, (lines 1500-3000). Similar to Kallimakhos

kai Khrysorrhoe¤.

Die¤ge¤sis Apollo¤niou. This romantic poem is also considered a work of

the 14

th

century. Unlike the two previous ones, however, it is thought to

be a translation of an Italian poem that occurred in the western territories
of the Byzantine empire.

Phlo¤rios kai Platziaphlore¤ (lines 1-1500). A romantic poem similar to

Die¤ge¤sis Apollo¤niou.

Thre¤nos Ko¤nstantinoupoleo¤s. An elegy for the fall of Constantinople

composed around that time (1453) by someone living in the area.

Thanatikon te¤s Rhodou. Another elegy recounting the plague that be-

fell the city of Rhodes (1499), composed by native poet Geo¤rgillas
shortly after the event of the plague.

background image

Methodology

21

Katalogia. A collection of love songs from the island of Rhodes, com-

piled before the end of the 15

th

century.

Phalieros. The database includes two works of Marinos Phalieros, a

member of the Venetian aristocracy of Crete. They were both written
during the first half of the 15

th

century.

Spanos. A quite explicit satire of a Byzantine liturgy, composed during

the 15

th

century. It contains many archaistic elements (which is to be

expected because the ecclesiastical language was highly stylized),
though the pronoun placement patterns seem unaffected.

Homilia tou nekrou basilia. A poem in the long tradition of encounters

with the dead, Homilia recounts the story of a traveler happening upon
the remains of a dead king, who warns him about the ephemeral nature
of earthly gains. It was probably composed in Crete during the second
half of the 15

th

century.

Apokopos. Another Cretan poem of the 15

th

century recounting a trip

to Hades. This time, though, the reader is advised to enjoy life, for the
dead are quickly forgotten.

Khroniko to¤n Tokko¤n (lines 500-2000). A chronicle about the Tocco

dynasty of Epeiros (North-Western Greece). Written around the begin-
ning of the 15

th

century.

Die¤ge¤sis tou Aleksandrou (lines 1-1500). The rhymed version of the

Alexander poem, most likely composed in Zakynthos around the begin-
ning of the 16

th

century.

Depharanas, Tribo¤le¤s, Gioustos. All three of these poets are from the

Heptanese region (Gioustos and Depharanas from Zakynthos, Tribo¤le¤s
from Kerkyra (Corfu)). Their works date from the beginning of the 16

th

century.

Aito¤los. This is a poem from the middle of the 16

th

century, which

does not have a title. According to Bànescu the poem’s composer hails
from Aito¤lia (western Greece), hence the listing.

These are the texts that constitute the database for investigating the

position of weak pronouns in Later Medieval Greek and Early Modern
Greek. It is obvious that these texts span a rather large time period
(roughly five centuries) and were written in different parts of the empire.
Most scholars, however, maintain that there is little differentiation within
this body of texts, either according to time-period, or by geographical
origin (cf. Browing 1983, Joseph 1978/1990, Mackridge 1993, Horrocks
1997), especially with respect to the variation of pronoun placement.
More significantly, the information about the authors of these texts is

background image

22

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

lacking in most cases, and one has to rely on the educated guesses of
philologists. For these reasons the investigation will begin by treating the
texts as a single group, as Mackridge himself did.

Unlike Mackridge, however, I have not included the Cypriot texts in

this group, for, as I will show in Chapter 5, they differ substantially from
the other texts with respect to pronoun placement. There are three Cyp-
riot texts from this period. The first one, Assizes, is a text of laws of the
period, written in the middle of the 14

th

century. The other two,

Ekse¤ge¤sis te¤s Glykeias Kho¤ras Kyprou by Makhairas (15

th

century, sec-

tions 175-200 and 300-330), and Die¤ge¤sis Khronikas Kyprou by Bous-
tro¤nios (16

th

century, sections 75-135), are chronicles of events taking

place in Cyprus. Boustro¤nios’ chronicle starts where Makhairas’ ends.
Unfortunately, I only had access to small excerpts from the Assizes, so
only the two chronicles were consulted in this study.

For the examination of weak object pronoun placement in Early Mod-

ern Greek, the standard editions of the poems of Ero¤phile¤ (lines 1-500),
Thysia tou Abraam (lines 1-500), and Boskopoula were chosen from the
works of the Cretan Rennaisance (17

th

century). In addition, prose texts

from Baleta’s Anthologia te¤s De¤motike¤s Pezografias (Anthology of de-
motic prose, pages 113-256), also written in the 17

th

century, were ex-

amined. These excerpts are usually two or three pages long, and come
from various places of the then Greek-speaking world. Finally, for Early
and Middle Medieval Greek, Maas’ (1912) compilation of the rhythmi-
cal Acclamations were consulted, since they are believed to be the clos-
est approximation of the vernacular of the time (6

th

-10

th

century).

Accountability and the linguistic variable in morphosyntactic
research

The variationist framework employed in this research was developed as
an answer to the problem that the fact of language change over time
posed for structural linguistics. Since it is assumed that in order for lan-
guage to function efficiently it has to have structure, and that structure is
equated with homogeneity, the breakdown of structural homogeneity
implicit in language change posed the following question: How can lan-
guages remain viable while changing? A solution is provided by the
‘variationist’ approach.

… native-like command of heterogeneous structures is not a matter

of multidialectalism or ‘mere’ performance, but is part of unilingual

background image

Methodology

23

linguistic competence … in a language serving a complex (i.e., real)
community, it is absence of structured heterogeneity that would be
dysfunctional. (Weinreich, Labov, Herzog 1968: 101)

If language is in essence a heterogeneous system then it is not sur-

prising at all that it remains functional while changing. In the past 40
years or so, a great amount of work has been invested into showing that
there is structure to the variation that is observed in language, and de-
termining both the linguistic (from within the linguistic system) and the
non-linguistic (from the social environment) parameters that impose this
structure.

The quantitative analysis of variation is built around the notion of a

‘variable’, namely the linguistic unit for which there exists variation.
The concrete manifestations of the ‘variable’ that comprise the whole of
the variation are the ‘variants’. Thus, in this study, the ‘variable’ is weak
object pronoun placement
, whose ‘variants’ are preverbal and postver-
bal
. Successful variationist studies depend crucially on the correct iden-
tification of the ‘variable’ and ‘variants’, and finally on ‘accountability’,
‘… perhaps the single most important methodological maxim for studies
of variability’ (Winford 1990: 227). What the principle of ‘accountabil-
ity’ requires is that the researcher must be true to his or her data set,
should include all variants of a variable, and when excluding a variant
must make this information explicit and give a reason for the exclusion.
Thus, future researchers should be able to get the same results if they
follow the steps of a previous study. In Labov’s (1982: 30) words: ‘all
occurrences of a given variant are noted, and where it has been possible
to define the variables as a closed set of variants, all non-occurrences in
the relevant environments.’ This statement, originally designed for the
investigation of morpho-phonological variation, has faced some criti-
cism as regards syntactic variation.

The (closed set of) variants of a particular variable were originally

construed as different ways of saying the same thing, and it is not self-
evident that this can hold for syntactic variation the same way that it
holds for phonological (or morphophonological) variation. As Romaine
(1984: 410-411) observes:

The differing semiotic functions of phonological as opposed to syn-
tactic units are at issue here. The distinction between phonology and
syntax depends upon the acknowledged properties of duality and ar-

background image

24

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

bitrariness. The success of Labovian methods in dealing with
phonological variation can be attributed to the arbitrariness of the lin-
guistic sign … The relationship between the rules of syntax and the
meaning of a sentence is by contrast iconic … In other words, we
might say that sentences are not produced as unanalyzed holons, but
are interpreted anew on each occasion.

As an example of Romaine’s point, consider the following. In the Ar-

vaniti and Joseph (1999, 2000) investigation of voiced stop prenasaliza-
tion in Modern Greek, there is no question that the prenasalized voiced
stop and plain voiced stop variants have the same referent (e.g., both
/ambEli/ and /abEli/ signify ‘vineyard’). The situation however is not so
clear with variation at the syntactic level. For instance in Modern Greek
both

θá /Ta/ plus Past tense and πρéπει νá /prEpi na/ plus Past tense

carry the meaning of ‘inferred certainty’ (Mackridge 1985—cf. example
1 and 2). The question, however, of whether these two constructions are
completely synonymous, and can therefore be used interchangeably, as
/ambEli/ and /abEli/ could be used, cannot be answered without investi-
gating the semantic nuances of each construction. Furthermore, they
each might have different syntactic properties.

(1)

;Ο Μáκης

/φερε

μπáλα.

o makis

EfErE

bala

the Makis–Nom sg

bring–3sg Past

ball–DOsg

Θà

το

μíλησε

1 Γιργος.

Ta

tu

milisE

o jorVos

marker

he–IOsg WP

speak–3sg Past

the Giorgos–Nom sg

‘Makis brought a ball. Giorgos must have spoken to him’

(2)

;Ο Μáκης

/φερε

μπáλα.

o makis

EfErE

bala

the Makis–Nom sg

bring–3sg Past

ball–DOsg

Πρéπει

νà

το μíλησε

1 Γιργος.

prEpi

na

tu milisE

o jorVos

It must be

Subj Marker

to him spoke

the Giorgos–Nom sg

‘Makis brought a ball. It must be that Giorgos spoke to him’

In order to resolve some of this controversy Romaine (1984: 419) dis-

tinguishes between four different types of variables.

background image

Methodology

25

1.

‘Pure’ phonological

2.

Morphophonemic

3.

Morphosyntactic or morpholexical

4.

‘Pure’ syntactic

According to Romaine, the first type of variant is conditioned by

phonological factors as well as social and stylistic ones and is exempli-
fied by postvocalic r deletion in American English. The second and third
types are conditioned by phonological and grammatical factors as well
as social and stylistic ones and are exemplified by the t/d deletion in
English and the deletion of the complementizer que in Montreal French
respectively. The fourth type, however, is conditioned by syntactic fac-
tors but not by any social or stylistic ones. The agentless passive of Eng-
lish is the example Romaine uses for this last type. The distinction be-
tween types three and four (which have direct relevance to this research)
is not only based, according to Romaine, on the fact that type three is
affected by social/stylistic factors while type four is not. Another key
difference between the two is that morphosyntactical variation concerns
the presence or absence of a particular element, while for syntactic
variation ‘a whole construction or arrangement of items which alternates
is required.’

Similar doubts about the extension of the methodology have also been

voiced by Harris (1984), Winford (1984), and Cheshire (1987). They
acknowledge that a large part of the problem lies in the inherent diffi-
culty of defining the concept ‘syntactic variable’ in a precise way. In
fact, Winford (1984) points out that Romaine herself had been inconsis-
tent with the use of her own definition. Despite these hesitations the
three also agree that the concept of the linguistic variable can be ex-
tended as an analytical tool beyond the phonological level. Cheshire
(1987), however, believes that the extension cannot go further than the
morphosyntactic level, while Winford (1993: 142) cautions that ‘estab-
lishing semantic equivalence in such situations requires great care.’ The
article itself is an example of such an approach as Winford painstakingly
identifies the different strategies that Trinidad Creole employs in the
expression of the meanings associated with perfect have in Standard
English.

The weak pronouns that are under investigation here appear to be the

inhabitants par excellence of morphosyntactic space. However, they do
not absolutely conform with Romaine’s conditions, as these are stated
above. First, it is not likely that any information will be obtained about

background image

26

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

the stylistic/social factors that affect the variation, but this is due to the
fact that very little of a concrete nature is known about the authors of the
texts and the intended audiences. Second, we are not dealing with ‘the
presence or absence of a particular element’ as Romaine’s classification
requires, yet the variation between preverbal and postverbal placement
of the weak pronoun is too specific to be classified as a case of variation
where ‘a whole construction or arrangement of items which alternates is
required.’ It is safe to assume that this is indeed a case of morphosyntac-
tic variation and that the methodology employed for phonological vari-
ables can safely be extended to the analysis of Later Medieval Greek
weak object pronoun as well.

Defining the set of variants

An additional problem concerning the issue of ‘accountability’ is that of
defining the ‘set of variants’. Many times, there are tokens which on the
surface seem to belong to the ‘set of variants’ and therefore to the scope
of the study, when in fact they do not. The task of distinguishing these
tokens is not always an easy or uncontroversial task (see for example
Blake 1997, Winford 1992, Rickford et al. 1991 on the correct set of
variants for the AAVE copula, and Pappas 1999, 2001 on Early Modern
Greek counterfactuals). In the present study of Later Medieval Greek,
there are three instances of forms, which are homophonous with the
weak pronouns forms and may in some circumstances be mistaken for
verbal arguments.

The possessive pronouns (also weak)

Some of the genitive forms of the pronouns (

μο, σο, το, τς, το,

μς, σς, τν/τοúς) also function as possessive pronouns as well (i.e.,
‘my’, ‘your’, ‘his’, ‘her’, ‘its’, ‘our’, ‘your’, ‘their’). By Later Medieval
Greek these forms are fixed in post-noun position (Horrocks 1990: 48),
and could be interpreted as a verbal argument if they also precede the
verb, as in example (3).

(3)

ε8ς

τ7ν βουλν

του

σéβη

is

tin vulin

tu

EsEvi

to

the will–Acc sg

he–PS sg

respect–3sg Past

‘He respected his will’ (Moreas, 201)

In theory,

του could be either a verbal argument of σéβη or a posses-

sive pronoun attached to

βουλν. The answer is given by the fact that it

background image

Methodology

27

does not bear an accent.

6

For, if we compare this example with example

(4), which can be found in the same text a few lines above example (3),
we see that in this case it is accented (

το).

7

Thus, the pronoun

του in

example (3) can only be a possessive pronoun.

(4)

1

κóντος

το

/δωκε

o

kondos

tu

EDokE

the–Nom sg

count–Nom sg

he–IOsg WP

give–3sg Past

‘The count gave him’ (Moreas, 181)

In addition to this distinction, it has also been the case that since the

invention of the accent marks in the 3

rd

century BC, a proparoxytone

word (word with the accent three syllables from the end) that is followed
by an enclitic is written with a second accent on its final syllable (Smyth
1956: §183c, Kaisse 1982, Warburton 1970). This accenting convention
can also be found in the LMG texts, where in some cases it helps
distinguish possessive pronouns from object pronouns (example 5). The
added accent on

δáσκαλον indicates that the pronoun is enclitic to it.

(5)

τòν δáσκαλóν

της

λéγει

ton Daskalon

tis

lEVi

the teacher–IOsg

she–PSsg

say–3sg Pres

‘She tells her teacher’ (Apollo¤nios, 334)

The definite article and relative pronouns

The third person forms or the weak pronouns (

τóν, τν, τó, το, τς,

το, τοúς, τíς/τéς/τáς, τá, τν/τοúς) are also homophonous with the
accusative (singular or plural) or genitive (singular or plural) forms of
the definite article. In addition, the accusative forms also functioned as
relative pronouns in Later Medieval Greek (Horrocks 1997). There is, of
course, no confusion between the weak pronouns and the forms of the
definite article that are homophonous to them, because as was mentioned
above a weak pronoun that is placed after the head that selects it loses its
accent marking. On the other hand, a definite article that precedes a noun
or adjective does not lose its accent. This distinction is illustrated in ex-
ample (6).

(6)

Τρéμουν

τòν Βελισáριον

trEmun

ton vElisarion

fear–3pl Pres

the Belisarius–DOsg

background image

28

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

καì

%γαπον

τον

kE

aVapun

ton

and

love–3pl Pres

he–DOsg WP

‘They fear Belisarius…and (also) love him’ (Belisarios, 203)

There is, however, potential for confusion between the weak object

pronouns and the relative pronouns, as they can both appear before verbs
and be written with an accent. Consider, for example, the following
sentence from Poulologos (267):

(7)

τ7ν Dχλησιν

τ7ν

ε'χαν

tin oxlisin

tin

ixan

the commotion–DOsg

???

have–3pl Past

The question here is whether the second

τν is a personal pronoun or a

relative pronoun. Is the correct translation of the phrase ‘the commotion,
they had it’ or ‘the commotion which they had’?

This question, in fact, seems to have been the impetus for Mackridge’s

(1993) work concerning the problem of weak pronoun placement in Me-
dieval Greek. Mackridge accurately shows that in phrases such as the
one above, the second

τν can only be a relative pronoun, which makes

the translation ‘the commotion which they had’ the only possible one. A
personal pronoun, which is coreferent with a full noun-phrase (otherwise
known as a ‘doubling pronoun’—Mackridge 1993) would have to appear
postverbally as in example (8), or (9).

(8)

τ7ν γνẃμην

/χεις

την

tin Vnomin

Exis

tin

the opinion–DOsg

have–2sg Present

she–DOsg

‘The opinion, you have it’ (Poulologos, 571)

A final comment regarding ‘accountability’ concerns the length of the

texts used. Ideally, each text would contribute the same number of to-
kens to the database so that they would all have the same weight in the
statistical study. However, given the differences in the length of texts
and the fact that the frequency of certain constructions differs according
to the genre of writing—for example paraenetic poems have a prepon-
derance of imperative forms—this would be impossible to achieve with-
out limiting the breadth of the research. Admittedly, this is a slight dis-

background image

Methodology

29

advantage for the statistical analysis but it does not compromise it in any
serious way.

Conclusion

These, then, are the major issues concerning the use of textual evidence
in studies of diachronic syntax. It was established that the use of the
variationist methodology in conjunction with the careful analysis of ex-
ceptions allows us to deduce linguistic competence from patterns of lan-
guage usage. In addition, I have set clear guidelines for the identification
of appropriate tokens to be used in this study of variation in the place-
ment of weak object pronouns in Later Medieval Greek. Further details
about specific tokens will be presented along with the analysis of the
major patterns.

Based on this methodological approach, the next chapter presents a

detailed account of the variation in weak object pronoun placement in
Later Medieval Greek texts. In order to facilitate the comparison with
Mackridge’s account, the analysis follows the general frame of his de-
scription, either confirming or disproving his observations.

background image

30

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

5

Syntacticians traditionally mark ungrammatical sentences with a star

(*), whereas generally accepted sentences are left unmarked. When they
want to indicate that they are not sure what the grammaticality status of
the sentence is, they mark with a question mark (?) or several of them
(???) reflecting several degrees of ‘uneasiness’ with a sentence. Hence
the term ‘question mark’ judgement.

6

This, of course, is based on the assumption that written accents, as they

appear in the manuscripts, can be taken at face value in interpreting
examples. According to Holton (electronic communication–via
Mackridge, 11/27/00) “accents are clearly written, at least in the best
manuscripts … enclitics are often joined to the preceding noun or verb
… [and] there are very few ambiguities of the type [mentioned here]”.
Since I am not examining the manuscripts themselves, however, I
remain cautious about what can be inferred from the presence or absence
of the accents.

7

These accents are not indications of stress, just orthographic indicators

about the status of the pronoun. Similarly, in Standard Modern Greek
monosyllabic verbs are not written with an accent, even though they are
accented phonologically e.g.,

θα τον δω ‘I will see him’ not θα τον

δως (phonetically [QA ton ·Do]).

background image

30

3

Data Analysis

In this chapter, I present the results of tabulating preverbal and postver-
bal tokens of pronoun placement according to Mackridge’s classifica-
tion, and compare them to his description. After the more transparent
discrepancies are settled, I turn my attention to the groups of elements
that Mackridge identified, in order to determine whether the cohesive-
ness of the groups and their relevant ranking are confirmed by the statis-
tical analysis. Finally, I make an initial attempt to uncover the unifying
characteristic for each group of elements, and in doing so I present a new
description of the variation that is based on the existence of seven fac-
tors: clause-initial position,

ο ‘not’, reduplicated object, subject, tem-

poral expression, fronted constituent, and function word.

Raw data

By tabulating the postverbal and preverbal placement of pronouns ac-
cording to Mackridge’s classification, I am accepting both his and
Rollo’s view that the variation depends crucially on the nature of the
immediately preceding element, but I am not doing so blindly. As will
become clear in the discussion of the data below, other possible reasons
for the variation (e.g., type of clause) can be easily dismissed. I choose
Mackridge’s system over that of Rollo simply because the former is
much more elaborate and refined than the latter. As was done there, I
present the results for finite, non-imperative verb-forms first, namely the
indicative and the subjunctive. The pattern of pronoun placement associ-
ated with other verb-forms will be examined in Chapter 4.

background image

Data Analysis

31

As Table 3.1 demonstrates, the results of this study concur with those

of Mackridge in most cases, and prove that Rollo overestimated the im-
portance of postverbal pronoun placement in a number of environments,
most notably those of a preceding subject and adverb. The reasons for
this will be examined later. In any case, for the rest of the chapter, I fo-
cus on the results of Mackridge only. The first discrepancy that is evi-
dent concerns the causal conjunction

διóτι. While Mackridge asserts

that when

διóτι precedes the verb, the pronoun appears postverbally, the

results of this study show that pronoun placement is preverbal as can be
seen in example (1). The number of tokens for

διóτι is extremely small

(only four), but Mackridge does not provide us with the number of his
tokens, which would facilitate the comparison between the two results.
One possible explanation for it may be the fact that Mackridge’s relevant
example comes from the Chronicle of Makhairas (14

th

century), which

was written in Cypriot Greek. As will be demonstrated later in Chapter
5, there is a significant difference between Cypriot Greek and the Greek
of other areas with respect to pronoun placement.

(1)

διóτις

τòν

πλáκωσε

Diotis

ton

EplakosE

because

he–DOsg WP

crushed–3sg Past

‘Because it crushed him’ (Rimada, 351)

In the case of

ε8, the facts are slightly more complicated. In six of the

seven tokens that were found in this database,

ε8 never immediately pre-

cedes the verb–pronoun complex, because the contrastive particle

δé /DE/

is interpolated. Mackridge’s findings seem to confirm this observation.
Of his three

ε8 examples, two of them (his 5 and 16a) actually have ε8 δé

while the third (his 6) has

ε8 δè καí (example 5 is repeated here as 2).

The only token where

ε8 immediately precedes the complex is Phlo¤rios,

ln. 1013 (example 3). Mackridge does notice this interpolation but he
claims that it does not affect the rule (1993: 328 fn.1). However, this
statement is inconsistent with his previous claim that pronoun placement
is determined by the nature of the immediately preceding element.

(2)

ε8 δè

συμβε

σε

i DE

simvi

sE

if though

happen–3sg Pres

you–IOsg WP

‘If though it happens to you …’ (Spaneas, 209)

background image

32

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(3)

ε8

χáνεις

με

i

xanis

mE

if

lose–2sg Pres

I–DOsg WP

‘If you lose me’ (Phlo¤rios, 1013)

Furthermore, a closer investigation of the role that

δé plays in the pat-

tern of variation shows that when the complex is immediately preceded
by

δé the pronoun is placed postverbally 11 times and preverbally 3

times. More importantly, there is one token in which

δé follows the con-

ditional conjunction

áν, and the pronoun appears postverbally (cf. ex-

ample 4), even though, as Mackridge himself points out,

áν is associ-

ated with preverbal pronoun placement. Thus, Mackridge’s statement
that

δé does not affect the placement of the pronoun cannot be correct.

For this reason, I have removed

ε8 from the list of possible factors and

replaced it with

δé.

(4)

àν δè

εEρουν

με

Ean DE

Evrun

mE

if though

find–3pl Pres

me–DOsg WP

‘If, though, they find me’ (Achilleid, 178)

Another discrepancy between Mackridge’s findings and the results of

this investigation is observed in the environment of reduplicated objects.
In the database used in this study, there are a large number of preverbal
tokens, whereas Mackridge claims that in such constructions postverbal
pronoun placement is almost categorical. Thus, although examples such
as (5) are common, one also finds several constructions such as example
(6), where the pronoun is placed preverbally.

(5)

τò συμβàν

θρνει

το

to simvan

ETrini

to

the event–DOsg

mourn–3sg Imperfect

it–DOsg WP

‘He mourned about the event’ (Phlo¤rios, 732)

(6)

κF μèν

μF

λéγχει

k EmEn

m

ElENxi

and me–DOsg

me–DOsg WP

criticize–3sg Pres

‘And even me he criticizes’ (Phlo¤rios, 404)

background image

Data Analysis

33

Table 3.1

Results of token count based on Mackridge’s classification

Preceding element

Pre V

Post V

Initial

59

719

Coordinating conjunctions

58

681

τι ‘that’

8

30

διóτι ‘because’

4

0

δé (interjection)

3

11

ο(κ) ‘not’

0

56

Reduplicated object

39

79

να ‘to, in order to’

24

0

νá ‘to, in order to’

1436

3

ς ‘let’

65

1

μ, μηδé, οδéν, δéν ‘not’

431

3

Interrogative pronouns

164

2

πẃς ‘that’

3

0

Temporal and Comparative conjunctions

237

1

ν, áν ‘if’

84

1

Relative pronouns

275

3

Object

357

21

Adverb (non-temporal)

295

33

Prepositional Phrase

246

36

Subject

334

130

Temporal expression

86

63

Results of pronoun placement by text can be found in the Appendix, Tables 1-8.

There are no obvious cases of disagreement between the two studies as

far as the other environments identified by Mackridge are concerned. It
should be noted, however, that in this study, subject complements that
appear before the verb are not treated as semantically emphasized ele-
ments, as this position is considered canonical (cf. examples 7 and 8).
Moreover, I have added preverbal prepositional phrases to this subgroup,
a category of constituents that Mackridge does not mention. Finally,
while Mackridge only discusses the temporal adverbs

πáλιν /palin/

‘again’,

τóτε /totE/ ‘then’, πáντα /panda/ ‘always’ and εθúς /EfTis/

‘immediately’, in this study, the category has been expanded to include
all temporal expressions, as can be seen in examples (9) and (10).

background image

34

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(7)

κενος

τò

πεδéχτηκεν

Ekinos

to

EpEDExtikEn

he–Nom sg

it–DOsg WP

accept–3sg Past

‘He accepted it’ (Achilleid, 1192)

(8)

1 φóβος

ξυπν

σε

o fovos

Eksipna

sE

the fear–Nom sg

awaken–3sg Pres

you–DOsg WP

‘Fear awakens you’ (Glykas, 170)

(9)

συντóμως φéρνουσí

τον

sindomos

fErnusi

ton

soon

bring–3pl Pres

he–DOsg WP

‘Soon they bring him’ (Belisarios, 344)

(10)

τρες μνας

τò

%νατáσσεται

tris minas

to

anatasEtE

three months

it–DOsg WP

assail–2sg Pres

‘For three months he has been assailing it’ (Achilleid, 397)

Data analysis

The question of whether or not Mackridge’s grouping of different envi-
ronments under the relevant headings is correct is, obviously, a more
complex matter and requires the use of statistical techniques. The groups
that concern us are the following:

• Group 1, in which those elements that are associated with postverbal

placement (phrase initial verb, coordinating conjunctions, redupli-
cated objects and

τι and ο) are grouped together.

• Group 2, in which a fronted non-temporal adverb, object, object

complement and prepositional phrase are grouped together.

• Group 3, in which a large number of particles, pronouns and con-

junctions are grouped together.

In order for these groupings to be validated two requirements need to

be satisfied. First, the statistical analysis has to show that the variation
between the factors that are grouped together is not significantly differ-

background image

Data Analysis

35

ent, and, second, the factors should all belong to a category that would
justify their similar behavior.

Statistical analyses do not actually have the power to prove that two

elements have the same effect on a certain pattern, in this case pronoun
placement. Instead, the analysis shows whether or not the elements un-
der investigation have effects that are significantly different. If not, then
the effects are assumed to be the same, until someone can prove other-
wise. In this study, the JMP 3.2.1 software for Macintosh was used to
perform OneWay Anova tests on the effect of the various elements.
Since the number of observations varies greatly from text to text, the
only way the Anova can be successfully carried out is if these observa-
tions are transformed into scores that show normal distribution and have
constant variance. To do this, I calculated the percentage that each one
of these observations (e.g., number of postverbal tokens) constitutes over
the total amount (number of postverbal tokens + number of preverbal
tokens) and then took the arcsin value of that percentage value. This is
standard practice in statistics, and the transformation is known as the
arcsin transformation (Woods et al. 1983: 220). In this fashion a score
ranging from 0 to 1.57... was entered for each text; if a particular con-
struction did not occur at all in the text, that entry was left blank.

In the following graphs, the x axis lists the preceding elements which

are compared while the y axis runs from 0 to 1.57 (the arcsin score). The
dark squares represent where a particular text scores; larger squares indi-
cate a score for multiple texts. According to the JMP software manual,
the diamond-shaped figures are a schematic representation of the mean
and standard error for each sample group. The line across the diamond
represent the mean of the sample group, while the height of the diamond
represents the 95% interval of confidence for each group. The circles to
the right of the graphs are schematics of the results of the Tukey-Kramer
tests (comparisons of all pairs); when the angle of two intersecting cir-
cles is less than ninety degrees or the circles do not intersect at all, then
the effects of the factors that they represent are significantly different.

Postverbal placement of pronoun

s

Figure 3.1 demonstrates the results of the OneWay Anova test for the
environments listed in Group 1. The only one that has a significantly
different effect on the placement of the pronoun is a preceding redupli-
cated object.

background image

36

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

postverbal

0

1

coordnating initial

oti

ou

red. obj.

preceding element

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

Figure 3.1

Postverbal pronoun placement for Group 1

But why do these four environments affect pronoun placement in the

same way? Is the similarity accidental or is it determined by a shared
characteristic? One potentially rewarding starting point to answering this
question is to examine whether they can all be subsumed under the cate-
gory ‘clause-initial’. Coordinating conjunctions are natural candidates
for inclusion in this category since they link clauses without belonging to
them, thus leaving the following verb in clause-initial position (examples
11 and 12). With respect to

τι, Mackridge mentions that it shows a

pattern of ‘incomplete subordination’, functioning more as a link be-
tween clauses rather than the first element of a subordinate clause (cf.
example 13). This position about Medieval and Early Modern Greek

τι

is also supported by Jannaris (1968: §2031-2032) who maintains that:

… very frequently the indirect discourse, though opening with the in-
finitive or by

τι (3ς) passes suddenly to the direct discourse … In

this popular mode of direct discourse, it is very common to indicate
the dependence of the verbatim clause by placing before it the con-
junction

τι, which then seems redundant … and corresponds to our

modern colon (:) …

Thus it seems reasonable that the cases in which the complex is imme-

diately preceded by the conjunction

τι can also be reduced to instances

background image

Data Analysis

37

of a clause-initial verb, and justifies the positing of an overarching fac-
tor, which is labeled, for the sake of simplicity, initial.

(11)

γρáφει γρáμματα,

στéλνει

τα

Vrafi Vramata

stElni

ta

he writes letters

send–3sg Pres

it–DOpl WP

‘He writes letters, he sends them’ (Achilleid, 403)

(12)

καì βλéπει

το

1 πραματευτς

kE

vlEpi

to

o pramatEftis

and

see–3sg Pres

it–DOsg

the itinerant salesman–Nom sg

‘And the itinerant salesman sees it’ (Poulologos, 152)

(13)

πρτον μèν

τι ε'χα

την

proton mEn

oti

ixa

tin

first

that

have–1sg Past

she–DOsg WP

‘First, that I had her’ (Digene¤s, 1597)

If it can also be shown that the cases involving

ο should be subsumed

under the category of initial, this would provide a generalization for the
entirety of Mackridge’s first rule, a much desired result. One avenue that
seems promising takes into consideration the claim by both Mackridge
(1993, 1995) and Horrocks (1990) that

ο is not an independent word

but rather a ‘proclitic’ whose phonological host is the verb (cf. Smyth
1956, Jannaris 1968: §97 and West 1982 for a similar view of

ο in

Ancient Greek). If this is true, then the placement of the pronoun when
the complex is immediately preceded by

ο may be an epiphenomenon

of the location of the entire string

ο–verb–pronoun in clause-initial

position. In particular, if in all

ο cases this compound was at the

beginning of a clause, then we could easily explain this as another
instance of the effect of factor initial. However, the actual tokens
involving

ο show a more complicated pattern. In the majority of these

(31 out of 57), the

ο–verb complex is either at the beginning of the

clause, as in example (14), or follows a coordinating conjunction.

(14)

οκ

%στοχε

το

uk

astoxi

to

not

miss–3sg Pres

it–DOsg WP

‘He does not miss it’ (Achilleid, 767)

background image

38

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

In the other cases, the

ο–verb complex is preceded by an element of

the same clause, but in most of these (18) the specific element is either a
subject (cf. example 15) or a temporal expression; in the remaining 8
examples we find either object complements or non-temporal adverbs
(cf. example 16). The absence of variation between preverbal and post-
verbal positioning among these 26 examples is puzzling. After all, if

ο

is to be considered as prosodically or syntactically attached to the verb,
then one would expect that the presence of an object or non-temporal
adverb before the

ο–verb complex would be associated with preverbal

pronoun placement. Obviously

ο cannot be easily grouped with all

other elements that have just been subsumed by the factor initial. Its
status, as well as the issue of reduplicated objects is addressed in the
next chapter, where I explore the variation in full detail.

(15)

νúκτα

ο καταπονε

τον

nikta

u

kataponi

τον

night–Nom sg

not

tire–3sg Pres

he–DOsg WP

‘Night does not tire him’ (Lybistros, 2534)

(16)

φíλον

οκ

/χουσíν

σε

filon

uk

Exusin

sE

friend–DO sg

not

have–3pl Pres

you–DOsg WP

‘They do not treat you like a friend’ (Spaneas, 18)

Preverbal placement of pronouns

Moving on to the instances of preverbal placement, Figure 3.2 clearly
demonstrates that the constituents comprising Group 2 do not show any
significant difference from each other. In addition, the members of this
group form a natural class. The canonical word order in Medieval Greek
is SVO (Mackridge 1993, Taylor 1994, Horrocks 1997), which means
that constituents such as objects, prepositional phrases, and VP adverbs
should follow the verb. The fact that they are all fronted in these con-
structions (see examples 17, 18 and 19) and the pronoun appears prever-
bally may well be the unifying feature that underlies the similarity in
pronoun placement, and justifies grouping these contexts under the fac-
tor fronted constituent. However, the hypothesis that, by being placed
before the verb, these constituents are emphasized and that it is emphasis
that is the determining factor for preverbal pronoun placement is a more
complex issue that is addressed in the next chapter.

background image

Data Analysis

39

(17)

ζημíα

μς

κáνει

zimia

mas

kani

damage–DOsg

I–IOpl WP

make–3sg Pres

‘He does damage to us’ (Tokkoi, 530)

(18)

στò σπíτι

τòν

παγαíνει

sto spiti

ton

paVEni

at the house

he–DOsg WP

go–3sg Pres

‘He leads him to the house’ (Rimada, 278)

(19)

βασιλικà

μF

%νáθρεψε

vasilika

m

anETrEpsE

royally–Adv

I–DOsg WP

raise–3sg Past

‘He brought me up royally’ (Phlo¤rios, 1024)

preverbal

0.0

0.5

1.0

1.5

adverb

object

prphrase

preceding element

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

Figure 3.2

Preverbal pronoun placement for Group 2

In the final group, we find no significant difference between its mem-

bers. This is clearly demonstrated in Figure 3.3. However, the members
of this group do not form a natural category. With respect to function,
some of these elements introduce subordinate clauses (e.g., complemen-
tizers such as

ν ‘if’), while others (e.g., the wh-phrases) introduce both

subordinate and main clauses, i.e., interrogative sentences (cf. examples

background image

40

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

20, 21 and 22). Furthermore, some elements have very distinct functions
but the same effect on pronoun placement, while others have very simi-
lar functions but a contrasting effect on pronoun placement. The nega-
tive markers exemplify this. On the one hand,

μ and δéν are very dis-

tinct from one another as the former is used in subordinate clauses, ge-
rundive constructions and prohibitives, whereas the latter is used only in
main clauses. Nevertheless, they are both associated with preverbal
placement, as can be seen in examples (23) and (24). On the other hand,
both

δéν and ο are used in the same context (main clauses), yet ο, un-

like

δéν, is associated with postverbal placement. Thus, the reason why

the elements of this group are associated with the same pattern of pro-
noun placement cannot be found in their grammatical function.

preverbal

0.8

0.9

1.0

1.1

1.2

1.3

1.4

1.5

1.6

Q.prn Relt.prn Tem.conj na-as

negation

preceding element

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

Figure 3.3

Preverbal pronoun placement for Group 3

(20)

ν

μς

φéρνουσιν

an

mas

fErnusin

if

I–IOpl WP

bring–3pl Pres

‘If they bring us’ (Gioustos, 44)

(21)

τì

τòν

θéλετε;

ti

ton

TElEtE

what

he–DOsg WP

want–2pl Pres

‘What do you want him for?’ (Apollo¤nios, 530)

background image

Data Analysis

41

(22)

τà

μè

λαλες

ta

mE

lalis

that which

me–DOsg WP

speak–2sg Pres

‘What you are saying to me’ (Digene¤s, 671)

(23)

μ7ν

τòν

πιστεúεις

min

ton

pistEvis

not

he–DOsg WP

believe–2sg Pres

‘Do not believe him’ (Glykas, 132)

(24)

οδèν

σè

ε'δα

uDEn

sE

iDa

not

you–DOsg WP

see–1sg Past

‘I did not see you’ (Moreas, 260)

On the other hand, it is not possible to argue that these elements form

a natural class based on their position in a phrase structure tree. At least
under common assumptions in GB/Minimalism, complementizers oc-
cupy the C node, wh-phrases are found in Spec CP, and markers (such as
νá and ς) head their own functional projections. Understanding why all
these elements are associated with the same pronoun pattern is a chal-
lenging task, which will be explored from different perspectives in the
following chapters. Nevertheless, the one characteristic that these words
share and which sets them apart from all other elements associated with
preverbal placement is that they only have grammatical meaning. Ac-
cordingly, and in recognition of the overwhelming preverbal pattern that
they effect, they will be considered as different facets of the factor func-
tion word
, a label that is used pretheoretically in this study.

Comparing postverbal and preverbal pronoun placement

The classification of these various elements into possible factors allows
for an across the board comparison of their effect on pronoun placement
which will prove or disprove the validity of Mackridge’s relative ranking
of environments. Figure 3.4 shows such a comparison according to the
preverbal score of each factor. The resulting rankings are different from
Mackridge’s description in two significant ways. First, there is signifi-
cant difference between factors initial (IN) and ou, on the one hand, and
reduplicated object (RO) on the other, whereas Mackridge has listed
these under the same group, labeling their effect on pronoun placement
as more or less obligatorily postverbal. In the case of the factors favoring

background image

42

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

preverbal placement the comparison shows that the effects of subject
(SB) and temporal expression (TE) are not significantly different, and
thus should not be listed separately.

preverbal

0

1

FC

FW

IN

RO

SB

TE

ou

factor

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

Figure 3.4

Preverbal pronoun placement by factor

The results of this analysis suggest that the pattern of variation for

finite verb-forms is best described by the following statements:
1.

When the verb is in clause-initial position, or preceded by the nega-
tive marker

ο, the pronoun almost always appears after the verb.

2.

When the verb is immediately preceded by a reduplicated object,
postverbal position is favored but not as categorically as above.

3.

When the verb is preceded by a subject or a temporal expression,
preverbal placement of the pronoun is slightly favored.

4.

When the verb is preceded by a fronted constituent, there is a strong
but not categorical tendency for the pronoun to appear preverbally.

5.

Finally, when the verb is preceded by a function word, preverbal
placement is near-categorical.

This alternative description, which is based on a larger and more var-

ied database than Mackridge’s and is also confirmed by a quantifica-
tional analysis of the data, is represented diagrammatically in Figure 3.5
so as to provide a clearer contrast to Mackridge’s description.

background image

Data Analysis

43

preverbal

almost preverbal

normally preverbal

free variation

postverbal

function word

fronted constituent

subject and temporal
expression

reduplicated object

initial,

ο

Figure 3.5

Diagrammatic ranking of pronoun placement (left according to

Mackridge 1993, right according to present study)

Conclusion

The results presented in this chapter demonstrate that Mackridge’s de-
scription of the variation was much closer to the actual data than Rollo’s.
Nevertheless, there are a few discrepancies, most notably the unexpected
high number of preverbal pronouns associated with a preceding redupli-
cated object, and the lack of any significant difference between factors
subject and temporal expression which have led to a new description of
the facts. These discrepancies also raise some interesting questions,
which are examined closely in the next chapter in light of the linguistic
parameters that affect variation in weak object pronoun placement.

background image

44

4

Linguistic Parameters

The previous chapter provided a detailed and coherent description of the
facts about weak object pronoun placement. In this chapter, the pattern
of variation is examined more closely in order to determine whether
there are any linguistic parameters that affect the position of pronouns in
the clause. Evidence is provided that certain lexical items, such as

ν

and

λος alter the expected pattern of variation when they appear in spe-

cific constructions, and explanations are sought for these types of inter-
actions. On the other hand, it is demonstrated that previously proposed
parameters such as emphasis or discourse constraints do not have a de-
monstrable effect on the variation. The last section presents an in-depth
analysis of weak object pronoun placement with non-finite forms—an
issue that has not been fully examined until now.

The distinction between

ο and ν ο

As was mentioned at the end of Chapter 3, it sometimes happens that the
pattern of variation associated with a specific factor can be further ana-
lyzed according to various parameters. The pattern of variation associ-
ated with the negative marker

ο is a striking example of such a case, in

which the results of the analysis may vary significantly if one is not care-
ful during the collection of tokens. In fact, the statement in Chapter 3
that ‘when

ο immediately precedes the verb complex the pronoun is

placed postverbally 57 out of 57 times’ is somewhat misleading, because
the pattern of variation is not so clear at first glance. On the contrary,

ο

is the only environment for which there is published disagreement about
the facts concerning the variation in weak pronoun placement. Horrocks

background image

Linguistic Parameters

45

(1990), while examining the placement of unstressed elements through-
out the history of Greek, wrote the following concerning

ο and weak

pronoun placement in the later medieval period:

… the clitic was naturally drawn to second position within that com-
plex, in accordance with the pattern we have seen many times al-
ready. This also tends to happen with the negative

ο, which must

similarly have been felt to ‘belong’ to the verb in a particularly close
way, both phonologically and semantically. [By ‘complex’ Horrocks
refers to the string complementizer (or negative marker)–verb, and
not the weak object pronoun–verb string as I do in this study.

]

Horrocks does not make an explicit statement about the placement of

the weak object pronoun in this context, yet one reasonable way to inter-
pret the quote above is that he is identifying

ο as one of the environ-

ments in which weak pronouns are placed in preverbal position. This is
also evident from the example that he offers:

(1)

ν ο τòν

ε8π

an

u

ton

ipo

if

not

he–IOsg WP

say–1sg Pres

‘If I do not say to him’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, III 43—Horrocks’ 28)

On the other hand, Mackridge (1993: 340), in his rule (1b) makes the

claim that ‘when the verb-phrase comes immediately after … the nega-
tive adverb

ο the order V+P is more or less obligatory’, as seen in ex-

ample (2).

(2)

οκ

/μαθéν

το

uk

EmaTEn

to

not

learn–3sg Past

it–DOsg WP

‘He did not learn it’ (Belisarios, 269)

The task of resolving the contradiction between these two statements

about the effect of the negative marker

ο is made especially difficult by

the fact that the example offered by Horrocks is precisely the type of
construction that Mackridge (1993: 329) identifies as the only instance
where the preverbal order is allowed. He claims that ‘where

ο coexists

with

ν in the same clause, the pronoun is placed before the verb.’

background image

46

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(3)

ν ο τò

πáρη

an

u

to

Epari

if

not

it–DOsg WP

take–3sg Pres

‘If he does not take it’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, IV 514—Mackridge’s 19)

This discrepancy in the literature was not noticed until Pappas (1997),

although I could not fully comment on it at the time due to the small
amount of data that I had collected. The expanded database that is avail-
able now, however, allows for a comparison between

ο and ν ο. The

results are listed in Table A.9 of the appendix, and show that when

ο

precedes the verb–pronoun complex, the pronoun is placed postverbally
57 out of 57 times, but, when

ν ο precedes the complex the pronoun is

placed preverbally 19 out of 21 times. Example (4) presents one of the
two irregular tokens. Clearly then, Mackridge’s evaluation was correct
on both points, but it should be noted in Horrocks’ defense that he was
examining the position of clitics in the entire history of Greek, and it is
quite possible that he was misled by the example he cited, or just the
tokens with

ν in general.

(4)

ν οκ ε8π

το

an

uk

ipo

to

if

not

say–1sg Pres

it–DOsg WP

‘If I do not say it’ (Poulologos, 316)

Although Mackridge’s description is correct, his explanation of the

interaction of the two constraints is hard to follow. Indeed, it is difficult
to interpret the word ‘coexists’ in any theoretically meaningful way. For
instance, does Mackridge use it to mean ‘when

ν precedes ο’ i.e., for

the case at hand? Does he also mean ‘when

ο precedes ν’?, a case that

is not of any interest to us since it is covered by Mackridge’s description
of categorically preverbal placement, as can be seen in Chapter 1.

It seems that the most reasonable interpretation of Mackridge’s state-

ment is that he assumes that when

ν and ο ‘coexist’, i.e., when they

are placed side by side before the verb–pronoun complex, there is some
type of formal conflict between the two affecting environments, which is
resolved by postulating a rule-precedence hierarchy in which the

ν con-

straint overrides the

ο constraint. Indeed, in his other article, which is

essentially the Greek version of the (1993) work, Mackridge (1995: 912)
refers to ‘conflict’ between the two constraints: ‘

Σè περιπτẃσεις που

background image

Linguistic Parameters

47

συγκροúονται οJ κανóνες (1β) καì 2 …’ [in those cases where the rule
(1b) and 2 are in conflict]. The verb

συγκροúονται literally means to

‘collide’, thus validating the interpretation I offer of his (1993) term ‘co-
exist’. Even this interpretation, however, runs into two problems.

First, both rules require that the affecting environment be immediately

before the verb-pronoun complex, so that in the

ν ο cases there really

is no formal conflict between the two rules, since

ν does not immedi-

ately precede the verb complex. Accordingly, the

ν rule cannot over-

ride the

ο rule.

The second problem is that even if the rules were to be reformulated in

order to accommodate cases like this one, there would be a problem in
the case of the negative marker

ο μ (/u mi/)

8

where the pronoun is al-

ways placed preverbally (see Table A.9 in the appendix and examples 5
and 6).

(5)

ο

μ7 σè

βαρεθ

u

mi

sE

varETo

not

you–DOsg WP

be bored–1sg Pres

‘(So that) I am not bored with you’ (Poulologos, 366)

(6)

ο

μ7

τà

γεúεσαι

u

mi

ta

VEvEsE

not

it–DOpl WP

taste–2sg Pres

‘You do not taste them’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, II 103)

As seen in Chapter 1, Mackridge (1993: 340—rule 2) also identifies

μ as an environment associated with preverbal pronoun placement.
Thus, what we have here is two cases where

ο, a factor associated with

postverbal pronoun placement is either preceded or followed (immedi-
ately) by an environment associated with preverbal pronoun placement
(

ν and μ). In both cases the result is preverbal placement of the pro-

noun. Trying to sort this out with rule-precedence arguments would fail
since it would have to be posited that in one case it is the environment
closest to the verb that takes precedence (as in

ο μ), while in another

case it is the factor furthest from the verb that wins out (as in

ν ο).

Clearly, this is not the best way to construct a grammar.

As was discussed in Chapter 3, both Mackridge and Horrocks (1990)

believe

ο to be a clitic attached to the verb, a position that follows the

characterization of

ο as a proclitic in Ancient Greek (Smyth 1956, West

background image

48

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

1982). There it was mentioned that if this were indeed so, the only ex-
planation for the categorical postverbal placement of pronouns when the
complex is immediately preceded by

ο is that the compound

ο–verb–pronoun appear immediately after factors that favor postverbal
placement, such as initial, coordinating conjunction and so forth. How-
ever, it was also demonstrated that the pronoun appears postverbally
even when the compound

ο–verb–pronoun follows elements associated

with preverbal placement such as subjects, object complements, and
temporal expressions. This fact would seem to contradict the hypothesis
that the position of the weak object pronoun is affected by the clitic
status of

ο.

With this observation in mind, it is quite surprising that, when the

conditional conjunction (

"áν precedes the compound ο–verb–pronoun,

the pronoun appears preverbally between

ο and the verb. The explana-

tion to this problem may be found in the proclitic nature of (

"áν (Jan-

naris, 1968: §97d), if we assume that it procliticizes to

ο, thus forming

a unit that needs no phonological host.

9

In fact, Jannaris (1968: §99,

1827) mentions the ability of

ο to ‘annex enclitics’ and form stressed

forms; this may be a case in which the annexed form is proclitic. As a
result of this,

ν ο would be considered as another function word (the

negative counterpart of

ν) and the preverbal pronoun placement would

be canonical.

The following example (7), found in the poem of Glykas, one of the

earliest attestations of ‘vernacular’ Greek, indicates that there may have
been some uncertainty as to what the host of

ο was in this situation.

(7)

ν

σè

οκ

κβáλλουσιν

an

sE

uk

Ekvalusin

if

you–DOsg WP

not

cast out–3pl Pres

‘If they do not get you out’ (Glykas, 229)

In this case the weak object pronoun does not appear next to the verb;

instead the negative marker

ο is interpolated. It is reasonable to hy-

pothesize that in this instance

ο procliticizes to the verb and that the

pronoun appears before the extended verb. Since this is an early attesta-
tion it may be indicative of a transitional phase, in which the constraint
that the pronoun be adjacent to the verb was not absolute, and therefore,
a different ordering of these phonologically dependent elements (weak
object pronoun,

ο) was possible. The existence of all three possibilities

background image

Linguistic Parameters

49

(

ν–ο–pronoun–verb in example 3, ν–ο–verb–pronoun, in example

4, and

ν–pronoun–ο–verb, in example 7) in a total of 22 tokens is

indeed suggestive of great fluidity in the ordering possibilities of these
elements.

An alternative to the ‘

ν procliticizes to ο’ explanation is that ο

procliticizes to the pronoun–verb complex, instead of forming a mor-
phological unit with

ν. Although this proposition is logically possible,

it cannot explain why the pronoun still appears postverbally when the
compound

ο–verb–pronoun is preceded by an element which is associ-

ated with preverbal placement, e.g., a subject or an object.

However, if one accepts that both the weak pronoun and the negative

adverb

ο are competing for verb adjacency then it is possible to explain

this curious pattern as follows.

First let’s assume these two constraints:

1.

If unstressed,

ο must procliticize to the verb appearing adjacent to

it.

2.

Pronouns must appear adjacent to the verb, although they can be
placed either in postverbal or preverbal position.

If these assumptions are granted, then:
1.

When

ο is preceded by a content word (subject, adverb, etc.) it

must attach immediately to the left of the verb. Since the weak pro-
nouns also have a requirement of adjacency they occupy the posi-
tion immediately to the right of the verb, thus the order
ο–verb–pronoun.

2.

If

ο forms a morphological unit with another element (e.g., ν, μ)

then this unit is stressed and functions as an independent word, in
which case the weak pronoun appears preverbally, yielding

ν

ο–pronoun–verb.

One of the advantages of this analysis is that it provides justification

for the fact that

ο is the only factor showing 100% postverbal pronoun

placement. Another advantage is that it also allows for a coherent ac-
count of the disappearance of

ο from the language, which up until now

is supposed to have happened due to competition from the negative
marker

δéν, as discussed in Jannaris (1968). The facts revealed above,

however, indicate that an equally important factor may have been the
constraint that

ο had to appear in the position immediately to the left of

the verb, a position which was eventually (in Early Modern Greek) co-
opted by weak object pronouns. This is by no means to be construed as
an assertion that speakers had no other alternative. Obviously, the con-

background image

50

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

straint that

ο had to appear immediately to the left of the verb could

just as easily have been eliminated, or the weak object pronouns could
not have taken over the left-adjacent position to the verb. The fact is that
in an investigation of textual evidence scholars rarely get to examine the
full-fledged variation that is present in a language, as those who study
synchronic phenomena are able to. It must be assumed that there was a
much richer pattern of variation, which, for reasons that we may never
uncover, was filtered down to the level of variation that can be observed
in the texts.

Nonetheless, an interesting point of comparison concerning the analy-

sis presented above comes from the facts of the Greek dialect of Pontic,
which was spoken in several pockets along the southern coast of the
Black Sea. Two of its major varieties are oenountiaka, a western variety,
which shows the effects of dialect contact with Standard Modern Greek
(cf. Kontosopoulos 1981), and trapezountiaka, an eastern variety spoken
in the vicinity of Trabzond. The variety of trapezountiaka has retained
many archaic features one of which is the use of the negative adverb

οκ

(in the form of ’

κι or ’κ) and it also shows robust postverbal placement

of the weak object pronoun even after the future marker

θá (cf. Oiko-

nomide¤s 1958, example 8).

(8)

θá

4παíρ4

%τεν

Ta

pEr

atEn

Fut

take–1sg Pres

she–DOsg WP

‘I will take her …’ (Oikonomide¤s, 1958: 408)

The oenountiaka variety, on the other hand, has not only retained the

negative marker

οκ, but it has also borrowed δéν from Standard Mod-

ern Greek, and shows variation in pronoun placement, which is very
similar to the pattern of variation that one finds in Later Medieval Greek
(Oikonomide¤s 1958: 3, 413). Unfortunately, however, there are no ex-
amples in which the negative marker ’

κι is paired with a weak object

pronoun construction in the available data. Nevertheless, the interaction
between negative markers and weak object pronoun placement in Pontic
Greek, supports, on the one hand, the hypothesis of competition between
the two grammatical elements for preverbal position in LMG, while it
also shows that the development that took place between Later Medieval
Greek and Standard Modern Greek was not an inevitable one. It was
simply one of the many possibilities available.

background image

Linguistic Parameters

51

Differentiation within the factor reduplicated object

Another instance where the pattern of variation is more complex than it
first seems is when the weak object pronoun co-refers with a noun-
phrase object that immediately precedes the verb. In such constructions,
the weak pronoun can co-refer either with a direct object (example 9) or
with an indirect object (example 10).

(9)

τò διáδημαν

παíρνει

το

to DiaDiman

pErni

to

the crown–DOsg

takes–3sg Pres

it–DOsg WP

‘The crown, he takes it’ (Belisarios, 42)

(10)

τοùς τριακοσíους

%φνω

σας

tus triakosius

afino

sas

the three hundred–DOpl

leave–1sg Pres

you–IOpl WP

%πò

Kνòς

φαρíου

apo

Enos

fariu

from

one–Gen sg

horse–Gen sg

‘To the three-hundred, I leave you each a horse’ (Digene¤s, 1759)

In those cases where there is both an indirect and a direct weak object

pronoun (‘ditransitive’ constructions), only the direct object noun-phrase
which co-refers with a pronoun appears in the clause, as in example (11).
However, the number of such tokens is too small to provide any real
indication of a pattern.

(11)

λα

%φηγθη

τς

τα

ola

afiViTi

tis

ta

all–DOpl

narrate–1sg Past

she–IOsg WP

they–DOpl WP

‘All things, I narrated them to her’ (Lybistros, 1847)

The first matter to raise concern in this construction is that there is the

possibility that the co-referring pronoun is part of a ditransitive con-
struction where both objects appear in pronominal form, as in example
(11) above. One has to wonder whether the position of the second weak
pronoun is affected by the placement of the pronoun that co-refers with
the noun-phrase. Examine, for example, the following construction:

background image

52

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(12)

τò ποσòν

γρáφω

σς

το

to poson

Vrafo

sas

to

the amount–DOsg

write–1Pres sg

you–IOpl

it–DOsg WP

‘The amount, I write it to you’ (Thre¤nos, 777)

In this example there is a direct object (

τò ποσóν), the pronoun that

co-refers with it (

το), and an indirect object which appears in pronomi-

nal form (

σς). Both pronouns are placed postverbally, presumably ac-

cording to Mackridge’s rule that makes mention of the ‘doubling pro-
noun’ constraint, although he does not discuss this particular case and
there is no obvious reason why the pronoun

σς could not appear before

the verb. Indeed

,

such constructions in which the two pronouns are split

on either side of the verb, although not found in this database, have been
documented in Joseph (1978/1990) for LMG and in Janse (1998) for
Cappadocian Greek (see example 13).

(13)

καì τẃρα μè

ε'πé

το

kE tora

mE

ipE

to

and now

me-DOsg WP

say-2sg Impv

it-IOsg WP

‘And now tell me it!’ (Lybistros 2276—Paris version, cited in Joseph

1978/1990)

As mentioned above, the number of ditransitive reduplicated object

constructions in this database is very small (there are only six). In three
of the examples, both pronouns appear postverbally, while in the other
three the pronouns appear preverbally.

One interesting fact that does arise from examining these constructions

is that all three situations in which the pronouns appear preverbally in-
volve the adjective

λος /olos/ ‘all’, which is the element immediately

preceding the verb complex as in example (11).

10

Moreover, this adjec-

tive when it is found in reduplicated object constructions seems to affect
a strong tendency for preverbal pronoun placement (cf. example 14).

Table A.10 in the appendix presents the effect that adjusting for this

parameter has on the dataset. Of the 118 tokens of reduplicated object
constructions there are 38 in which the element immediately preceding is
a form of the adjective

λος. Of these 38, 24 show preverbal placement

and 14 postverbal placement. If we exclude these tokens from the factor
reduplicated object, there are 65 examples with postverbal placement
and 15 examples showing preverbal placement. This new pattern of

background image

Linguistic Parameters

53

variation, as it turns out is not significantly different from the patterns of
variation for the other factors identified in Group 1 (Chapter 3) as can be
seen in Figure 4.1. (

ο has been excluded from this test).

(14)

τèς χρες μου

1λóγυρα

tEs xorEs mu

oloVira

the-countries-my

all around

λες

τèς

%φανíζει

olEs

tEs

afanizi

all–DOpl

they–DOpl WP

destroy–3sg Pres

‘My countries all around, all he destroys them’ (Rimada, 1308)

It is only natural to wonder why the adjective

λος should be associ-

ated with a pattern of pronoun placement that is the inverse of what is
expected for the reduplicated object construction in general. It is sug-
gested here that the model for such positioning can be found in the

λος

partitive construction, exemplified in (15).

(15)

λοι

τους

τ7ν

γρικοσι

oli

tus

tin

Vrikusi

all–Nom sg

they–PSpl WP

she–DOpl WP

listen–3pl Pres

‘All of them hear it’ (Rimada, 642)

(16)

καì λους

τους

νíκησε

kE

olus

tus

EnikisE

and

all–DOpl

they–PSpl WP

win–3sg Past

In the case of example (15), it is evident not only from the accent

markings but also from the context, that

τους is not an argument of the

verb but of the adjective and has a partitive sense. However, in a sen-
tence like example (16), we know that the weak pronoun is an argument
of the adjective and translate it as ‘and he defeated them all’ (Rimada,
322) only because there is no accent on

τους. The alternative interpreta-

tion, though, namely that

τους is an argument of the verb, with a trans-

lation ‘and all, he defeated them’ is also possible. In fact, there is no rea-
son to believe that this type of construction would be any clearer for lis-
teners of LMG, since the only disambiguating factor would have been
the constraint that doubling pronouns must follow the verb. It seems
likely, then, that in sentences such as these,

τους may have become am-

background image

54

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

biguous. It could be either a partitive pronoun qualifying the adjective or
an argument of the verb.

Figure 4.1

Comparing factors in Group 1 but excluding tokens with

λος from

the factor reduplicated object

The crucial aspect of this ambiguity is that the partitive pronoun is of

the same gender, number and case as the adjective, as indeed would have
been the case for a weak object pronoun. Thus, the shift to reduplicated
object constructions with

λος in which the pronoun is placed prever-

bally was most probably based on a four-part analogy, essentially a
process in which the speaker/hearer extracted a pattern of matching gen-
der/number/case marking in both the adjective and the pronoun based on
the ambiguity between partitive and reduplicated object constructions, as
in Figure 4.2. This yielded constructions such as (14) above and (17).

(17)

λην

τ7ν

Lξεúρεις

olin

tin

iksEvris

all–DOsg

she–DOsg WP

know–2sg Pres

‘You know her completely’ (Gioustos, 184)

In Chapter 7, this hypothesis will be integrated within the broader ex-

planation of the diachronic development of the pattern of pronoun
placement in Post-Hellenistic Greek. Moreover, in Chapter 8, I offer a
possible way of handling the object pronoun pattern from a synchronic

postverbal

0

1

coordnating

initial

oti

red. object

factor

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

background image

Linguistic Parameters

55

point of view that makes crucial reference to the pattern of the partitive
construction.

λους

:

τους

: :

λην

:

X

X

=

τν

Figure 4.2

Four-part analogy schema for the change in pronoun placement in

λος constructions

The effect of emphasis on pronoun placement

Emphasis is another linguistic factor, which Mackridge identifies as
playing a role in the variation of pronoun placement. For him, the differ-
ence in pronoun placement between fronted constituent (‘almost cate-
gorically preverbal), subject (‘normally preverbal’), and temporal ex-
pression
(‘free variation’) is based on the fact that these elements are
differently emphasized. His reasoning is as follows: the canonical word
order of Later Medieval Greek is SVO, with a non-overt subject more
often than not. Thus, when an object (or non-temporal adverb) is placed
before the verb as in example (18), it receives special emphasis, which
allows it to ‘attract’ the pronoun to the preverbal position. Subjects are in
a canonical position when they precede the verb and this ‘… does not
necessarily result in [their] being specially emphasized’ (Mackridge
1993: 320), which results in a less robust pattern of preverbal placement
associated with immediately preceding subjects. Finally, temporal ex-
pressions (see example 19) ‘… are not normally emphatic in themselves,
but tend instead to place emphasis on the following verb …’ (ibid: 322),
which, according to Mackridge, weakens the preverbal placement pat-
tern even more.

(18)

μανδáτον

τòν

δκα

mandaton

ton

EDoka

message–DOsg

he–IOsg WP

give–1sg Past

‘I gave him a message’ (Poulologos, 576)

(19)

πáλε %πομοιρáζεις

τα

palE

apomirazis

ta

again

break up–2sg Pres

they–DOpl WP

‘Again you break them up’ (Poulologos, 395)

background image

56

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

This line of reasoning is problematic in several ways. First, the argu-

ment is circular. Mackridge begins by asserting that it is the emphasized
status of fronted objects that ‘attracts’ the pronoun to the preverbal posi-
tion at the beginning of his analysis. Then, however, he interprets the
‘freer’ placement of object pronouns that is associated with the factor
temporal expression as an indication that temporal adverbs are not as
emphatic as fronted objects, thus explaining the difference in pronoun
placement between factors temporal expression and fronted constituent.

Second, in order for the ‘attraction’ mechanism to work, the ‘attract-

ing’ element (whether subject, object, or adverb) would have to be a
phonological host for the pronoun, as can be seen from Halpern’s (1995)
treatment of Bulgarian clitics, in which he assumes that they are uni-
formly enclitic, despite evidence (Ewen 1979) that they may be at times
proclitic. However, such a position cannot be entertained for Later Me-
dieval Greek because, as was mentioned earlier, the verb is the only
available phonological host for the pronoun, and it is difficult to justify
how another element, which does not bind the pronoun would affect its
position. In a similar vein, Wanner (1981b: 200) criticizes the use of the
term ‘attraction’ by prescriptivist grammarians in Portuguese as an ex-
planation of the variation between preverbal and postverbal placement of
weak object pronouns. He writes:

Attributing proclisis to the presence of particular words is satisfactory
only in a framework which does not recognize linguistic structure be-
yond the level of abstractness of the word, i.e., the typical prescriptive
grammar tradition; in addition, it is a confusion of cause and effect.

Finally, if emphasis does indeed ‘attract’ the pronoun to the preverbal

position, full pronoun subjects should be associated with categorical
preverbal placement of the weak pronoun. This is the expectation, be-
cause, as in any (so-called) ‘empty subject’ or ‘pro-drop’ language, full
pronoun subjects in Medieval Greek should be an indication that there is
emphasis placed on the subject—see Haberland and van der Auwera
(1990). In turn, one would expect that, whenever a full pronoun subject
immediately precedes the verb complex, the pronoun will appear in pre-
verbal position, if not all the time, at least in the majority of cases. How-
ever, the comparison test shows that there is no significant difference
between pronoun placement associated with full pronoun subjects and
that associated with other types of subjects (cf. Figure 4.3). Example

background image

Linguistic Parameters

57

(20) demonstrates a postverbal pronoun after the full pronoun

γẃ. Thus,

the emphatic status of the element preceding the verb complex does not
seem to affect the placement of the pronoun.

(20)

λλος

φιλε

τ7ν

%γαπ

alos

fili

tin

aVapo

another–Nom sg

kiss–3sg Pres

Rel prn

love–1sg Pres

κF γẁ

στερεúγομαí

την

kj EVo

stErEvVomE

tin

and I–Nom sg

to be deprived–1sg Pres

she–DOsg WP

‘Another man kisses the one I love and I am deprived of her’ (Katalogia,
434)

Figure 4.3

Comparing pronominal vs. nominal subjects

The effect of discourse constraints on pronoun placement

The possibility that discourse constraints may affect the placement of the
pronoun, especially in cases where a subject immediately precedes the
verb–pronoun complex has been brought up by Janse in two papers
(1994, 1998). Janse claims that in Cappadocian Greek (which also shows
variation between preverbal and postverbal pronoun placement) the pro-
noun is placed preverbally when the subject (especially subject pro-
nouns) ‘constitute[s] the information focus of the respective utterances,

preverbal

0.0

0.5

1.0

1.5

nominal

pronoun

factor

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

background image

58

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

since they carry new information’ (cf. example 21, taken from Janse
1998).

(21)

tis

t

a!lakse

who–Nom sg

it–DOpl WP

change–3sg Past

oVo

d

a!laksa

I–Nom sg (EMPHATIC)

it–DOpl WP

change–1sg Past

‘—Who changed them?
—I changed them’

However, Janse does not show that being the ‘information focus’ of an

utterance is the necessary and sufficient condition for preverbal place-
ment, since he does not discuss examples with subject pronouns (or
nominal subjects for that matter) and postverbal object pronoun place-
ment to show that in these cases the subject is not the ‘information fo-
cus’ of the utterance. Furthermore, it can be shown that in the Later Me-
dieval Greek texts this distinction does not affect the placement of the
pronoun. The two passages below (examples 22 and 23) come from the
same text and have the same interpretation with respect to the fo-
cus–topic distinction, yet the pronoun is placed postverbally in one and
preverbally in the other.

(22)

1κáποτF

%πεσẃσασιν

Nλθαν ε8ς

τò Μοντóριον

okapot

apEsosasin

ilTan is

to montorion

sometime

finish–3pl Past

come–3pl Past

the Montorion

1 δοùξ

τοùς

%ποδéχθηκεν

o Duks

tus

apoDExTikEn

the duke–Nom sg

they–DOpl WP

receive–3sg Past

‘In time they finished [their journey], they came to Montorion. /
The duke received them’ (Phlo¤rios, 303-304)

(23)

καβαλλικεúουν

ρχοντες

#πáγουν

Fς τò παλáτι,

kavalikEvun

arxondEs

ipaVun

s to palati

ride–3pl Pres

lords–Nom

go–3pl Pres

to the palace

κι 1 βασιλεùς

δéχθην

τους

kj o vasilEfs

EDExTin

tus

and the king–Nom sg

receive–3sg Past

they–DOpl WP

‘The lords ride, they go to the palace, /
and the king received them’ (Phlo¤rios, 938-939)

background image

Linguistic Parameters

59

Nevertheless, this distinction between a focus and a topic reading

could prove useful when examining preverbal objects. As Androulakis
(1998) points out, in Standard Modern Greek, a preposed object that
carries a focus reading is distinguished from a preposed object that is a
topic by the fact that in the latter case a doubling pronoun is used. This
claim has been widely accepted (see also Warburton 1975, Kazazis and
Pentheroudakis 1976, Horrocks 1983, Joseph 1983b, Mackridge 1985,
Stavrou 1985, and Philippaki-Warburton 1985). Examples (24) and (25)
demonstrate this contrast.

(24)

τòν Νικολ

τòν

θαυμáζω

ton nikoli

ton

Tavmazo

Nikolis–DOsg TOP

he–DOsg WP

admire–1sg Pres

γιà τ7ν #πομον του
ja tin ipomoni tu
for his patience

‘Nikolis, I admire him for his patience’ (Androulakis 1998: 150)

(25)

τòν ΝΙΚΟΛΗ

θαυμáζω

γιà τ7ν #πομον του

ton nikoli

Tavmazo

ja tin ipomoni tu

Nikolis–DOsg FOC

admire

for his patience

‘It is Nikolis that I admire for his patience’ (Androulakis 1998: 150)

However, it is not certain that this was the case for Later Medieval

Greek as well. In fact, without the necessary prosodic information (i.e.,
information about sentence stress), this distinction between topic and
focus is hard to confirm based on the surrounding context alone. Take
for example the following two clauses that appear one after the other in
the poem Thre¤nos te¤s Ko¤nstantinoupoleo¤s:

(26)

γíνηκε

4Αντíχριστος

τòν κóσμον

σακτανíζει/

EVinikE

andixristos

ton kosmon

saktanizi

became

antichrist

the world–DOsg

bedevils–3sg Pres

τò γéνος τò ;Ρωμαικòν

καταδοúλωσéν

το

to VEnos to romaikon

EkataDulosEn

to

the race the Roman–DOsg

enslave–3sg Past

it–DOsg WP

‘He became the antichrist, he bedevils the world, /
the Roman race, he enslaved it’ (Thre¤nos, 601-2)

background image

60

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

There do not seem to be any contextual factors that would make the

two objects (

τòν κóσμον, and τò γéνος τò ;Ρωμαικóν) different with

respect to the focus–topic distinction, yet only the latter is reduplicated.
Such examples are not rare in the corpus and suggest that, although there
is good reason to believe that the distinction between an object that is a
topic and an object that receives focus is the reason for the use (or not)
of a doubling pronoun in Standard Modern Greek, the same claim cannot
be made for Later Medieval Greek. If there is a structural explanation for
this part of the variation, it must lie elsewhere.

Weak object pronoun placement with non-finite forms of the
verb

It is reasonable to investigate whether the status of the verb-form (finite
or non-finite) affects the placement of pronouns in Later Medieval Greek
as this has been accepted as the determining factor for pronoun place-
ment in Standard Modern Greek (cf. Joseph 1978/1990, Mackridge
1985). In SMG, the pronouns appear preverbally (cf. examples 27 and
28), except when the verb is a gerund, or an imperative

11

in which case

they appear postverbally.

(27)

Τ7ν

ξéρω

tin

ksEro

she–DOsg WP

know–1sg Pres

‘I know her’

(28)

Τ7 Ρωξáννη,

τ7ν

ξéρω

καλá

ti roksani

tin

ksEro

kala

the Roxanne–DOsg

she–DOsg WP

see–1sg Pres

well

‘Roxanne, I know her well’

(29)

Μαθαíνοντáς

το

maTEnondas

to

learn–Gerund

it–DOsg WP

‘Upon learning it’

(30)

μ7

βλéποντáς

το

mi

vlEpondas

to

not

see–Gerund

it–DOsg WP

‘By not seeing it’

background image

Linguistic Parameters

61

(31)

UΑσε

μο

το

asE

mu

to

leave–2 sg Impv

I–IOsg WP

it–DOsg WP

‘Leave it to me!’

As can be seen above, the imperative in Standard Modern Greek, and

the clearly non-finite gerund show the same pattern of postverbal pro-
noun placement. This fact—in conjunction with the observation that the
imperative is morphologically marked only for number—has been inter-
preted as an indication that in SMG the imperative is a non-finite form
(Joseph 1978/1990, 1983a, 1985, Mackridge 1985, Joseph and Warbur-
ton 1987, Horrocks 1990, Nevis and Joseph 1993). In Later Medieval
Greek, on the other hand, there are three clearly non-finite forms, the
participle (present active or perfect passive), the gerund and the infini-
tive. The status of the imperative is much harder to determine, yet, as
will be seen briefly, pronoun placement indicates that it behaves more
like a non-finite form than a finite one. In the next sections, the pattern
of pronoun placement associated with these forms is examined.

Participles

Present Active

These forms show marking for gender, number, and case as in Ancient
Greek. There are only nine examples of pronoun placement that involve
a present active participle, and most of them are from Pto¤khoprodromos.
Some illustrative examples are provided below (32-36). It must be noted
that the use of these forms in Pto¤khoprodromos is generally accepted as
an archaizing aspect of his mixed language (Horrocks 1997: 78). Thus,
while these constructions are interesting to report, they do not contribute
to our understanding of pronoun placement in LMG.

(32)

%τíμως

μοι

λαλοσα

atimos

mi

lalusa

deceitfully

I–IOsg WP

speak–Pres Act Prcle

‘Speaking to me deceitfully’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, A 155)

(33)

τατα

μοι

προσοιποσα

tafta

mi

prosipusa

this–DOpl

I–IOsg WP

tell–Pres Act Prcle

‘Telling me these things’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, I 198)

background image

62

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(34)

καì συνβυθíζουσáν

με

kE

sinviTizusan

mE

and

sink–Pres Act Prcle

I–DOsg WP

‘And sinking me’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, IV 243)

(35)

καì σον

σè

φυλáττων

kE soon

sE

filaton

and safe–Acc sg

you–DOsg WP

keep–Pres Act Prcle

‘And keeping you safe’ (Glykas, 341)

(36)

τòν

καταφλéξαντá

σε

ton

kataflEksanda

sE

the–Acc sg

burn thoroughly–Pres Act Prcle

you–DOsg WP

‘The one who burnt you thoroughly’ (Achilleid, 1410)

Perfect Passive

Perfect passive participles in LMG end in –

μéνος, and are mostly used

as adjectives or as complements of the verb

/χω /Exo/ ‘I have’ in the

perfect periphrasis (or of the past form

ε'χα /ixa/ ‘I had’ in the pluper-

fect periphrasis—cf. Horrocks 1997: 304). In the database used in this
study, there are only four instances of such a construction that also in-
clude a weak object pronoun. In all of them the pronoun is placed before
the

/χω form, as in example (37).

(37)

καì

χρóνο

Vναν 9μισυ

kE

xrono

Enan imisi

and

year–Acc sg

one and a half–Acc sg

μè

ε'χε

φυλακισμéνον

mE

ixE

filakismEnon

I–DOsg WP

have–3sg Past

imprison–Perf Pass Prcle

‘And for a year and a half he had me imprisoned’ (Lybistros, 2349)

In these constructions, the semantic arguments of the perfect passive

participle have become syntactic arguments of the

/χω form, since weak

object pronouns must always appear adjacent to the verb that selects
them. This phenomenon is known as ‘argument incorporation’, or ‘ar-
gument composition’, or ‘clitic climbing’. Despite the fact that the two
verb-forms share arguments, it is not necessary that they be adjacent.
This can be seen in example (38) where the adverb

δ can be interpo-

lated between the

/χω form and the participle.

background image

Linguistic Parameters

63

(38)

τà

μDχει

δ

γραμμéνα

ta

moxi

Do

VramEna

Rel prn

I–IOsg+have–3sg Pres

here

write–Perf Pass Prcle

‘Which he has written to me here’ (Rimada, 716)

Gerund

These forms, although clearly derived from the above-mentioned present
active participles, show no gender, number, or case agreement. Instead,
they vary between a form with final -

ς and one without it. The final -ς is

most likely due to analogical spreading, either from the masculine nomi-
native singular or from the adverbial -

ς (see Horrocks 1997: 229). There

are several constructions of a gerund with a weak pronoun argument;
they are all found in later texts (15

th

and early 16

th

century), and in all of

them the pronoun appears postverbally as in examples (39) and (40):

(39)

γẁ

θωρẃντα

σε

EVo

Toronda

sE

I–Nom sg

look–Gerund

you–DOsg WP

‘I, looking at you’ (Rhodos, 211)

(40)

καì βλéποντá

την

kE

vlEponda

tin

and

see–Gerund

she–DOsg WP

‘And seeing her’ (Tribo¤le¤s I, 275)

Unfortunately, there are no instances of a negated gerund (

μ + ger-

und) with a weak object pronoun in this database. Such examples are
crucial because they would provide information concerning the interac-
tion between the finiteness of the verb-form and pronoun placement. A
putative example such as *

καì μ7 βλéποντá την /kE mi vlEponda tin/

‘and not seeing her’ would clearly indicate that the pattern of pronoun
placement associated with gerunds could only be postverbal. Although
no firm conclusions can be reached in their absence, it is my intuition
that this may have indeed been the case and that postverbal pronouns
were the only option in this context for the language of LMG.

Infinitives

In this section, I examine the pattern of pronoun placement associated
with two vestiges of the infinitive that survive in Later Medieval Greek:
the articular infinitive and infinitival complement structures.

background image

64

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Articular infinitive. I use this term descriptively, in order to capture the
fact that in all of these uses the infinitive is marked by a definite article.
However, only constructions as in (41) seem to be directly linked to the
‘articular infinitive’ of Classical Greek (see Joseph 1978/1990, 1983a for
more details on the history of the Greek infinitive). In the texts examined
for this study, an infinitive marked by a definite article can be used ei-
ther as the complement of a preposition or a verb (42), as a clause with a
final sense (43) or as a nominalized adjunct (44). This last construction
has also been labeled an ‘absolutive infinitive’ by Hesseling (1892), and
a ‘temporal infinitive’ (Mihevc!-Gabrovec! 1973). Horrocks (1997: 98,
280) provides a more detailed discussion of these uses. In all cases, the
pronoun is always placed postverbally.

(41)

Wρξατο

το

γελν

με

irksato

tu

VElan

mE

begin–3sg Past

the–Gen sg

laugh–Infin

I–DOsg WP

‘He began to make fun of me’ (Pto¤khoprodromos, I 190)

(42)

ε8ς

τò

εεργετσαι

σοι

is

to

EvErVEtisE

si

towards

the

benefit–Infin

you–IOsg WP

‘Towards benefiting you’ (Spanos, 690)

(43)

χρóνον %νáλωσα

...

το

ε#ρεν

σε

xronon analosa

... tu

Evrin

sE

time I spent

... the–Gensg

find–Infin

you–DOsg WP

‘I spent [much] time in order to find you’ (Spanos, 606)

(44)

Τò

8δεν

τα

to

iDin

ta

the–Acc sg

see–Infin

it–DOpl WP

‘Upon seeing them’ (Digene¤s, 785)

Infinitive as the complement of a verb. Infinitival complement structures
begin to lose ground to finite complementation constructions as early as
Hellenistic Greek (Joseph 1978/1990, Browning 1983, Joseph 1983a,
Horrocks 1997). However, an infinitival complement is still a possibility
in the texts of Later Medieval Greek. Most appear in the periphrases of
the future tense (

θéλω ‘I want’ + infinitive, example 45), the conditional

(

Wθελα ‘I wanted’ + infinitive, example 46) and the pluperfect

12

(

ε'χα ‘I

background image

Linguistic Parameters

65

had’ + infinitive, example 47), but there are also some examples of a
standard infinitival complement (example 48).

(45)

θéλω

σς

%φηγεσθαι

TElo

sas

afiVisTE

want–1sg Pres

you–IOpl WP

narrate–Infin

‘I will narrate (it) to you’ (Belisarios, 570)

(46)

ταν Wθελες

δοξασθν

otan

iTElEs

DoksasTin

when

want–2sg Past

glorify–Pass Infin

‘When you would be glorified’ (Digene¤s, 252)

(47)

δèν σF ε'χα

δẃσει

DEn

sixa

Dosi

not

you–IOsg + have–1sg Past

give–Infin

‘I had not given you’ (Katalogia, 323)

(48)

ταν 9λπιζα

/χειν

σε

φíλον

otan

ilpiza

Exin

sE

filon

when

hope–2sg Past

have–Infin

you–DOsg WP

friend

‘When I hoped to have you as a friend’ (Spanos, 1293)

The role of infinitival complements in the variation of weak object

pronoun placement has not been investigated in depth. Mackridge (1993:
338) only discusses the cases of

θéλω and Wθελα periphrases for which

he states that the position of pronouns in ‘the future and volitive con-
struction

θéλω + infinitive’ can be handled according to his rules as long

as one treats the pronouns as elements that always attach themselves to
θéλω and not to the infinitive. What Mackridge overlooks in this as-
sumption, however, is that, since the pronouns must appear adjacent to
the verb that selects them as arguments, attachment to

θéλω implies in-

corporation of the semantic arguments of the infinitival form as syntactic
arguments of the

θéλω form (which Joseph 1978/1990 describes via the

optional application of ‘clause union’). And although this may be a pos-
sibility for the future constructions (as it was for the

/χω + passive parti-

ciple periphrases) there is no evidence that it also occurs in volitive con-
structions. Perhaps, though, Mackridge’s mention of ‘future and volitive

background image

66

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

construction’ is merely a way to avoid the controversy over the meaning
of these

θéλω + infinitive constructions.

Nonetheless, according to this assumption, the contrast between ex-

ample (49) and example (50) is explained, as follows. In (49) the pro-
noun attaches to

θéλει, and is then ‘attracted’ to the preverbal position

by the relative pronoun

τις since this is a function word. On the other

hand, in (50) the pronoun once again attaches to

θéλει, but this time it

appears postverbally, because the complex follows immediately after the
negative adverb

ο.

(49)

τì

τòν

θéλω

ποíσει

ti

ton

TElo

pisi

what

he–IOsg WP

want–1sg Pres

do–Infin

‘What I will do to him’ (Digene¤s, 936)

(50)

ο θéλει

τèς

πομεíνει

u

TEli

tEs

pomini

not

want–3sg Pres

it–DOpl WP

suffer–Infin

‘He will not suffer them’ (Rimada, 1016)

The question of which of the two verbs is the host of the pronoun is

important. If Mackridge is correct that the

θéλω form is always the host,

then these constructions will have nothing to contribute to our discussion
of weak object pronoun placement and non-finite verb-forms. If, how-
ever, there are examples in which the pronoun unambiguously attaches
itself to the infinitival form, then these constructions must be taken into
consideration.

One problematic aspect of Mackridge’s analysis concerns the accen-

tuation of the pronoun. As was discussed in Chapter 2, when the host of
a weak pronoun is a proparoxytone element to its left, the rule of secon-
dary stress should take effect adding an accent to the last syllable of this
element. Since, according to Mackridge, all weak object pronouns that
appear to the left of

θéλω in a periphrastic construction are enclitics, one

would expect that when the pronoun appears after a proparoxytone form
of

θéλω, such as Wθελα, a secondary accent would be placed on this

form (

Wθελá). This prediction, however, is not borne out in the one case

of a

θéλω periphrastic construction that fits this description, as can be

seen in example (51).

background image

Linguistic Parameters

67

(51)

μρμυκα

καì

κẃνωπα

os

mirmika

kE

konopa

like

ant–Acc sg

and

mosquito–Acc sg

Wθελα

τòν

συντρíψειν

iTEla

ton

sindripsin

want–1sg Past

he–DOsg WP

crush–Infin

‘Like an ant and a mosquito I would have crushed him’ (Achilleid, 1696)

There is one other example (52), in which a secondary accent does

appear, but this is a suspicious case. First, it seems to have a volitional
meaning—Pichard (1956) translates it as ‘voulait m’epouser’—and this
makes the separation between the Infinitive and its object pronoun sur-
prising. Moreover, the infinitive is in the first hemistich while the rest of
the VP is in the second hemistich (see Chapter 5 for more details about
the meter of the poetry). This construction is extraordinary for a variety
of reasons, and therefore it is wise not to base any conclusions on it. So
far, the available evidence casts doubt on Mackridge’s claim that the
pronoun always attaches itself to the

θéλω form.

(52)

λαβεν

γυνακα

Wθελé

με

lavin

VinEka

iTElE

mE

take–Infin

woman–DOsg

want–3sg Past

I–DOsg WP

‘He wanted to marry me’ (Kallimakhos, 651)

Another argument against Mackridge’s implicit assumption that

θéλω

future periphrases always involve ‘argument incorporation’ is that the
infinitive can in some cases be preposed, as in example (53); such ‘free-
dom of movement’ is considered to be evidence against ‘argument in-
corporation’ (cf. Abeille and Godard 1996).

(53)

ε#ρεν

τ7ν

θéλω

Evrin

tin

TElo

find–Infin

she–DOsg WP

want–1sg Pres

‘I will find her’ (Phlo¤rios, 267)

If this were true, the pronoun in these cases should remain both the

semantic and syntactic argument of the infinitive, which in turn means
that the pronoun would be ‘enclitic’. However, the pronoun is written
with an accent when it appears preverbally (proclitic) and without an

background image

68

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

accent when it appears postverbally (enclitic), and out of the 10 in-
stances of a future periphrasis with a preposed infinitive, 8 of them have
a written accent on the pronoun. Thus, instances such as (53) may be an
indication that the pronoun is attached to the

θéλω form, and, subse-

quently, that ‘argument incorporation’ has taken place, despite the
counterevidence provided by the preposed infinitive. The two non-con-
forming examples are:

(54)

γẃ

δẃσει

τες

Wθελα

EVo

Dosi

tEs

iTEla

I–Nom sg

give–Infin

she–DOpl WP

want–1sg Past

‘I would have given them’ (Rimada, 1270)

(55)

χαρíσει

του

τ7ν

θéλω

xarisi

tu

tin

TElo

grant–Infin

he–IOsg WP

she–DOsg WP

want–1sg Pres

‘I will grant her to him’ (Lybistros, 2510)

Example (55) is particularly telling because if one were to adhere to

what the written accents indicate, then the indirect object

του and the

direct object

τν do not form a cluster, as the IO attaches to the infinitive

and the DO to

θéλω. This is indeed quite surprising, since split pronoun

constructions have not been previously reported in future or other pe-
riphrases. Such an example unavoidably casts serious doubts as to
whether the written accents can be trusted as a guide at this intersection
of two highly volatile constructions, weak pronoun placement and the
future periphrasis construction.

On the other hand, if the data is analyzed from the perspective of pro-

noun placement, Mackridge’s assertion that weak object pronouns be-
have as if they are always attached to the

θéλω form seems to be con-

firmed. Table A.11 in the appendix presents the results of coding the
tokens of

θéλω periphrasis + weak object pronoun constructions, ac-

cording to the character of the immediately preceding element, namely if
it is associated with preverbal or postverbal placement, or if it does not
affect placement in one way or another. Thus, postverbal environment,
includes the factors initial, coordinating conjunction, reduplicated ob-
ject
, and

ο. The column preverbal environment includes the factors

function word and fronted constituent, whereas neutral (for lack of a
better term) includes the factors subject and temporal expression. As the

background image

Linguistic Parameters

69

Table shows, in

θéλω periphrases with a postverbal environment, the

pronoun is placed between

θéλω and the infinitive 42 out of 44 times; in

preverbal environments the pronoun appears to the left of

θéλω 35 out of

39 times, while in neutral environments there are 8 post-

θéλω tokens

and 4 pre-

θéλω tokens. It must be noted, however, that in this last cate-

gory, the total number of tokens is too small to lead us to definite con-
clusions.

As a result, all one can say definitely is that ‘argument incorporation’

must occur at least in certain occasions, namely when the pronoun ap-
pears to the left of

θéλω. In the absence of any conclusive evidence con-

cerning the host of pronouns that appear between the

θéλω form and the

infinitive, it seems more straightforward to adopt the idea that all the
periphrastic tense constructions involve some type of ‘argument incorpo-
ration’ mechanism. At the same time, it must be noted that the LMG
evidence seriously challenges the notion that such mechanisms lead to a
strong linear adjacency requirement.

Finally, with respect to true complement infinitives, it can be said that

these constructions are rare, and appear mostly in texts before the 14

th

century (Digene¤s, Pto¤khoprodromos, Spaneas, Glykas, Poulologos, Mo-
reas
). The specific verbs found with an infinitive complementizer in the
corpus are

%ρχíζω /arxizo/ ‘I begin’, (L)μπορ /imboro/ ‘I am able to’,

Kλπíζω /Elpizo/ ‘I hope’, θαρρ /Taro/ ‘I dare’, and τολμ /tolmo/,
also ‘I dare’.

Only ten examples of this construction can be found in the database,

and in seven of these, the pronoun appears after the infinitive as in ex-
ample (48) above and (56) below. However, since these constructions
seem to be archaisms (note the use of the –

σθαι infinitive) it may be that

the pattern of pronoun placement associated with them is also archaic.

(56)

Wρξατο

εXχεσθαí

του

irksato

EfxEsTE

tu

begin–3sg Past

wish–Infin

he–IOsg WP

‘She began to wish him’ (Digene¤s, 810)

The evidence available from the LMG texts does not lead us to any

clear conclusions about the placement of pronouns that are arguments of
non-finite verb-forms. At best, all one can say is that texts from before
the 14

th

century have both a wider range of non-finite forms and varia-

tion between preverbal and postverbal pronoun placement in association

background image

70

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

with them. In texts after the 14

th

century, if the thorny issue of peri-

phrastic tenses is put aside, the available non-finite verb-forms are ger-
unds and articular infinitives, which in many respects are functionally
equivalent. In this context, the placement of the pronouns is robustly
postverbal. When these facts are compared with the situation in Standard
Modern Greek (cf. examples 27-31) it is clear that at least the beginning
of the partition of weak pronoun placement according to the finiteness of
the verb-form is found in 14

th

century texts. Now that the evidence for

the unambiguously non-finite forms has been established, I discuss pro-
noun placement in the case of imperative forms.

Imperative

When the verb is in the imperative form, the placement of the weak ob-
ject pronoun seems to vary in much the same way that it does for the
finite verb-forms, the indicative and the subjunctive. Mackridge (1993:
330) is so convinced of this that he emphatically asserts that, when an
imperative is preceded by a fronted constituent, the constraint for pre-
verbal pronoun placement applies as strongly as it would for any other
finite verb-form. Citing examples similar to (57), he claims that such
placement is ‘absolutely standard’ in texts from all regions dating from
the 12

th

to the 15

th

centuries.

(57)

6γíα τ7ν

ε8πé

aVia

tin

ipE

holy

she–DOsg WP

call–Impv sg

‘Call her holy’ (Thre¤nos, 35)

The textual evidence, however, does not support such a strong claim.

The relevant data will be discussed presently, but before delving into the
details, one must consider how an accurate comparison between the fi-
nite forms and the imperative forms can be conducted. First, the envi-
ronments in which either a finite form or an imperative form could ap-
pear need to be separated from the rest. Accordingly, all environments
subsumed under factor function word must be excluded since the im-
perative cannot appear after such words. Next, one must exclude the
negative marker

ο because the imperative does not have a directly ne-

gated form (instead

μ plus the subjunctive is used). The conjunction

τι must be removed from the list as well. Thus, the environments

13

in

which pronoun placement can be compared based on the verb-form are
represented by the following factors: initial, coordinating conjunction,

background image

Linguistic Parameters

71

reduplicated object, fronted constituent, subject, and temporal expres-
sion
.

As before, the remaining factors can be grouped into three categories,

preverbal, postverbal, and neutral environment. Cataloguing the data
according to these categories reveals that there is a difference between
the placement of the pronoun with finite verb-forms on the one hand and
imperative verb-forms on the other. Except for postverbal environment,
however, the number of tokens is small, and comes from a limited
amount of texts. For example, only ten texts have imperatives after a
fronted constituent so the results, which are presented in detail in the
appendix (Table A.12), are suggestive, but not conclusive. Nonetheless,
the patterns we observe are remarkably different.

For instance, when there is a fronted constituent immediately preced-

ing a finite verb, the pronoun appears preverbally 898 out of 988 times.
In the case of the imperative, however, this only occurs 15 out of 32
times. Examples (58) and (59) show postverbal pronoun placement in
such situations. Even though this result is only suggestive, these num-
bers do not in anyway confirm Mackridge’s intuition that whether the
verb is of imperative or indicative/subjunctive form does not affect the
placement of the pronoun.

Similarly, for the factor subject we find no preverbal pronouns if the

verb-form is imperative, while for finite verb-forms, an immediately
preceding subject is associated with preverbal placement 334 out of 464
times. Finally, for temporal expression we have 8 preverbal instances
out of 24 when the verb-form is imperative but 86 preverbal instances
out of 149 for a finite verb-form.

(58)

ε8ς τ7ν καρδíα σου

θéς

το

is tin karDia su

TEs

to

in the heart your

place–Impv sg

it–DOsg WP

‘Place it in your heart’ (Spaneas, 135)

(59)

τ7ν λúσην τοúτην

δóς

μας

tin lisin tutin

Dos

mas

the solution this

give–Impv sg

I–IOpl WP

‘Give us this solution’ (Gioustos, 416)

Thus, although preverbal pronoun placement is possible with impera-

tive verb-forms in Later Medieval Greek, it is extremely restricted, espe-

background image

72

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

cially when compared to the situation with the indicative and subjunctive
on the one hand, and the pattern associated with the gerunds on the
other. Contrary to Mackridge’s observation then, the imperative—with
respect to weak object pronoun placement—behaves more like the ger-
und and the infinitive (the articular one at least) than the finite verb-
forms. The reasons for this differentiation are discussed in Chapter 7.

Conclusion

In this chapter, the variation in weak object pronoun placement was ex-
amined in more detail, and it was demonstrated that there are several
linguistic factors that affect it. The data analysis confirmed Mackridge’s
(1993) observation that although

ο is associated with postverbal pro-

noun placement, an immediately preceding

ν ο is associated with pre-

verbal placement. Furthermore, it was shown for the first time that
within the factor reduplicated object, the adjective

λος behaves con-

trary to the general pattern since one finds pronouns in the preverbal
position when

λος is reduplicated. It was also demonstrated that when

the verb is of imperative form, pronoun placement is predominantly
postverbal, although there are not enough tokens to conduct a detailed
analysis of the pattern of variation. It is, nonetheless, clear that the avail-
able data does not confirm Mackridge’s (1993) claim that the imperative
shows no difference from the indicative with respect to object pronoun
position. On the other hand, it was clearly demonstrated in this chapter
that neither emphasis on a preverbal element (e.g., object, subject etc.),
nor the topic–focus distinction have any affect on the position of weak
object pronouns. In the next chapter, I examine several non-linguistic
parameters that may play a role in the pattern of pronoun placement.

background image

Linguistic Parameters

73

8

Jannaris (1968: §1827-1828) puts forth the argument that

μης in this case is a

“corruption” of

μηςν, a conjunction expressing “confindent expectation” and

that “copiers [after 300 A.D] … mistook it for another negative intended to
strengthen

ουη, and so changed it to the classical and familiar μης.”

9

In Pappas (2000, 2001a), I suggested that

ουη enclitizes to αYν in these cases.

Both hypotheses yield the same result—a new morphological unit that can carry
stress—but this one is more consistent with what is already known about the
status of

ουη and αYν in Ancient Greek.

10

According to Mackridge (1985)

οZλος exhibits some idiosyncrasies in

Standard Modern Greek as well, since it is the only adjective that has to be
doubled by a pronoun.

11

Joseph (1978/1990, 1983a), and Nevis & Joseph (1993) mention that the past

passive participle may have a weak pronoun argument in some rare cases. The
example they cite is

(

[

)

δεχοςμενοςς

το

DExomEnos

to

accept–Past Pass Prcle Nom sg

it–DOsg WP

‘Accepting it’
I did not encounter any such examples in this database of the LMG texts.

12

According to Joseph (1983a: 64, 2000a), Horrocks (1997: 304) the present

perfect periphrasis with the present tense of

εYχω ‘I have’ was modelled on the

pluperfect form at a much later time.

13

Since the imperative in LMG appear only in the 2

nd

singular and plural forms

with rare, highly stylized, and presumably consciously archaizing uses of the 3

rd

singular or plural it would seem appropriate to exclude all non-2

nd

person forms

from the finite verb-form database. It is, however, a reasonable assumption that
the person of the form does not affect the placement of the weak object pronoun,
and so it is not necessary to do so.

background image

73

5

Non-linguistic Parameters

Having considered the linguistic parameters that affect the variation in
pronoun position in Chapter 4, I proceed in Chapter 5 with the investi-
gation of the possible effects of non-linguistic parameters. Since most of
the texts analyzed in Chapters 3 and 4 are works of poetry, the first sec-
tion of this chapter focuses on the role of meter and how it interacts with
pronoun placement. In the second section, the possibility of dialectal
variation is entertained and it is demonstrated that, contrary to
Mackridge’s and Rollo’s observations, the pattern of weak object pro-
noun placement in the Medieval Cypriot chronicles is different from the
pattern one sees in documents from other areas of the Byzantine Empire.

Metrical constraints and weak object pronoun placement in
LMG texts

All but one of the texts that comprise the database for the investigation
of Later Medieval Greek weak object pronoun placement are written in
verse. Only the text of Spanos is a work of prose. The type of verse used
in all the other texts is known as the

πολιτικóς ‘political’ verse, which,

according to Horrocks (1997: 256) is ‘the standard accentual metre of
oral folk songs, medieval and early modern vernacular poetry, and much
learned Byzantine writing.’ A standard line written to fit this metrical
scheme, again according to Horrocks (ibid: 257), is ‘eight syllables in
the first hemistich followed by strong caesura, while the second hemi-
stich is seven syllables long. Typically, there are two clearly felt stresses
in each half of the line,’ occurring either on the second or fourth syllable
and the sixth or eighth syllable in the first hemistich, while in the second

background image

74

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

hemistich the accent may fall either on the tenth or twelfth syllable and,
obligatorily, on the fourteenth. The overall rhythm is thus iambic. Al-
though this is the prescribed meter, quite often the writers use sixteen-
syllable lines with the same accentual pattern.

The fact that these works are written in verse has played an important

role in scholars’ critical assessment of weak object pronoun placement in
LMG. Most if not all philologists who have prepared editions of the
manuscripts have treated the preservation of the metrical integrity of the
‘politikos’ verse as their most important duty. Thus, the assumption that
these pronouns could be placed either before or after the verb without
jeopardizing the linguistic authenticity of the text has become quite a
convenient tool for them, as will be seen in the examples below. The
practice seems to have been so widespread that it inspired Mackridge
(1993) to search for the rules that govern pronoun placement. Despite all
this, there has been no serious discussion of the possible effect that the
metrical constraints of the ‘politikos’ verse have had on the placement of
weak object pronouns specifically. Even Mackridge just briefly mentions
that the effect of meter may be seen in some instances when a subject
immediately precedes the verb complex, but he does not go into any
further detail (1993: 331).

The following discussion, however, will demonstrate that there are

certain metrical requirements, which interact with pronoun placement in
specific and consistent ways. This should not be taken, however, as evi-
dence that these metrically determined patterns of pronoun placement
are not grammatical, i.e., that the general audience would have perceived
these constructions as non-Greek or, even worse, that they would have
been unable to parse them. After all, there are several ways in which the
metrical requirements could have been satisfied. This is especially true
within the context of a written tradition, where the composer—one
would presume—could revisit passages and make the necessary changes
in order to satisfy both grammatical and metrical requirements. Thus, I
assume that, at worst, these constructions would have been perceived as
stylized but not as unintelligible. Moreover, one should allow for the
possibility that infrequent occurrences of less-than-grammatical con-
structions would be accepted if they were experienced within a robustly
grammatical context, while a succession of them would indeed prohibit
comprehension. As a consequence, it should be clear that the following
discussion of the effect of metrical constraints on the placement of weak
object pronouns is not meant to be taken as an argument for or against

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

75

the grammaticality of certain constructions. Instead, it is used as a
method to evaluate the validity of some of the statistical patterns, which
were presented in Chapter 3 within the context of another overarching
parameter.

The interaction between accent and pronoun placement

A close examination of those examples of pronoun placement in which
the verb complex is immediately preceded by a subject or a temporal
expression shows a curious interaction between the requirements for
accent placement of the ‘politikos’ verse and the position of the pronoun.
In particular, it seems that in the overwhelming majority of cases (almost
90%), if the alternative pronoun placement was chosen (e.g., postverbal
instead of preverbal) then the meter would have been affected in one of
the two following ways.

In the first case, the seventh syllable would be stressed, which ac-

cording to the formulation given by Horrocks above is not the preferred
accentual pattern. Indeed, from my own examination of the texts it
seems to be highly disfavored. As can be seen in an example such as (1),
where the complex is immediately preceded by a subject, the postverbal
placement of the pronoun would result in the accentuation of the seventh
syllable and not the eighth as is required. The latter is shown in bold in
the last line, which presents the alternative (altrn) position of the pro-
noun in order to demonstrate that this would indeed result in a violation
of the metrical requirements. Example (2) demonstrates that a violation
would also occur if a postverbal pronoun were changed to preverbal po-
sition.

(1)

κF οJ Τορ-κοι

τ7ν

-κó-ψα-σιν

kj i turki

tin

Ekopsasin

syll

1 2-3

4

5-6-7-8

and the Turks

she–DOsg WP

cut–3pl Past

τ7ν τα-πει-ν7ν

τ7ν Πá-τραν

tin tapinin

tin patran

9 10-11-12

13 14-15

the humble–DOsg

the Patra–DOsg

altrn

κF οJ Τορ-κοι

-κó-ψα-σíν

την

τ7ν τα-πει-ν7ν

τ7ν Πá-τραν

‘And the Turks cut her down, the humble (city of) Patra’ (Thre¤nos, 86)

background image

76

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(2)

1 4πι-τρα-πé-ζης

κροú-ει

τον

o pi trapEzis

krui

ton

syll

1 2 3-4-5

6-7

8

the table-master

hits

he–DOsg WP

σπα-θé-αν

4ς τò σκου-τá-ριν

spaTEan

s to skutarin

9-10-11

12 13-14-15

sword-strike

to the shield

altrn

1 4πι-τρα-πé-ζης τον κροú-ει σπα-θé-αν 4ς τò σκου-τá-ριν

‘The table-master hits him on the shield with the sword’ (Phlo¤rios, 671)

In the second type of violation, the fourteenth syllable would not be

accented. However, an accent on the fourteenth syllable is obligatory,
and, in the relevant constructions, it is clear that the alternative place-
ment of the pronoun would result in a disallowed rhythmic pattern, as
examples (3) and (4) demonstrate.

(3)

1 τα-ξι-διá-ρης

θé-λει

με

o taksiDjaris

TEli

mE

syll

1 2-3-4-5

6-7

8

the traveler–Nom sg

wants–3sg Pres

me–DOsg WP

οJ (ρ-ρω-στοι

ζη-τον

με

i arosti

zitun

mE

9 10-11-12

13-14

15

the sick–Nom pl

ask for–3pl Pres

me–DOsg WP

altrn

1 τα-ξι-διá-ρης θé-λει με οJ (ρ-ρω-στοι μè ζη-τον

‘The traveler wants me, the sick ask for me’ (Katalogia, 428)

(4)

πó-θος

σè

κα-τα-πó-νε-σεν,

poTos

sE

kataponEsEn

syll

1-2

3

4-5-6-7-8

passion

you–DOsg WP

hurt–3sg Past

%-γá-πη

-φλó-γι-σéν

σε

aVapj

EfloVisEn

sE

9-10-11

(11)-12-13-14

15

love

inflame–3sg Pst

you–DOsg WP

altrn

πó-θος σè κα-τα-πó-νε-σεν, %-γá-πη σ4 -φλó-γι-σεν

‘Passion has hurt you, and love has inflamed you’ (Digene¤s, 866)

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

77

The same violation of the metrical requirements occur when the verb

complex is preceded by a temporal expression, as can be seen from ex-
amples (5) and (6).

(5)

σúν-το-μα τòν

-τú-φλω-σε

sindoma

ton

EtiflosE

syll

1-2-3

4

5-6-7-8

soon

he–DOsg WP

blind–3sg Past

altrn

σúν-το-μα -τú-φλω-σé

τον

‘Quickly, he blinded him’ (Belisarios, 350)

(6)

συν-τó-μως φéρ-νου-σíν

τον

sindomos

fErnusin

ton

syll

9-10-11

12-13-14

15

soon

bring–3sg Past

he–DOsg WP

altrn

συν-τó-μως τòν

φéρ-νου-σιν

‘Quickly, they bring him’ (Belisarios, 344)

The facts concerning weak object pronoun placement and the factor

fronted constituent provide a very illuminating point of contrast to the
pattern associated with the above-mentioned factors. The instances in
which a change in pronoun placement would affect the meter of the
poem are still the vast majority. However, the percentage of instances in
which the pronoun could switch position without violating the con-
straints listed above is triple the analogous percentage for factors subject
and temporal expression (182 tokens out of 890, roughly 21%, vs. 28 out
of 405, 7%, see Table A.13). The OneWay Anova test also confirms that
preverbal pronoun placement associated with factor fronted constituent
is not as affected by the metrical constraints of the genre as it is when the
factors in questions are subject and temporal expression (see Figure 5.1
below).

(8)

λε-γ-τον

τòν

-ποι-σεν

lEVaton

ton

EpjisEn

syll

9-10-11

12

13-14-15

legate–Acc sg

he–DOsg WP

make–3sg Past

altrn

λε-γ-τον

-ποι-σεν

τον

‘He made him a legate’ (Moreas, 485)

background image

78

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

metrically constrained

0.4

0.6

0.8

1.0

1.2

1.4

1.6

frnt cns

subj/tem

factor

All Pairs
Tukey-Kramer

0.05

Figure 5.1

Comparison of the effect of metrical requirements on factors

fronted constituent vs. subject and temporal expression

Example (8) is particularly telling because in the Chronicle of Moreas

there are some possible alternatives to a form such as

ποισεν, which

would allow the postverbal placement of the pronoun without any viola-
tion of the metrical constraints. One of them is the common form
/ποικεν, which, with a following weak object pronoun gives /ποικéν
τον. The fact that these verb-forms were interchangeable is underscored
by one of Alexiou’s (who published the authoritative edition of Digene¤s
Akrite
¤s Escorial) editorial decisions. In the second hemistich of line
1596 of the epic, Alexiou emends the wording from what it is in (9) to
the order of (10) for ‘the purposes of meter’. It appears that what he was
trying to achieve here was to have the [E] of

/ποικα immediately follow

the [a] of

κακá so that the two vowels would coalesce into one syllable,

thus giving the more canonical 15 syllable line. Mackridge (1993: 336)
believes this is an erroneous emendation only because it violates his own
third rule (the pronoun should be preverbal when an object immediately
precedes the verb-pronoun complex), not because the verb-form is not a
valid part of the language. The original composer’s or author’s choice of
a differently accented form of the verb in order to keep the pronoun pre-
verbal in the context of a fronted constituent demonstrates that this con-
straint is real and not simply an epiphenomenon of the metrical require-
ments.

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

79

(9)

τρí-α κα-κà

τ7ν

-πο-κα

tria kaka

tin

Epika

syll

9-10 11-12

13

14-15-16

three evil–DO pl

she–IOsg WP

do–1sg Past

‘I did three evil things to her’ (Digene¤s, 1596)

(10)

τρíα κακà

/ποικá την

tria kaka

Epika

tin

The poem of Belisarios offers more proof that the preponderance of

preverbal pronoun placement after a fronted constituent is independent
of the constraints of the ‘politikos’ verse. In fact, the following two ex-
amples, one the first hemistich of a line (11), the other the second (12),
show that the author was able to manipulate the accenting of the verb in
order to keep the pronoun preverbal.

(11)

πλο-τον πο-λù

τοùς

/-δω-κεν

pluton poli

tus

EDokEn

syll

1-2 3-4

5

6-7-8

wealth much

they–IOpl WP

gave–3sg Past

‘He gave them much wealth’ (Belisarios, 245)

(12)

πλο-τον πο-λù

σ4

-δ-κεν

pluton poli

s

EDokEn

syll

9-10 11-12

13-14-15

wealth much

you–IOsg WP

gave–3sg Past

‘He gave you much wealth’ (Belisarios, 431)

It is obvious from these two examples that

πλοτον πολù δκεν

τους is a viable alternative for example (11) which would confirm to the
metrical constraints. The fact that the author chose against it clearly indi-
cates that the position of the pronoun is a more important concern. More
significantly, in all but one of the tokens in which the pronoun is placed
postverbally (89 out of 90), a change to preverbal placement would vio-
late the metrical constraints, as in example (13).

The above discussion presents strong evidence that the preponderance

of preverbal pronoun placement associated with factors subject and tem-
poral expression
is an epiphenomenon of the metrical constraints. This
in turn would suggest that the variation between preverbal and postver-

background image

80

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

bal pronoun placement in these contexts is actually free. On the other
hand, the postverbal tokens associated with the factor fronted constituent
appear to be such in order to fit the meter, thus suggesting that the pat-
tern of preverbal placement in this context is much more robust than was
suggested by the statistical analysis in Chapter 3.

(13)

καì %-πò τ7ν κá-ψα βλé-πεις

με

kE apo tin kapsa

vlEpis

mE

syll

1-2 3 4-5

6-7

8

and from the heat

see–2sg Pres

I–DOsg WP

altrn

καì %-πò τ7ν κá-ψα μè

βλé-πεις

‘And due to the heat, you see me …’ (Phalieros I, 484)

Since this avenue of investigation has proven fruitful thus far, it is

reasonable to ask if these considerations should be extended to the rest
of the data as well, in order to determine whether the variation seen in
association with other factors is also affected by these same require-
ments. With respect to finite verbs, however, as can be seen in Chapter
3, this is not an issue since the placement of the pronoun for other factors
is near categorical in one direction or the other. The only question that
can be raised concerns the effect of

λος, which, as was demonstrated

earlier, is associated with preverbal placement, and one may wonder
whether that pattern was also affected by metrical constraints.

However, in the case of

λος, for 10 of the 24 preverbal instances, the

pronoun position does not interact with the meter. In example (14), when
the pronoun is placed postverbally, the accent falls on the sixth syllable,
a pattern that still conforms to the constraints. The high percentage
(roughly 42%) of metrically unaffected constructions indicates that the
pattern of variation associated with

λος does not need to be revised.

(14)

-λα

τς

τà

%-νá-φε-ρε

ola

tis

ta

anafErE

syll

1-2

3

4

5-6-7-8

all–DO pl

she–IOsg WP

it–DOpl WP

report–3sg Past

altrn

-λα

%-νá-φε-ρé

της

τα

‘All (things), he reported them to her’ (Tribo¤le¤s I, 324)

On the other hand, metrical constraints do seem to have an effect on

pronoun placement in the case of the imperative verb-forms. Specifi-

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

81

cally, there is evidence that preverbal pronoun placement with impera-
tives is used in order to fit the meter. Of the 25 instances in which a pro-
noun that is the object of an imperative appears preverbally, there are
only 2 in which the meter would not have been affected by the alterna-
tive choice (Digene¤s, 800 and Spaneas, 369—see example 15). How-
ever, there are also several instances of postverbal pronouns with im-
peratives, which interact with the metrical constraints (example 16).

(15)

σúν-το-μα

τος

τò

χáρισε

sindoma

tis

to

xarisE

syll

1-2-3

4

5

6-7-8

soon

they–IOpl WP

it–DOsg WP

grant–Impv sg

altrn

σúν-το-μα

χá-ρι-σé

τοις

το

‘Give it to them in timely fashion’ (Spaneas, 369)

(16)

Πρ-τον

ε8-πé

μας

μá-να

proton

ipE

mas

mana

syll

1-2

3-4

5

6-7

First

tell–Impv sg

I–IOpl WP

mother

altrn

Πρ-τον

μας

ε-πé

μá-να

‘First, tell us …’ (Lybistros, 1449)

This evidence provides further support for the claim made at the end

of Chapter 4 that the pattern of variation in weak object pronoun place-
ment associated with imperatives is not of the same sort as that found
with finite verb-forms. Although there are some bona fide examples of
preverbal pronoun placement associated with an imperative verb-form
the overall pattern of variation in this environment is more similar to that
of non-finite forms.

The effect of the caesura

Since it has been shown that the metrical requirements of the texts play a
role in pronoun placement in some instances, one must also examine
whether the ‘strong caesura’, i.e., the rhythmic break between the two
hemistichs affects the placement of the pronoun as well. Indeed,
Mackridge (1993: 330, fn. 2) names the caesura as possibly having an
effect on the placement of the pronoun, especially when the verb com-
plex immediately follows an adverbial phrase that is syllabically long
enough to occupy the entire first hemistich.

background image

82

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Implicit in this line of reasoning is the assumption that pronoun

placement may also be prosodically constrained as pronouns cannot ap-
pear in clause-initial position because that is also the initial position of
an intonational phrase; similarly, the caesura marks the beginning of a
new intonational phrase so pronouns should not appear immediately af-
ter it. Accordingly, one would expect weak pronouns to appear after the
verb when the verb complex is immediately preceded by a caesura.

The database contains 256 instances in which the verb complex ap-

pears immediately after the caesura. In 207 of these the pronoun appears
postverbally as in example (17), while in 49 it appears preverbally as in
example (18). The || symbol indicates where the caesura occurs.

(17)

γαλνισε,

σẃνει

με

ValinisE

||

soni

mE

calm–Impv

be enough–3sg Pres

I–IOsg WP

$ κουρασιá μου
i kurasja mu
my fatigue–Nom sg

‘Calm down, I am tired enough’ (Phalieros I, 103)

(18)

τò γéνος φυσικà

μς

κáμνει

to VEnos fisika

||

mas

kamni

the race naturally

we–DOpl WP

make–3sg Pres

ν4 %γαπομε
naVapumE
to love

‘The race makes us love naturally’ (Phalieros I, 514)

A closer examination of the data, however, shows that the great ma-

jority of postverbal placement is found in environments that already fa-
vor postverbal placement (initial, reduplicated object, imperative). There
are no instances in which the caesura follows one of the members of the
factor labeled function word (e.g., an interrogative pronoun, a negative
marker etc.). When the caesura follows immediately after a constituent
which belongs to one of the factors fronted constituent, subject, or tem-
poral expression
, both preverbal and postverbal placement equally apply
(32 tokens vs. 29 tokens respectively), as examples (19) and (20) dem-
onstrate.

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

83

(19)

τò ψυχικòν

τς

/δωκεν

1 ρχος

to psixikon

||

tis

EDokEn

o arxos

the mercy

she–IOsg WP

give–3sg Past

the lord

‘The lord gave her mercy’ (Apo¤llonios, 570)

(20)

%πóφασιν

κρíνουν

την

νà 4ποθáνη

apofasin

||

krinun

tin

napoTani

decision

judge–3pl Pres

she–DOsg

to die

‘They hand down a (painful) decision, for her to die’ (Phlo¤rios, 441)

Thus, the caesura does not seem to affect the placement of the pronoun

in any significant way. As far as the length of the constituent is con-
cerned, this does not seem to affect pronoun placement either, as the
existence of constructions such as example (21)—where a preverbal
pronoun follows a hemistich-long object—indicate. More importantly,
there is no ruler by which the length of a constituent can be measured
and assigned either to the short end or the long one, as there is no scale
that can measure the weight of a NP in English. This avenue, then, has
not much to offer in the way of an explanation.

(21)

τ7ν προθυμιàν

τς νιóτης μου

tin proTimjan

tis ≠otis mu

||

the desire

of my youth

μο

λèς νà βáλω κáτω;

mu

lEs na valo kato

I–IOsg WP

tell to put down

‘Are you telling me to suppress the desire of my youth?’ (Phal. I, 56)

Pronoun placement according to chronology and geography

The fact that variation in a speech community can also be linked to a
number of social parameters, such as education, class status, political
ideology and the interpersonal networks of individual speakers has been
well established in the past 40 years, and its ramifications for language
change have been and are still being explored (Labov 1963, 1994, Fasold
1972, J. Milroy 1992, L. Milroy 1987). Unfortunately, as has been
pointed out in Chapter 2, in this case there is very little information
about the authors of the extant texts to allow a detailed sociolinguistic
study of the variation. In many cases, all we have are educated guesses

background image

84

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

based on partial information about the place of origin of a text and the
time-period in which it was composed.

In fact, Mackridge (1993: 321) pays little attention to the geographic

origin and chronology of texts as important factors in the variation. In-
stead, he claims that the more important rules enjoy a fair measure of
stability among all speakers of Greek for a period of 600 years (11

th

to

17

th

century AD), and that the geographic origin of the texts does not

seem to affect the pattern of weak object pronoun placement in any sig-
nificant fashion, although he seems to allow for the possibility of varia-
tion that depends on geography. Once again, Mackridge does not pro-
vide any specifics, but he is not alone in thinking that there is no dialect
variation with respect to pronoun placement. Horrocks (1997: 212-214)
discusses the formation of dialectal differences in the medieval period
but does not mention pronoun placement as one of the dialectal features;
and in his discussion of the language of Kornaro’s Ero¤tokritos (late 16

th

century), Horrocks (ibid: 310) claims that ‘object pronouns are regularly
placed after the verb that governs them unless there is a subordinating
conjunction or sentential operator in initial position (i.e., the normal
pattern of medieval Greek is preserved
…)’ [emphasis added].

Determining whether chronology is a factor affecting the placement of

weak object pronouns may indeed be impossible. The main reason is that
the texts have come down to us in manuscripts or printings that were
produced many years after the original composition of a work, and, as a
result, there are only approximate dates for the texts. Consequently, one
can only provide a general chronological partition for the corpus, ac-
cording to which the original compositions of the poems of Digene¤s,
Pto¤khoprodromos, Glykas and Spaneas are placed in the period before
the 14

th

century. The rest of the works comprising this database are be-

lieved to have originated between the beginning of the 14

th

and the mid-

dle of the 16

th

century, some of them being easier to date than others.

However, the small number of texts of the early period does not make
for a valid sample. Therefore, a comparison of texts based on their chro-
nology faces too many methodological problems to be informative.

On the other hand, some interesting comparisons can be made based

on the place of origin of the texts and their authors. I begin with a dis-
cussion of the facts for Cypriot Greek texts of the Later Medieval period,
which have been assumed to show the same pattern of pronoun place-
ment as texts from other regions. As has been shown already in Chapter
3, Mackridge uses an example from the chronicle of Makhairas to sup-

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

85

port his claim that ‘the pronoun is placed postverbally if the verb com-
plex is immediately preceded by the causal conjunction

διóτι’ (/Dioti/

‘because’). However, texts from the other parts of the empire show pre-
verbal pronoun placement associated with

διóτι although the number of

tokens is too small to allow one to reach a definite conclusion. Rollo
(1989) makes claims about Medieval Greek by using examples for the
Cypriot texts also; and Horrocks (1997: 282-289) does not mention pro-
noun placement as a feature of the emerging Cypriot dialect either.

However, an examination of the two Cypriot chronicles, the one by

Makhairas, and the other by Boustro¤nios, shows a pattern of pronoun
placement that is different from the other body of texts in several ways.
If one analyzes the data according to the 8 factors, which were identified
in Chapters 3 and 4 as having an affect on the placement of the pronoun
in the group of texts from elsewhere in the empire, one notices the fol-
lowing distribution (metrical requirements are not an issue here since
both chronicles are written in prose).

Table 5.1

Pronoun placement in the Cypriot chronicles

Text

Æ

Makhairas

Boustro

¤

nios

Factor

Ø

PRE V

POST V

PRE V

POST V

initial

0

103

0

105

reduplicated object

0

9

0

6

function word

43

2

58

1

fronted constituent

1

10

0

4

subject

0

23

0

6

temporal adverb

1

4

0

3

gerund

0

13

0

6

imperative

0

2

0

2

infinitive

0

0

0

0

Total

45

166

58

133

Examples (22) through (25) demonstrate the pattern of postverbal

placement while examples (26) and (27) show instances of preverbal
pronoun placement. Examples for the gerund, the imperative and the
infinitive are not given.

(22)

καì

/σφιξé

τον

kE

EsfiksE

ton

and

compell–3sg Past

he–DOsg WP

‘And he compelled him’ (Makhairas, §313)

background image

86

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(23)

Καì κενος

/δωκéν

τον

kE kinos

EDokEn

ton

and that one–Nom sg

give–3sg Past

he–DOsg WP

‘And that one gave him’ (Makhairas, §315)

(24)

;Ο κουβερνοúρης

μνυσéν

το

o kuvErnuris

EminisEn

to

the regent–Nom sg

transmit–3sg Past

it–DOsg WP

‘The regent sent word’ (Makhairas, §313)

(25)

κατà τò ... συνθιν

/δωκéν

του

τον

kata to siniTin

EDokEn

tu

ton

according to the custom

give–3sg Past

he–IOsg

he–DOsg

‘According to the … custom he gave him (the prisoner) to him’ (Mak-
hairas
, §313)

(26)

τà χαρτιá,

τà

το

/δωκεν

ta xartia

ta

tu

EDokEn

the papers–DOpl

which–DO pl

he–IOsg WP

give–3sg Past

‘The papers, which he gave to him’ (Makhairas, §313)

(27)

νà

το

δẃσουν

θáνατο

na

tu

Dosun

Tanato

Subjun

he–IOsg WP

give–3pl Pres

death–DOsg

‘Let them give him death’ (Makhairas, §313)

Furthermore, the excerpt from the Assizes that is included in Baleta’s

Anthologia shows the same pattern, namely a clear-cut distinction in
pronoun placement between function word, which is associated with
categorically preverbal pronouns, and all other factors, which are associ-
ated with categorical postverbal placement. This is significantly different
from the complex pattern of variation witnessed in the texts from other
parts of the empire according to the description given in Chapter 3. It
also explains why Rollo (1989: 136, 139), who refers mostly to the Cyp-
riot texts, takes the position that for all LMG texts, when a subject im-
mediately precedes the verb complex the pronoun is most likely to be
placed postverbally, and the fact that he does not distinguish the role of
temporal adverbs, which led him to include even adverbs in general as
environments that are associated with postverbal pronoun placement.

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

87

Troviamo in-fatti durante questo periodo adoperata l’enclisi nei se-
guenti casi:
1) quando il verbo era immediatamente preceduto dal soggetto …
4) spesso quando il verbo era preceduto da un avverbio

It should be noted, of course, that the evidence comes from two

sources only (albeit these are sizable texts and constitute most of the
Medieval Cypriot corpus), and, as such, it can only be suggestive. How-
ever, the pattern of pronoun placement in these texts is so strikingly dif-
ferent from the pattern seen before, in its clear distinctions, that it would
be a mistake not to consider it independently.

Another major difference in the pattern of variation, which seems to

depend on the text’s provenance, involves the greater occurrence of pre-
verbal placement at the beginning of the clause in the Chronicle of Mo-
reas
where the pronoun appears preverbally in initial position 17 out of
31 times, as seen in example (28).

(28)

φαíνει με,

σè

τò

γρáφω

fEni mE

sE

to

Vrafo

it seems to me

you–IOsg WP

it–DOsg WP

write–1sg Pres

‘It seems to me, I am writing it to you’ (Moreas, 1454)

What is interesting, however, is that 13 of these tokens are instances of

a parenthetical statement

σè λαλ or σè λéγω, raising the question

whether the metrical constraints are at play in some fashion in this case,
inasmuch as the two verbs differ accentually. Indeed, in 10 of these in-
stances the parenthetical comes either at end of the first hemistich—in
which case it is

σè λαλ—or at the end of the line—in which case it is

σè λéγω. However, the postverbal alternatives in these cases, namely
λéγω σε and λαλ σε, would be metrically appropriate for the end of the
first hemistich and the end of the line respectively, which means that the
metrical constraints do not play a role here. The interchangeability of
preverbal

σè λéγω and postverbal λαλ σε is shown in example (29).

(29)

κF -ντú-σαν

τον,

σè

λé-γω

kjEndisan

ton

sE

lEVo

9-10-11

12

13

14-15

dress–3pl Past

he–DOsg

you–IOsg WP

say–1sg Pres

altrn

κF -ντú-σαν

τον,

λα-λ

σε

‘And they dressed him, I tell you’ (Moreas, 985)

background image

88

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Mackridge does take notice of these parenthetical phrases in the

Chronicle of Moreas (1993: 333 fn. 2). Especially since such preverbal
pronoun placement in clause-initial position does not occur in the more
eastern texts until a much later date, Mackridge believes that these ex-
amples foreshadow the change that eventually occurred in all western
and mainland Greek dialects with respect to pronoun placement, that is
the categorical use of preverbal position when the verb is of finite form.

However, as can be seen in the appendix, texts from western Greece

that were written much later (almost three centuries later in some cases)
than the Chronicle of Moreas favor postverbal pronoun placement at the
beginning of clauses. That information is repeated here in Table 5.2 and
argues against the suggestion that what is found in Moreas is the incep-
tion of dialect differentiation. Whatever the reasons for it, preverbal pro-
noun placement in clause-initial position is a phenomenon unique to this
text.

Table 5.2

Pronoun placement in western texts according to factor initial

Factor

Æ

Initial

Text

Ø

PRE V

POST V

Aito

¤

los

0

5

Moreas

17

14

Tokkoi

0

30

Rimada

4

51

Gioustos

3

4

Defaranas

3

8

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

1

26

Total

27

138

One final irregularity in the pattern described so far is observed in

some western texts with respect to postverbal pronoun placement associ-
ated with the factor coordinating conjunction. As can be seen in Table
5.3, these texts (underlined) do not show the same tendency for postver-
bal placement as other texts do (Moreas, Tokkoi, and Tribo¤le¤s, for ex-
ample).

As was the case for clause-initial preverbal pronouns in Moreas, meter

does not seem to affect this preponderance of preverbal placement after a
coordinating conjunction. Since the three longest texts were composed
on the island of Zakynthos, it may be that this is an indication of the in-
ception of a dialectal differentiation with respect to pronoun placement

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

89

in western Greek. As was mentioned above, Modern Greek dialectal
evidence from the 20

th

century would support the claim that the pronoun

placement pattern of Standard Modern Greek (preverbal only in finite
forms) had its beginnings in mainland or western Greek, mainly because
the eastern dialects (notably Crete, Rhodes, Khios, Mykonos, the Pontic
dialects, and, of course, Cypriot—see Pangalos 1955, Kontosopoulos
1981, Drachman 1994, Oikonomide¤s 1958, Newton 1972) still maintain
vestiges of postverbal placement with finite verb-forms. Without any
corroborating evidence though, this observation about Zakynthian Greek
can only be a suggestion.

Table 5.3 Pronoun placement in western texts according to factor coordinating
conjunction

Factor

Æ

Coordinating

Text

Ø

PRE V

POST V

Aito

¤

los

3

1

Moreas

1

33

Tokkoi

1

56

Rimada

18

47

Gioustos

7

3

Depharanas

7

4

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

4

30

Total

40

174

Conclusion

In this chapter an attempt was made to identify parameters that are not
associated with the structure of the language per se, yet still affect the
pattern of variation in object pronoun position. There is, unfortunately,
limited evidence about the source of many of the documents, which in
turn reduces our ability to determine whether there is any social meaning
encoded in the variation. Nonetheless, it was clearly demonstrated that
the metrical requirements of the compositions do affect pronoun place-
ment when the factor preceding the verb complex is either subject or
temporal expression or when the verb is in the imperative. Furthermore,
it was shown that, contrary to standard opinion, the language of the Cyp-
riot chronicles is significantly different from that of the other LMG texts
with respect to pronoun placement. As a result of the investigation into
the effect that both linguistic and non-linguistic parameters have on the
pattern of weak object pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek it is

background image

90

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

necessary to correct the initial description of the phenomenon that was
provided in Chapter 3.

First, it must be noted that there are two distinct varieties of LMG with

respect to weak object pronoun placement. The variety spoken in the
area of the empire that included the Balkan Peninsula and Asia Minor,
and the variety spoken on the island of Cyprus. Byzantine LMG can be
described by the following statements:
1.

When a finite verb is in clause-initial position, when it is immedi-
ately preceded by a reduplicated object other than the adjective
λος, or when it is immediately preceded by the negative marker
ο, the weak object pronoun almost always appears after the verb.

2.

When a finite verb is immediately preceded by a subject or a tempo-
ral expression, placement of the weak object pronoun varies freely
between preverbal and postverbal position.

3.

When a finite verb is immediately preceded by a fronted constituent
or a function word, its weak object pronoun almost always appears
before the verb.

4.

If the form of the verb is imperative, the pronoun is placed over-
whelmingly but not categorically in postverbal position.

5.

If the form of the verb is non-finite (gerund, infinitive), the pronoun
is placed categorically in postverbal position.

Chapter 3

Final Pattern

function word

fronted constitu-
ent

subject, temporal
expression

reduplicated ob-
ject

initial,

ο

function word, fronted
constituent

subject, temporal ex-
pression

imperative verb-form

initial,

ο

, reduplicated

object, non-finite verb-
form

Figure 5.2

Diagrammatic comparison of the initial and final description of

weak object pronoun placement in LMG

background image

Non-linguistic Parameters

91

The description of the variation has been significantly changed from

that given at the end of Chapter 3 even though it still contains five
statements. The new description covers the entire range of possible verb-
forms: finite, non-finite and the imperative. Moreover, the description of
the variation associated with finite verb-forms has been simplified from
five statements to three since it was possible to demonstrate a) that redu-
plicated objects do not have a significantly different effect on pronoun
placement than when the verb is clause-initial; and b) that the number of
postverbal tokens of pronoun placement associated with the factor
fronted constituent is most likely due to metrical constraints. The dia-
gram in Figure 5.2 provides a visual comparison of the two descriptions.

Later Medieval Cypriot Greek, on the other hand, can be described by

a single statement: Weak object pronouns are placed postverbally except
when the verb is immediately preceded by a function word. In the next
chapter, the clearer picture of the facts that we have obtained for Byzan-
tine LMG will provide the standard by which to evaluate possible expla-
nations regarding this phenomenon. The Cypriot pattern will play a cru-
cial role in understanding the variation from a diachronic perspective.

background image

92

6

Previous Proposals

The previous chapters have presented in full detail the complexity of the
variation in the placement of weak object pronouns. Although it was
shown that this complexity can be reduced if we take into account the
various parameters that affect pronoun position, the overall pattern is
still intricate enough that there is no obvious generalization to account
for it. In this chapter, I present several accounts proposed by other re-
searchers who have dealt with this phenomenon. In the first section, pro-
posals specifically about Later Medieval Greek pronoun placement are
considered, but it is demonstrated that all of them are flawed in one way
or another. In the second section, I evaluate the analyses that have been
suggested for similar phenomena in other languages, most notably Old
Spanish. These too however, are not able to handle the LMG facts. Since
all of the above proposals attempt to explain weak object pronoun
placement by making appeals to phrase structure, the third and final sec-
tion of the chapter examines the viability of two other approaches, one
that is prosodically based, and a morphological one. Unfortunately, these
do not fare any better than the syntactic accounts. In the conclusion, I
identify the two key characteristics of the variation in LMG that make it
so challenging to explain.

Previous explanations for the LMG facts

In this section, I summarize and evaluate several previous attempts at
explaining the facts about weak object pronoun placement in Later Me-
dieval Greek. In addition to Mackridge’s two papers (1993 and 1995),
there have been four other attempts to deal with the LMG facts specifi-

background image

Previous Proposals

93

cally (Horrocks 1990 and 1997, Philippaki-Warburton 1995 and Con-
doravdi and Kiparsky 2001). None of these accounts presents a complete
explanation of the phenomenon because they do not cover the facts
about non-finite verb-forms and the imperative. I will show, however,
that in addition to being descriptively incomplete, these proposals also
face other problems that render them incorrect.

Horrocks (1990)

Horrocks’ article is an examination of clitic placement throughout the
history of the Greek language. In the section concerning LMG, he pro-
vides an account of how the pronoun position changed from postverbal
to preverbal by linking it to the change of

να [ina] to νá [na]:

Evidently as the phonologically and syntactically independent status
of

νá as a conjunction declined … it came to be felt more and more

as a part of the verbal complex, and as this happened, the clitic was
naturally drawn to second position within that complex, in accordance
with the pattern that we have seen already (1990: 49-50).

Horrocks thus maintains that the pronouns are still appearing in second

position as they did in Classical Greek (cf. Chapter 2), with the excep-
tion that the domain in which second position is determined has changed
from S to VP (or ‘verbal complex’ according to him). Unfortunately,
however, Horrocks’ account misrepresents the facts somewhat. For ex-
ample, as has been discussed already in Chapter 4, Horrocks claimed
that the negative marker

ο draws the pronoun to a preverbal position,

which is at odds with the present description of the facts, and seems to
be the result of his not recognizing the effect of the examples with

ν

ο. Furthermore, he states that ‘cases where the pronoun precedes … are
vastly outnumbered by the examples where the pronoun follows’, which
again is contrary both to Mackridge’s account, and to the results of this
study. Since, however, Horrocks’ explanation has more the air of a sug-
gestion for further research rather than a conclusive statement, his hy-
pothesis should not be critiqued too rigorously.

Philippaki-Warburton (1995)

According to Philippaki-Warburton, her analysis of weak object pronoun
or clitic (as she calls them) placement in Later Medieval Greek is based
on the description of the facts given by both Horrocks (1990) and
Mackridge (1993). This is a curious decision since, as we have seen,

background image

94

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

there is serious disagreement between these two proposals which she
does not resolve in any way. Nonetheless, Philippaki-Warburton pro-
poses the structure seen in (1) below, which she claims describes the
clause in Hellenistic Greek. However, since she is basing her analysis
mostly on Mackridge’s description of weak object pronoun placement,
the structure she proposes can only really apply to Later Medieval
Greek.

(1)

[

CP

C [

MP

M [

NegP

Neg [

FutP

Fut [

T/AgrP

Cl [

T/AgrP

T/Agr [

VP

]]]]]]]

In this structure, it is assumed that the clitic

14

(shown in bold charac-

ters) is adjoined to the Tense-Agreement Phrase. MP stands for Mood
Phrase and, according to Philippaki-Warbuton, can be occupied either by
the Indicative Morpheme (which is non-overt), the subjunctive marker
νá, or the Imperative Morpheme, which does have an overt manifesta-
tion.

According to this analysis, in a sentence such as (2) the Indicative

Morpheme of the second clause,

πáτησé τον, although it is not overt,

occupies the M-node. The verb is drawn to this position in order to re-
ceive indicative morphology (otherwise it would not be licensed) and as
a result appears in initial position. As a consequence, the pronoun order
is postverbal.

(2)

/σφαλε,

πáτησé

τον

EsfalE

EpatisE

ton

err–3sg Past

break–3sg Past

he–DOsg WP

‘He erred, he broke him (the oath)’ (Moreas, 80)

(3)

δèν μς

δíδει

DEn

mas

DiDi

not

we–IOpl WP

give–3sg Pres

‘He does not give us’ (Aito¤los, 174)

In the case of an imperative, it is the Imperative Morpheme that draws

the verb to the M-node, thus making the placement of the pronoun post-

verbal. When the sentence is negative, on the other hand, the negative

marker (e.g.,

δéν) is assumed to occupy the Neg

0

position and to block

the movement of the verb to any higher position thus yielding sentences

background image

Previous Proposals

95

such as (3). The future marker

θá has the same effect of blocking, while

the subjunctive marker

νá is considered to occupy the M-node and thus

the verb cannot move into that position.

Finally, with respect to the gerund, Philippaki-Warburton claims that

the ending –

οντας /ondas/ is not located in the M-node but is a Case

feature, which attributes a [– Tense/Agr] value to the gerund. The main
motivation for this analysis of the gerund is to capture the fact that the
gerund may be negated by the negative marker

μ in Standard Modern

Greek. Since the ending –

οντας is not in the M-node, the verb does not

need to be raised over the NegP-node and the sequence negative–gerund
is a viable one (example 4). This is in direct contrast with the case of an
imperative where the relevant morpheme is in the M-node and thus, the
verb needs to be raised over the NegP-node. This however cannot hap-
pen since Neg is occupied by

μ, which explains the ungrammaticality

of example (5).

(4)

Μ7

γνωρíζοντας τ7ν %λθεια

mi

Vnorizondas

tin aliTja

not

know–Gerund

the truth–DOsg

‘Not knowing the truth’

(5)

*Μ7 δσε
mi

DosE

not

give–Impv

‘Don’t give!’

However, by assuming that there is no verb movement in the case of a

gerundive construction, Philippaki-Warburton now needs a different
mechanism to account for the postverbal placement of pronouns in this
environment. Her solution is to postulate that in the absence of
[Tense/Agr] features, the clitics are not drawn to the adjunction site but
remain in the VP. This does succeed in producing postverbal pronoun
placement with gerunds as in example (6), but the postulate is not inde-
pendently motivated.

Philippaki-Warburton’s account is incomplete because it does not of-

fer an analysis of infinitival constructions. It would seem though that the
explanation she provides for the gerunds could be extended to handle
these facts as well, if one assumes that the infinitive morphemes are

background image

96

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Case features that do not appear under M but simply mark the infinitive
as [–Tense/Agr].

(6)

γυρεúοντá

την

VirEvonda

tin

search–Gerund

she–DOsg WP

‘Searching for her’ (Tribo¤le¤s I, 73)

Unfortunately, this account faces many problems, which cannot be

easily resolved. The most serious of these is the fact that the only moti-
vation for verb movement to the M-node is the presence of the Indicative
Morpheme. However, Philippaki-Warburton also claims that in negative
declarative sentences the presence of

δéν blocks the verb from moving

to the M-node. If this is so, then there is no way for the verb to receive
the indicative morphology in these constructions. Philippaki-Warburton
realizes this problem and claims that since the Indicative Morpheme is
not morphophonologically overt and the verb-form is morphologically
complete, the structure is grammatical even if verb movement does not
take place. What she fails to realize, however, is that this new assump-
tion removes the motivation for postulating a non-overt Indicative Mor-
pheme in the first place. Either way, one of the two conditions has to be
stipulated and this is undesirable.

Furthermore, it can be shown that this account makes the wrong pre-

dictions about pronoun placement in several instances, and thus lacks
observational adequacy. First, it predicts the wrong pronoun placement
in an indicative sentence with an interrogative pronoun such as

τíς, as in

example (7):

(7)

τìς

σè

παíρνει

tis

sE

pErni

who–Nom sg

you–DOsg WP

take–3sg Pres

‘Who is taking you?’ (Achilleid, 1697)

One would expect that the analysis of this sentence would be identical

to that of example (2) with the exception that in this case the interroga-
tive pronoun (a wh-phrase) is occupying the SpecC-node. According to
the discussion above, the verb should still move to M in order to receive
indicative morphology, which would yield the sentence

τìς παíρνει σε.

background image

Previous Proposals

97

However, postverbal pronoun placement with wh-phrases is very rare as
it occurs only twice in 166 instances (cf. Table A.14 in the appendix).

Second, this account is not able to capture the distinction between

factors reduplicated object and fronted constituent with respect to pro-
noun placement. It is reasonable to assume that in both of these cases the
constituent cannot block verb movement to the M-node in any way. Ac-
cordingly, Philippaki-Warburton’s account predicts that in both situa-
tions the verb should be able to move to the M-node and that the place-
ment of the pronoun should be postverbal. However, as it was clearly
demonstrated in Chapter 3, only reduplicated objects are associated with
postverbal placement; fronted constituents affect preverbal placement.

Third, this account predicts that when the form of the verb is impera-

tive, pronouns can only appear postverbally. This is obviously the wrong
prediction since even according to Mackridge’s description there can be
preverbal placement of weak object pronouns with an imperative.

Finally, there is no way in this analysis to allow for the fact that the

position of the pronoun can also vary between preverbal and postverbal
when the complex is immediately preceded by a subject or a temporal
expression. For these reasons Philippaki-Warburton’s (1995) account
cannot be sustained.

Horrocks (1997)

In his second attempt to explain the pattern of pronoun placement in
Later Medieval Greek, Horrocks proposes that there were two different
categories of conjunctions and complementizers in the language: tradi-
tional ones (

τι /oti/, διóτι /Dioti/, and ε8 /i/), and their modern coun-

terparts, which were the result of aphaeresis (

πẃς /pos/, ποú /pu/, νá

/na/,

ν /an/). Each of these classes was associated with a different

structure. The ‘traditional’ structure is seen in (8a), and the ‘modern’ one
in (8b). The most notable difference between the two is that the ‘tradi-
tional’ structure has a node ConP, headed by enclitic connectives (as
these were known for Classical Greek), while the ‘modern’ structure has
the node ClP which, according to Horrocks, is the post classical devel-
opment of ConP and the obligatory position for clitic pronouns.

(8a)

[

CP

C [

ConP

Con [

S

[

NP

subject ] [

VP

V + Cl ]]]]

(8b)

[

CP

C [

ClP

Cl [

S

[

NP

subject ] [

VP

V ]]]]

background image

98

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

According to Horrocks’s new analysis, when a traditional comple-

mentizer, such as

τι, occupies the C-node, the V + Cl complex can re-

main in the VP, or move to Con. When there is a modern complemen-
tizer, such as

ν in that position, the verb is obligatorily shifted to Cl in

the order clitic + verb. Furthermore, if C is empty in either case then V +
Cl—optionally, or V—obligatorily—will move into that position. Both
of these structures would thus yield the verb–clitic order when the verb
is in initial position in a main clause. Furthermore, Horrocks (ibid: 210)
asserts that

The presence of a preposed interrogative or focal phrase within CP
had the same effect as the presence of an overt complementizer in C,
i.e., to force the verb to appear after the clitic pronouns in ClP. Pre-
posed topics, however, were clearly placed outside the clause struc-
ture proper (being adjoined, say, to CP), so that the true clause-initial
position remained free to accept V, thereby effecting the normal
main-clause order (i.e., V + clitic) in these cases.

The first problem with this analysis is the spurious ambiguity in the

case of main clauses: it is impossible to know whether such sentences
are generated by structure (8a) or (8b). But perhaps this is to be expected
in a transitional period such as the one that Horrocks describes. A more
serious problem arises, however, when one considers that there is no
reason why a phonological change, such as the aphaeresis of initial vow-
els, should generate a new syntactic structure. This is especially true in
the case of the complementizer

ν. Smyth (1956) mentions that ν (an

Attic contraction of

áν) is found as a conditional particle in later Attic

writers and Plato. Thus, it is probably not a product of aphaeresis and
should be considered a ‘traditional’ complementizer. According to Hor-
rocks’ proposal, then, it should be associated with postverbal placement
in Later Medieval Greek. None of these predictions corresponds to the
actual facts. In addition, given Horrocks’ (1997: 208-209) analysis of

νá

as the result of aphaeresis from

να (after an irregular accent shift to

Jνá—although, Mendez-Dosuna (2000) argues that να was never ac-
cented so there could not have been any accent shift), one would expect
that

να would indeed belong to the group of traditional complementiz-

ers, and that it would be associated with postverbal pronoun placement.
However, Table A.5 in the appendix shows that in all 25 instances in
which

να is used in the database, the pronoun appears preverbally.

background image

Previous Proposals

99

Another problem with Horrocks’ account arises with his assertion that

the distinction between postverbal and preverbal placement is based on
whether the element immediately preceding the verb complex is a focal
phrase or a topic. However, it was shown in Chapter 4 that neither em-
phasis, nor discourse constraints seem to affect the placement of the pro-
noun in Later Medieval Greek.

Furthermore, Horrocks claims that ‘The complementizer

νá [na], how-

ever, ultimately came to function as a subjunctive marker, and in this
reduced role formed (along with negative particles and clitic pronouns)
part of a word-like

15

complex’ in a structure such as the one below (8c).

(8c)

[

CP

πẃς, etc. [

MP

νá [

NegP

δéν/μ(ν) [

ClP

Cl [

S

NP [

VP

V ]]]]]]

This is essentially the same structure that Philippaki-Warburton (1995)

proposes; in fact, Horrocks makes reference to Philippaki-Warburton
(1990) on which her 1995 article is based. The one crucial difference in
(8c) is that the position of the ClP is fixed above the S-node, and not
adjoined to TnsP as it is in (1). Accordingly, Horrocks (ibid: 212) men-
tions that ‘weak pronouns are therefore enclitic on the verb only in the
case of imperatives and gerunds, and these remain the only verb-forms
still subject to the rule of verb-preposing to C in standard speech.’ This
clearly clashes with Philippaki-Warburton’s claim that the gerund has to
appear below the NegP-node in order to capture the facts presented in
examples (4) and (5), and Horrocks does not offer an alternative way of
deriving the order negative–gerund.

Finally, and beyond the details of the analysis, Horrocks does not pro-

vide any real motivation for the existence of the three separate struc-
tures, and does not present any argumentation for the causes of the
changes. One has to wonder, especially, why the loss of an initial vowel
(as in the aphaeresis he proposes) would be associated with the restruc-
turing of phrase structure. Thus, even Horrocks’ latest analysis faces
many problems, some of which seem insurmountable.

Condoravdi and Kiparsky (2001)

The most recent proposal attempting to explain the pattern of weak ob-
ject pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek is that of Condoravdi
and Kiparsky (2001), which, it is safe to say, is the most sophisticated of
the group. Their investigation of this phenomenon is part of a larger ob-
jective, namely to understand the variation in phrase structure and word
order among Greek dialects. The facts about pronoun placement in the

background image

100

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

various dialects are used as diagnostics of clause structure. Since the
authors are also interested in tracing the historical development of this
dialectal variation, the discussion of Later Medieval Greek pronoun
placement occupies a central place in their argument.

Condoravdi and Kiparsky begin by proposing that the structure of the

clause in Later Medieval Greek was as follows:

(9)

[

CP

{Wh} [

C'

[

S

P

{FocXP, Emph Neg} [

S

'

[

S

0

{Neg, Mod}] [

TnsP

C l

[

TnsP

[

Tns0

V

j

] [

VP

t

j

]]]]]]]

The key aspect in which Condoravdi and Kiparsky’s account differs

from those before it is that they treat LMG weak pronouns as X

max

encli-

tics (following Halpern and Fontana 1994), which undergo the rule of
Prosodic Inversion, a mechanism proposed by Halpern (1995), and
which will be discussed below. Variation in weak object pronoun place-
ment occurs because ‘Clitics prosodically subcategorize for a prosodic
host to their left within the same CP’ (Condoravdi and Kiparsky: 6) but
‘[when] there is no available prosodic host to their left, they encliticize
onto the adjacent word on their right’ (ibid: 7). The authors elect to
combine a proposal about the structure of the clause with the operation
of a phonological rule because they are convinced that a strictly struc-
tural account cannot explain the fact that wh-phrases and complementiz-
ers pattern in the same way. The variation observed between the factors
reduplicated object, fronted constituent, subject and temporal expres-
sion
, depends on whether the constituent that immediately precedes the
verb complex is a topic or a focus element. For example, a reduplicated
object for Condoravdi and Kiparsky is a topic element and as such it is
adjoined to CP or

SP. Furthermore, ‘Adjoined constituents are not visi-

ble for cliticization’ (Condoravdi and Kiparsky: 7), a constraint that they
cannot independently motivate, unfortunately. As a result, when the verb
immediately follows a reduplicated object, there is no available host for
the pronoun and so it appears postverbally. On the other hand,

λος is a

focused element, and as such occupies the Spec

SP position and can

serve as a host for a weak pronoun.

This proposal is an improvement over previous analyses because it

manages to account for a wider range of facts and its theoretical basis is
more sound. However, it is based on a very controversial and ultimately
false assumption. The evidence concerning Later Medieval Greek weak
object pronouns does not in any way support the view that they are en-

background image

Previous Proposals

101

clitics, which can attach to any prosodic host to their left. In fact, all the
available textual evidence indicates that the only possible prosodic host
for such elements is the verb, whether it appears to the left or the right of
the pronoun. One argument supporting this claim is that, very often,
when a vowel-final pronoun appears before a vowel-initial verb, the or-
thography indicates that sandhi phenomena occur between the pronoun
and the verb. Furthermore, the pronoun–verb string is frequently written
as one word in these situations, as can be seen in example (10) where

τó

and

%πγγελαν are written as one word, ταπγγελαν.

(10)

τòν %μιρν

ταπγγελαν

ton amiran

tapigElan

the emir–IOsg

it–DOsg WP + announce 3pl Past

‘They announced it to the emir’ (Phlo¤rios, 1698)

Considerations of accent placement also suggest that preverbal pro-

nouns are attached to the verb and not to the word that appears to their
left. As has been mentioned already in Chapters 2 and 4, when a propar-
oxytone word is followed by an enclitic, a second accent is written on
the ultima. This is the case in all Later Medieval Greek texts when the
pronoun is possessive and clearly has a prosodic host to its left as in ex-
ample (11). On the other hand, there are several instances where a prop-
aroxytone word is followed by a weak pronoun without a second stress
accent. In these cases, it is the pronoun that bears the written accent.

(11)

τò φλáμουρó

τους

/ριξαν

to flamuro

tus

Eriksan

the flag–DO sg

they–PSpl

throw–3pl Past

‘They threw their flag’ (Tokkoi, 1740)

(12)

οJ γγελοι

τà

πρασιν

i aNgEli

ta

pirasin

the angels–Nom pl

they–DOsg WP

take–3pl Past

‘The angels took them’ (Thre¤nos, 140)

If Condoravdi and Kiparsky’s assumption that weak object pronouns

in Later Medieval Greek are indeed enclitics were correct, then one
would expect that the operation of the secondary stress rule would be
reflected in the orthography, i.e., that the sentence would be

οJ γγελοí

background image

102

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

τα πρασιν. All of these considerations indicate that in LMG—just as
in Standard Modern Greek—the prosodic host of weak object pronouns
is always the verb, a position which has been adopted by many others,
including Joseph (1978/1990), Horrocks (1990, 1997), Philippaki-War-
burton (1995), and Mackridge (1993, 1995, 2000). As will be discussed
in the following section, weak pronouns that are always attached to the
same host are classified by Halpern (1995: 63) as non-directional clitics,
and as such undergo a very different form of Prosodic Inversion. The
effect of that type of rule will be analyzed in more detail below, but it
will suffice to state here that, if Later Medieval Greek weak object pro-
nouns are treated as non-directional clitics, Halpern’s Prosodic Inversion
Rule predicts that when a verb is clause-initial its object pronoun should
precede it, a clearly undesirable result.

Furthermore, even if the authors’ hypothesis that the weak object pro-

nouns are enclitics could be reconciled with the textual evidence, there
are still some problematic aspects of their account that persist. First, one
must consider their claim that the peculiar pronoun placement pattern
associated with a reduplicated preverbal adjective

λος can be ac-

counted for if

λος is treated as a ‘focused floated quantifier in Spec SP’

(Condoravdi and Kiparsky: 24), instead of being a topic adjoined to

SP

which they consider to be the case for all other reduplicated objects. Un-
fortunately, this analysis cannot explain why in examples (13) and (14)
below, which are almost identical, the pronoun appears in different posi-
tions. In other words, why is

λους in (13) a topic and not a focus ele-

ment? There does not seem to be a clear answer to this question.
Mackridge (2000: 137) agrees that the available data about

λος cannot

be captured by a single rule because there seem to be an abundance of
counterexamples that would disprove any hypothesis concerning this
problem.

(13)

τοùς νδρες, … λους

κíνησéν

τους

tus AndrEs

olus

EkinisEn

tus

the men, …

all–DOpl

move–3sg Past

he–DOpl WP

‘The men, he deported them all’ (Tokkoi, 1010)

(14)

λα

τà

%φηγθη

ola

ta

afiViTi

all–DOpl

they–DOpl WP

narrate–3sg Past

‘He narrated everything’ (Tokkoi, 1090)

background image

Previous Proposals

103

Another problem for the Condoravdi and Kiparsky account stems from

the pattern of pronoun placement associated with the negative marker
ο. As was first observed in Mackridge (1993) and confirmed in this
study, the pronouns are always postverbal in this case except when

ο

itself is preceded by the conditional

ν, in which circumstance the pro-

nouns appear almost categorically preverbal (as can be seen in Table A.9
in the appendix). In their account, this negative marker (as opposed to
δéν and μ) ‘triggers V-to-C movement’ and, as a result, the verb ap-
pears to the left of the pronoun. The authors support this claim with
cross-linguistic evidence from Germanic languages, where negation and
wh-movement are the original triggers for V-to-C movement. However,
this cannot explain in any fashion why the same element does not trigger
V-to-C movement when it follows the conditional conjunction

ν.

Yet another point of contention arises out of the interaction of the

Condoravdi and Kiparsky proposal that weak object pronouns are ad-
joined to TnsP and their stipulation that adjoined elements cannot serve
as hosts. Let’s consider what this would mean for examples such as (15)
in which both the indirect object and the direct object are weak pro-
nouns. According to them, the first pronoun (

το) encliticizes to the

subjunctive marker

νá, and then the second pronoun (τá) encliticizes to

this new host (

νà το). But if both pronouns have to be adjoined to TnsP

then the second encliticization cannot occur according to Condoravdi
and Kiparsky’s adjunction constraint. As an adjoined constituent,

το is

no longer visible to encliticization. One way to avoid this problem is to
assume that, in such examples, the two pronouns are adjoined in one
position. However, as has been already mentioned in the second section
of Chapter 4, there do exist a few examples of ditransitive constructions
in which the pronouns appear on either side of the verb (cf. example 13
in that chapter). The hypothesis that weak object pronouns are adjoined
as a single unit predicts that this would be impossible and is thus clearly
defeated by the existence of such ‘split’ pronoun constructions.

(15)

νà

το

τà

πληρẃσουν

na

tu

ta

plirosun

Subjun

he–IOsg WP

it–DOpl WP

pay–3pl Pres

‘To pay them to him’ (Rimada, 300)

Finally, Condoravdi and Kiparsky’s account does not address the pat-

tern of pronoun placement for non-finite forms or the imperative. Weak

background image

104

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

object pronoun constructions with such verb-forms, however, present
problems for their analysis. For example, their proposal cannot explain
why pronouns are always postverbal with a gerund or an infinitive. They
would have to assert that in all such cases, there is no available host to
the left of the pronoun, which is simply not true. It is equally difficult to
explain the variation in the case of the imperative. If one were to assume
that the imperative form occupies the same place in the structure as do
the indicative and the subjunctive, then it would be difficult to explain
why postverbal pronoun placement is so much more prevalent with the
imperative than it is with those other finite verb-forms. On the other
hand, if one assumes (along with Rivero and Terzi 1995) that the im-
perative occupies C, it is difficult to justify the presence of preverbal
pronouns at all. In general, the facts concerning the imperative pose a
serious challenge for any structural explanation that claims to account
for the entire pattern of pronoun placement in LMG.

Syntactic approaches to similar phenomena in other lan-
guages

Variation between preverbal and postverbal placement of weak object
pronouns is not a phenomenon unique to Later Medieval Greek. In fact,
it is the Romance Languages that are best known for this alternation,
described by the Tobler-Mussafia Law (named after its two main con-
tributors, Tobler 1889 for Old French, and Mussafia 1898 for Old Ital-
ian). According to Wanner (1987: 68) this law ‘determines pronominal
clitic position in Old Romance; it is always next to the verb, and variably
before or after it depending on the syntactic context, in particular pro-
hibiting sentence initial clitic pronouns.’ In the past decade, there have
been several proposals, that have sought to account for this phenomenon
with a syntactic explanation, mainly in the GB/Minimalist framework.
Given the similarity between the data from Later Medieval Greek and
the description of the Tobler-Moussafia law, it is appropriate to evaluate
these proposals in order to determine if they can account for the LMG
facts as well.

Weak object pronoun placement in Old Spanish

Wanner (1991)

Wanner (1991) addresses the Tobler-Mussafia Law in the case of Old
Spanish.

16

He proposes that Old Spanish clitics should be treated as

background image

Previous Proposals

105

Heads (i.e., belonging to the category N), and that they move from
within the VP to a position he calls AGR

int

—where internal arguments

are checked. Postverbal pronoun placement occurs when the verb be-
longs to a main clause, which has an empty Comp position, and the verb
is able to move there, thus yielding the order verb–clitic. On the other
hand, when the verb belongs to a subordinate clause, its weak object
pronoun(s) will always appear in preverbal position, because (according
to Wanner) in such clauses the Comp position is always occu-
pied—usually by a complementizer—which makes the position unavail-
able for the verb. The same holds for main clauses, such as wh-ques-
tions, which also have a full Comp position. The two possibilities are
illustrated in (16a) and (16b).

(16a)

[

CP

[

C

V

1

] [

IP

[

I'

Polarity [

I'

[

AGRint

Clitic

2

] [

I'

AGRext [

VP

t

1

t

2

]]]]]]

(16b)

[

CP

[

C

wh-phrase] [

IP

[

I'

Polarity [

I'

[

AGRint

Clitic] [

I'

AGRext [

VP

V…t]]]]]]

As to the reason why the verb undergoes this movement, Wanner re-

mains slightly enigmatic. He does not address this question directly,
however, the quote provided below seems to indicate that this movement
occurs precisely in order to avoid a clitic–verb sequence next to a CP
boundary.

T[obler]M[ussafia] (as a corollary of its underlying mechanism of V-
to-C raising) makes crucial reference to [

CP

. In the context of [

CP

, only

/V–cl/ sequences will be observed. The nonoccurrence of /cl–V/ se-
quences in given string types in Old Spanish can thus be taken as an
indication of the presence of a left-hand CP boundary immediately
before the {V, cl} group (Wanner 1993: 336).

The preverbal placement of weak object pronouns that is associated

with some other element immediately preceding the verb complex, is
explained by assuming the presence of this constituent under ‘polarity’
which has the effect of breaking up the string CP boundary–clitic–verb.
This mechanism, however, would over-generate preverbal pronouns
when the element immediately preceding the verb complex is a subject,
a fact that Wanner is clearly aware of.

background image

106

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

The placement of the relevant subject NPs to the left of the matrix CP
implies discourse dynamic prominence of a topical nature, predicting
that only emphasized topical subject NPs would cause this enclitici-
zation. But this is not the case; rather the examples show a predilec-
tion for nonemphatic topical subject to have this effect, while all fo-
cused and many emphasized topical subjects combine with expected
proclisis … (ibid: 342).

In order to account for the free variation between postverbal and pre-

verbal placement in this environment, Wanner (ibid: 343) is forced to
postulate ‘a separate minor process of encliticization in the context of an
immediately preceding subject NP’, which he describes as a stylistic
operation that is optional. No further motivation is provided for this op-
eration. In fact, in a later paper, Wanner (1996) recognizes that both
mechanisms proposed in the above analysis lack independent justifica-
tion, and this weakens his account. As will be discussed below, Fontana
(1993, 1996, 1997) also maintains that Old Spanish variation in weak
object pronoun placement is not reason enough to justify invoking major
syntactic mechanisms such as verb movement. Finally, if we were to use
this approach with the LMG data, we would have to postulate two sty-
listic rules—one for subject, and one for temporal expressions. The re-
sult of this approach, however, would not be any more illuminating than
Mackridge’s list.

Fontana (1993)

Fontana (1993) presents a different account for the Old Spanish data.
Following Taylor (1990), Fontana accepts a three-part classification of
morphemes that are usually covered by the label ‘clitic’:

head-attaching elements which could be considered a subclass of af-
fix.
phonologically ‘weak’ elements that have the same syntactic distri-
bution as their full counterparts.
phonologically ‘weak’ elements whose distribution is governed by
idiosyncratic operations of phrasal attachment.

Next, based on a comparison between the Homeric Greek facts that

Taylor presents and the facts of Old Spanish, as well as the observation
that weak object pronouns could be separated from the verb, Fontana
argues that the weak pronouns in Old Spanish should be treated as ‘true

background image

Previous Proposals

107

clitics’, or, in Klavan’s (1982, 1985) terminology, as ‘phrasal affixes’.

17

As has already been implied during the discussion of the Condoravdi and
Kiparsky (2001) proposal, Fontana adopts the position that the Old
Spanish weak pronouns are X

max

elements that adjoin to the left periph-

ery of IP. This is demonstrated in (17).

(17)

[

CP

[

C'

C [

IP

[

NP

Clitic NP] [

I'

VP]]]]

According to the above structure, the default position for the weak

pronoun is preverbal. The reason, according to Fontana, that the weak
pronouns can also appear after the verb, is that Old Spanish is a ‘verb-
second’ language of the ‘symmetric’ type, and this results in occasional
postverbal pronoun placement through the interaction of several other
independent mechanisms. A verb-second language of the symmetric type
is a language in which the verb must appear in second position not only
in main clauses but also in subordinate clauses, as is the case in Yiddish
and Old Icelandic (see Santorini 1994, Thráinsson 1986). In the GB pro-
gram, these languages are distinguished from ‘asymmetric verb-second’
languages such as German and Dutch. The key difference between the
two types is that in asymmetric languages the verb is considered to move
into Comp, whereas the verb in symmetric languages is considered to
move into Infl.

However, as can be determined by an examination of the structure in

(17), verb movement to Infl still leaves the weak pronouns in preverbal
position. In order to achieve postverbal placement of the pronouns when
the verb is in clause-initial position, Fontana resorts to employing the
mechanism of Narrative Inversion. This is a stylistic operation that, un-
der certain specific discourse requirements, moves the verb into the
complementizer position. Narrative Inversion has received the greatest
attention in Icelandic and Old Icelandic especially. According to
Sigur∂sson (1990) the mechanism works as follows:

Declarative V1 orders in main clauses are, in general, prompted by
strong discourse cohesion … they cannot initiate the discourse … For
NI, a high degree of subject topicality is important … Thus in a count
I have made in four Icelandic narratives, it turned out that
V[erb]S[econd] order is more common in main clauses with first-
(and second-) person pronoun subjects than in sentences with third-

background image

108

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

person pronoun subjects, for which in turn, VS is significantly more
frequent than in sentences with full NP subjects.

Fontana discusses a series of examples from Old Spanish, which pro-

vide evidence that when a verb appears in clause-initial position, the
discourse environment fits the description of Narrative Inversion. This
allows him to move the verb to the complementizer position independ-
ently of clitic placement. According to Fontana, this gives his analysis
the advantage that it does not need to postulate a mechanism simply to
account for the Tobler-Moussafia facts, which he considers a minor part
of the language of Old Spanish. The following example (adapted from
Fontana 1993: 136) demonstrates how the movement of the verb to C
results in postverbal placement of the pronoun. In the following exam-
ples, the pronoun is shown in bold, while the verb and the traces linked
to it are shown in italics.

(18)

[

CP

[

C'

[

C

respondio] [

IP

[

NP

=les [

NP

el]] [

I'

t [

VP

t [

V'

que lo non farie

t]]]]]]

‘He responded to them that he would not do it’

The other case where pronouns appear postverbally is when a subject

immediately precedes the verb complex. In order to employ the Narra-
tive Inversion mechanism in such constructions, Fontana assumes that
these subjects do not occupy the SpecIP position but are instead adjoined
outside CP. These NPs are treated as adjuncts, which are coindexed with
a pro element in SpecIP, that receives Case and the T-role that are as-
signed by Infl and the predicate respectively. In other words, sentences
that show subject–verb–clitic order are treated as a type of left disloca-
tion (cf. example 19, also from Fontana 1993: 179). However, Fontana
does not address Wanner’s (1991) justified reluctance to topicalize sub-
jects without demonstrating an accompanying discourse effect.

(19)

[el

i

[

CP

[

C'

[

C

dexo] [

IP

[

NP

=lo [

NP

pro

i

]] [

I'

en aquel lugar]]]]]

‘And he left it in that place’

Although Fontana’s proposal to separate the facts about weak object

pronoun placement from the mechanism of verb movement is an in-
triguing one, this analysis cannot be applied in the case of Later Medie-
val Greek for a multitude of reasons. First, as has already been men-

background image

Previous Proposals

109

tioned during the discussion of the Condoravdi and Kiparsky (2001)
proposal, the Greek pronouns cannot be treated in the same way that
Fontana treats the Old Spanish pronouns because the former are not X

max

enclitics (pace Condoravdi and Kiparsky) and because there are no in-
stances of interpolation in LMG. Furthermore, there is no evidence that
Later Medieval Greek is a verb-second language. A random sample of
20 consecutive main clauses taken from 15 different texts, shows that the
verb in LMG does not have to appear in second position. The results in
Table 6.1 show that verb-initial placement is just as frequent as verb-
second placement (analytic results by text are available in the appendix,
Table A.14).

Table 6.1

Possible verb positions in Later Medieval Greek

Verb Position

Initial

Second

Medial

Final

Total

126

133

30

11

As Fontana (1993: 92) correctly remarks, however, a simple tabulation

of all the places where verbs appear is not enough to correctly determine
whether a language belongs to the verb-second group of languages.

… the blind superficial count of verb position that typically serves as
one of the main diagnostics in determining whether a language is a
legitimate member of the V2 class should be perhaps relegated to a
less prominent position in the essential characterizations of basic
syntax of this language group.

In other words, one must show that the clause-initial verbs in Later

Medieval Greek are not the result of Narrative Inversion, which is what
Fontana has demonstrated for the case of Old Spanish verb placement.
Fontana (1993: 107) argues for this position by developing two syntactic
constraints that the Narrative Inversion mechanism must observe in ad-
dition to having the semantic function of maintaining discourse cohe-
sion.

1.

Only NI structures are strictly a root phenomenon.

18

2.

Only in NI must subjects be definite NPs immediately following
the tensed verb.

background image

110

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Neither of these constraints is obeyed in Later Medieval Greek. The

first constraint is there to ensure that the verb-initial constructions found
in subordinate clauses are not treated as instances of NI (cf. Sigur∂sson
1990). The subjects in these subordinate verb-initial constructions are
new information (mostly indefinite NPs), and verb-initial order in subor-
dinate clauses exhibits VXS word order whereas in main clauses the
order is always VSX (see constraint 2). However, in Later Medieval
Greek, one can frequently find definite NP subjects in subordinate
clauses that do not convey new information but are topics. For example,
there is a passage in the Digene¤s text, where the four brothers of Di-
gene¤s’ mother approach the youngest brother to discuss the situation.
After their introductory address, we find the sentence:

(20)

ε'δεν

1 νεẃτερος

os

iDEn

o nEotEros

when

see–3sg Past

the youngster–Nom sg

‘When the youngster saw’ (Digene¤s, 421)

The information provided by the subject here is not new, and, moreo-

ver, the NP is definite. Similarly, there are several examples in LMG
where we find definite NPs immediately following the verb and pre-
serving discourse cohesion in subordinate clauses, something that is not
allowed, for instance, in Icelandic.

With respect to the second constraint, consider example (21). In this

example, the verb is in initial position, but the subject does not immedi-
ately follow it, and, moreover, the information conveyed by the subject
is new. The preceding lines discuss the deeds of a certain Kambanesis,
and in this line the topic of discourse switches. It is clear that examples
such as (21), which are quite frequent, violate the second of the con-
straints that Fontana claims are characteristic of Narrative Inversion.
Furthermore, there are several examples in which the subject is not only
removed from the verb but it is also an indefinite NP (22).

(21)

ε#ρéθηκεν

ε8ς τ7ν Βλαχíαν

EvrETikEn

is tin vlaxian

to be found–3sg Past

in Vlachia

ατóς 1 Μπονιφáτσος
aftos o bonifatsos
this Boniface–Nom sg

‘This Boniface, he found himself in Vlachia’ (Moreas, 1512)

background image

Previous Proposals

111

(22)

ε#ρíσκετον

κε

τóτε

που

EvriskEton

Eki

totE

opu

to be found–3sg Past

there

then

which

σè γρáφω, /

1κáποιος

μéγας

νθρωπος

sE Vrafo

okapjos

mEVas

anTropos

you–IOsg I write

some

great

man–Nom

‘There was there, then, at the time I am writing you about, / some great
man’ (Moreas, 1464)

Similar examples, especially cases where the subject introduces new

information, can be found in all texts, indicating that Narrative Inversion
did not occur in Later Medieval Greek. However, if Narrative Inversion
cannot be invoked to explain the presence of verb-initial declaratives in
LMG, then one cannot make the claim that the language is verb-second
in the same way that Fontana maintains that Yiddish, Icelandic and Old
Spanish are. For all these reasons, his proposal of autonomous verb
movement cannot be the explanation for Later Medieval Greek variation
in pronoun placement.

Weak object pronoun placement in Bulgarian and Old French

Two other languages whose weak object pronoun placement pattern can
be compared with that of Later Medieval Greek are Bulgarian and Old
French, in which the pronouns must appear adjacent to the verb, but their
position varies between postverbal or preverbal—depending on whether
the verb is clause-initial or preceded by some other element. Halpern
(1995) addresses these facts as part of his general discussion of second
position clitics and characterizes the weak pronouns as ‘verbal clitics’
because they have to appear next to the verb, unlike ‘true second posi-
tion (2P) clitics’ which have no such requirement. Halpern (ibid: 39)
further claims that these facts can be accounted for if one assumes that
‘the clitics take VP as their syntactic domain, but like 2P clitics (in some
cases) may undergo Prosodic Inversion.’ This is a phonological opera-
tion, which simply reverses the order between a clitic and its adjacent
phonological constituent. The mechanism is encoded into the algorithm
for Clitic Group (C) formation as follows (ibid: 63).

Revised algorithm C formation
I. For a D[irectional] CL[itic], X, which must attach to a

w

[phonological word] to its left (respectively right),

background image

112

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

a. if there is such a

w, Y, comprised of material which is syntactically

immediately to the left (right) of X, then adjoin X to the right (left) of
Y.
b. else attach X to the right (left) edge of the

w composed of syntactic

material immediately to its right (left).
II. For a simple (nondirectional) CL, attach the clitic to the

w, com-

posed of syntactically adjacent material, with which it shares the
greatest number of dominating syntactic nodes, respecting the syn-
tactic order.

Halpern also maintains that weak object pronouns in both Bulgarian

and Old French are enclitic.

19

The weak pronoun placement pattern in

both languages is accounted for by treating them as Directional Clitics
which always seek a host to their left. The following two examples from
Bulgarian (adapted from Halpern 1995) illustrate the process.

(23)

[

IP

[

VP

=mi=go [

VP

dade]]]

‡ [

IP

[

VP

dade=mi=go]]

‘S/he gives it to me’

(24)

[

IP

Tja [

VP

=mi=go [

VP

dade]]]

‡ [

IP

Tja [

VP

=mi=go [

VP

dade]]]

‘She gives it to me’

In example (23), there is no

w immediately to the left of the directional

clitic cluster mi go. Thus clause (Ia) of the algorithm cannot apply; under
clause (Ib) the pronoun is attached to the right edge of the verb dade. In
example (24), however, there is an

w, namely tja, to which the pronoun

can be adjoined. As a result, the pronoun appears preverbally.

In Later Medieval Greek, as has been shown earlier, the pronouns are

not directional; they are always dependent on the verb, both syntactically
and phonologically. Under clause II of the algorithm, they should be
attached to the phonological word ‘with which [they] share the greatest
number of dominating syntactic nodes’, presumably the verb. However,
this would yield preverbal pronouns, even in clause-initial situations,
which in this database favor postverbal pronouns. In addition, it would
predict preverbal pronouns when a subject immediately precedes the
verb, a situation in which postverbal and preverbal pronouns are equally
favored. Halpern’s Prosodic Inversion as it stands makes the wrong pre-
dictions for LMG.

background image

Previous Proposals

113

Similarly, the account proposed by Mis#eska Tomic! (1996: 831) for

Bulgarian cannot be transferred to LMG either. Her account assumes
that a non-finite verb-form (e.g., imperative) moves into Comp yielding
the V + CL order, while finite verb-forms cannot move in this way and
are thus regularly associated with CL + V order; nevertheless she also
claims that since pronouns in Bulgarian are phonologically enclitic, the
order CL + V is not allowed if it leaves the pronoun without a
phonological host to its left: ‘… in Bulgarian, for the most part, such
clitics look for phonological attachment to their left. Accordingly, the
clitics of the Bulgarian clitic cluster do not as a rule appear clause-ini-
tially …’ Once again the fact that LMG clitics are not directional makes
such an analysis untenable.

Weak object pronoun placement in some Modern Greek dialects

Another proposal that merits discussion is that of Drachman (1994) who
examines the structure for those Modern Greek dialects which show
variation between preverbal and postverbal placement (the Khios and
Kyklades varieties, which he calls the ‘Tobler-Mussafia dialects’). Fol-
lowing the Cardinaletti and Roberts (1991/to appear) solution of pro-
posing an extra AgrP-node, he proposes the structure seen in (25):

(25)

[

CP

[

C'

[

C

V] [

Agr1P

[

Agr1'

Clitic [

MP

[

Agr2P

t]]]]]]

According to Drachman, the presence of the clitic in Agr1 is what

triggers verb-movement to Comp, which then leads to the postverbal
placement of the pronoun. Although Drachman briefly addresses several
issues concerning GB theory, he does not explain how the clitic–verb
order comes to be in these dialects. In fact he never states how the clitic
comes to be in Agr1—is it base-generated or does it move to that posi-
tion? If it is base-generated in Agr1, then how do we get sequences like
θà μς γρáψει /Ta mas EVrapsi/ ‘he’ll write to us’ (Drachman’s ex-
ample 3a), where we would expect

μς θà γρáψει—since θá is usu-

ally assumed to be under MP? If the clitic moves to Agr1, where does it
move from, and what are the restrictions to that movement? Perhaps
these matters can be solved, though. The one aspect of Later Medieval
Greek that would remain problematic for this account (as it seems to be
for any other syntactic account) is the fact that pronouns may appear
both in postverbal and preverbal positions in the environment of a sub-
ject, a temporal expression or an imperative.

background image

114

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Other general approaches

In light of the shortcomings of all these mainly syntactic explanations it
may be legitimately asked whether a prosodic account should be pro-
posed, which would simply bar unstressed object pronouns from initial
position. The phenomenon, however, is too complex to yield to such an
analysis. As Wanner (1991: 320) observes, for Old Spanish ‘Prosody
must be supplemented by lexical and/or syntactic information, since
other (fully and near-) identical and typically unstressed forms do not
share the prohibition against initial occurrence …’

Similarly, in the Later Medieval Greek texts, there are other forms that

attach to verbs, like the negative adverb

ο, the subordinating marker

νá, the negative δéν etc. Why then are these not barred from this posi-
tion? To overcome this problem one would indeed have to postulate a
prosodic rule that has very specific lexical constraints on it, and this is
not desirable.

In addition, this type of explanation, which makes reference to the

utterance-initial position, would also have to provide some reason why
subjects, object, adverbs, etc. are sometimes considered parts of the ut-
terance and sometimes not. This is a difficult if not impossible position
to hold. A similar challenge for an analysis based on prosody is that it
would have to explain why a coordinating conjunction such as

καí can-

not be included in an utterance, when all other prosodic information,
including sandhi rules and its placement with respect to the caesura indi-
cate that it does.

Furthermore, the investigation into the effect of the caesura on pro-

noun placement (cf. Chapter 5) shows that this prosodic barrier does not
overrule syntactic constraints. If it did, then the position of the pronoun
at this important prosodic juncture would be categorically postverbal,
regardless of whether the constituent at the end of the first hemistich was
a subject or an object of the verb in question. The fact that, at these pro-
sodic breaks, preverbal placement is as common as postverbal placement
provides more evidence against a prosodic explanation of this phenome-
non.

Finally, I address the question of whether this phenomenon can be

accounted for with a morphological analysis, using the two most promi-
nent proposals, that of Klavans (1982, 1985) and the similar one by An-
derson (1992, 1993). In response to Zwicky’s (1977) pioneering treatise
on clitics, Klavans (1982) provides a unified framework for accounting

background image

Previous Proposals

115

for the position of clitics across languages. The framework is based on
five parameters

20

which are:

P1: Clitic identity (marking a clitic as [+clitic]).
P2: Domain of Cliticization (identifying the syntactic node which
determines the syntactic position of a clitic).
P3: Initial/Final (identifying which constituent, the first or the last,
under P2 is the host of the clitic).
P4: Before/After (refering to the locus of clitic attachment).
P5: Proclitic/Enclitic (identifying if the phonological liaison occurs to
the left–enclitic, or to the right–proclitic, of the host word or phrase).

Anderson (1993: 75) adopts a similar set of parameters:

a. S

COPE

: the clitic is located in the scope some syntactic constituent

(S, VP, NP, etc.—probably only X

max

) which constitutes its domain.

b. A

NCHOR

: the clitic is located by reference to the {

FIRST VS

.

LAST

VS

.

HEAD

} element of a specified sort within the constituent in which

it appears.
c. O

RIENTATION

: the clitic {

PRECEDES VS

.

FOLLOWS

} its anchor.

21

The problem that both of these analyses encounter with the Later Me-

dieval Greek data is that the Before/After parameter in Klavans (or the
{

PRECEDES VS

.

FOLLOWS

} parameter in Anderson) cannot be set without

extremely detailed information, not only about the finiteness of the
verb—as Klavans (1982: 107) does for Modern Spanish—but also with
reference to what immediately precedes the verb complex, encoded in
the parameter settings. Not only is it difficult to imagine how this op-
tionality could be written in the parameter settings, it would be impossi-
ble to encode it in the lexical entry itself as Klavans (1985: 118) sug-
gests. Although some analyses that have used this framework have pro-
posed not setting one of the parameters (for instance both Klavans 1985
for Nganhcara, and Taylor 1990 for Ancient Greek leave P4 as Be-
fore/After), a similar approach in LMG would require one to leave both
P4 and P5 open, and at the same time match the Before value for P4 with
a Proclitic value for P5 and an After value for P4 with an Enclitic value
for P5. Taken together with the fact that P3 has no applicable value
(since the Domain of Cliticization is V), this latest approach would es-
sentially indicate nothing more than ‘preverbal clitics are proclitic, post-
verbal clitics are enclitic.’

background image

116

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Conclusion

In this chapter several proposals that could have an impact on the phe-
nomenon of weak object pronoun variation, either directly or indirectly,
were examined. It was shown that none of these analyses can provide a
full, principled account of the phenomenon. Moreover, the discussion
has highlighted the trouble spots that any account would have to deal
with before putting the issue to rest. These are:
1.

The affinity of the pronoun to phonologically attach to the verb, and
thus varying between procliticization and encliticization.

2.

The unpredictability of pronoun placement, especially when the
complex is immediately preceded by a subject or a temporal expres-
sion.

One of the most surprising aspects of this pattern is how stable it

seems to have remained over a period of five centuries. In the next
chapter, documents from the period after 1550 are examined in an at-
tempt to understand how and when this system began to change.

background image

Previous Proposals

117

14

Although I do not necessarily agree with the use of the term ‘clitic’ in

these accounts, I will not engage, for the moment, in a discussion about
the status of the weak pronouns as ‘clitics’ or ‘affixes’. This issue will
be taken up in a later chapter. Here I will focus on the problematic
aspects of these analyses, even if their assumption that the pronouns are
clitics in LMG is granted.

15

Even though Horrocks refers to the weak object pronouns as ‘clitics’

his use here of the term ‘word-like complex’ to refer to the string
composed of the various verbal markers the object pronouns and the
verb suggests that he considers these elements to be affixes, as Joseph
(1988, 1989, 1990, 2000b) has argued.

16

Wanner (1981a) has also written about the situation in Old Italian

(OIt), where the pattern of variation in pronoun placement is quite
similar to LMG.

One finds postverbal placement when the verb

(finite—including imperative) is verb-form, or following certain
paratactic conjunctions (e.g., e ‘and’ or ma ‘but’, though not o ‘or’, peró
‘however’), and when the verb is of non-finite form (infinitive, gerund,
absolute participial constructions).

He accounts for these facts by

proposing that in OIt there were linearization principles that were
constrained syntactically: “clitics could not stand after a surface clause
boundary”. Furthermore, he notes that the system of clitic placement
was ‘mixed’, since the non-finite forms had to be morphosyntactic
exceptions while the conjuctions e and ma also had to be lexically
marked as associated with postverbal placement. In a (1996) article,
Wanner suggests that ‘clitic position’ in the Medieval Romance
languages can be better understood as a transition phenomenon, as the
languages moved from a second position schema of pronoun placement
(à la Wackernagel’s Law) to a schema in which the pronoun is attached
to the verb.

17

According to Wanner (1991, 1996), though, Old Spanish interpolation

was a much more complex phenomenon than indicated in Fontana’s
(1993) account.

background image

118

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

18

In fact, the constraint is poorly worded, for if taken literally it means

that any phenomenon that is restricted to root clauses is a NI structure.
This would make V2 in German, and tag-questions in English NI
structures, clearly an unwanted conclusion. Rather, the constraint seems
to mean “NI structures are only a root phenomenon.” I thank Martin
Jansche and Shravan Vasishth for helping me sort out the logical
implications of the constraint.

19

This is not an uncontroversial assumption. Halpern mentions that

“they may at times be proclitics (Ewen 1979: 5,6) but it does seem that
they are always subject to a constraint which prevents them from being
clause-initial". In Old French, however, there are some instances of
clause-initial ‘clitics’ (Adams 1987).

20

This, in fact, is only true of the (1980) dissertation. Already in the

introduction to the revised version of 1982, Klavans limits the set of
parameters to three, keeping P3, P4 and P5. The set of five parameters is
used here for ease of explication.

21

Proclisis vs. Enclisis, according to Anderson (1992: 203) “refers to the

properties (and consequences of a particular language’s rule(s) of Stray
Adjunction.”

background image

117

7

A Diachronic Perspective

In this chapter it is argued, based on the evidence of prose texts from the
17

th

century, that the stability of the pattern of pronoun placement dis-

cussed in the previous chapters is a stylistic effect, and that the ‘ver-
nacular’ must have advanced to a pattern more similar to that of Stan-
dard Modern Greek at a much earlier stage than the existing poems from
the Late Byzantine period suggest. Thus, a synchronic account of pro-
noun placement in Later Medieval Greek may indeed be impossible
since there is no clear evidence of what the actual set of facts would be.
Still, the phenomenon can be understood if examined from a diachronic
perspective; it is proposed here that the complex pattern of variation in
weak object pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek is a result of
ongoing analogical change, phases of which became stylized in poetry.
The ramifications of such a proposal for the questions raised in previous
chapters are also examined.

Pronoun placement in 17

th

century Greek: poetry vs. prose

The absence of accessible prose documents from the Later Medieval
Greek period complicates the investigation of weak object pronoun
placement in ways that have already been discussed. In the following
period, however, one can find both prose and poetics texts, and this al-
lows for an illuminating comparison of pronoun placement between the
two styles of writing. Table 7.1 below presents the findings of the ex-
amination of pronoun placement in prose texts written between the sec-
ond half of the 16

th

and the 17

th

century in several different areas of the

background image

118

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

former Byzantine Empire, while Table 7.3 presents the results of a simi-
lar investigation in Cretan poetry during the same period.

The data in Table 7.1 clearly show that with finite, non-imperative

verb forms, preverbal placement of pronouns is almost categorical in all
contexts (81% for initial, 80% for reduplicated object, 99% for function
word
, 94% for fronted constituent, 92% for subject and 73% for tempo-
ral expression
). Examples (1)-(4) demonstrate this pattern.

Table 7.1

Pronoun placement by factor in prose texts from different areas in

17

th

century Greek

Area

Æ

Crete

Mainland

Heptanese

Unknown

Factor

Ø

Pre

Post

Pre

Post

Pre

Post

Pre

Post

Initial

91

29

35

5

32

6

13

1

Red Obj

8

1

3

1

1

0

0

1

Fctn wd

245

5

117

0

68

1

33

0

Frd cnst

25

1

21

1

12

2

3

0

Subject

24

3

14

0

4

0

6

1

Temp

6

3

0

0

0

0

2

0

Impv

2

11

0

1

0

9

0

1

Gerund

0

12

0

3

0

11

0

1

Total

401

65

190

11

117

29

57

5

(1)

Τéλος πáντων

τò

%πεφáσισαν

tElos pandon

to

apEfasisan

anyway

it–DOsg WP

decide–3pl Past

‘Anyway, they decided it’ (Papasynodinos, 1630—Serres)

(2)

καì μο

/δωκε

kE

mu

EDokE

and

me–IOsg WP

give–3sg Past

‘And he gave me’ (Abatios, 1648—Kephalonia)

(3)

τà Jερà βιβλíα

τà

/γραψε

ta iEra vivlia

ta

EVrapsE

the holly books

it–DOpl WP

write–3sg Past

‘The … holy books, he wrote them’ (Loukare¤s, 1610-1635—Crete)

(4)

1 θεος ζλος

μè

κíνησε

o Tios zilos

mE

EkinisE

divine zeal–Nom sg

I–DOsg WP

move–3sg Past

‘Divine zeal moved me’ (Maximos, 1640—Peloponnesus)

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

119

Gerunds in texts from all areas show categorical postverbal placement,

as was noted for the Later Medieval Greek texts in Chapter 4. What is
more, there are two examples of negated gerunds in the texts (one from
the Heptanese, the other from Crete, see example 5) and in both of them
the pronoun appears postverbally. Thus, it seems reasonable to conclude
that at least by the early 17

th

century the object pronoun in a gerund con-

struction was always placed postverbally without regard to the element
that immediately preceded it.

(5)

μ7ν Lξεúροντáς

το

min

iksEvrondas

to

not

know–Gerund

it–DOsg WP

‘Not knowing it’ (Soumake¤s, 1646—Zakynthos)

When the verb is in the imperative form, the pronoun is placed post-

verbally if the verb complex is clause-initial, or immediately follows a
coordinating conjunction or a reduplicated object. However, when the
imperative is immediately preceded by some other element, there are
two instances of a preverbal pronoun, and one instance of a postverbal
one. All three examples come from Cretan writers so it is possible that
this pattern, altough widespread in LMG texts, by Early Modern Greek it
is retained only in this dialect.

(6)

τóτε

τò

ρáντισε

totE

to

randisE

then

it–DOsg WP

spray–Impv sg

‘Then spray it’ (Agapios, 1643—Crete)

Table 7.2

Pronoun placement by factor in 17

th

century Cypriot texts

Factor

Pre

Post

Initial

2

16

Red Obj

0

1

Fctn wd

30

1

Frd cnst

2

1

Subject

0

1

Temp

2

0

Impv

0

3

Gerund

0

0

Total

36

23

background image

120

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

The only exceptions to this description are found in texts written by

Cypriot writers (cf. examples 7 and 8 and Table 7.2) where the pattern of
pronoun placement is almost identical to that seen in the chronicles of
Makhairas and Boustro¤nios, which was discussed in Chapter 5. Even in
the later Cypriot texts, for the most part, the pronoun is placed prever-
bally only when a function word immediately precedes the verb com-
plex, while postverbal placement occurs with factor initial. There are not
enough tokens of constructions involving the other factors so we cannot
form any conclusions about them.

(7)

καì /δωκé

του

το

kE

EDokE

tu

to

and

give–3sg Past

he–IOsg WP

it–DOpl WP

‘And he gave it to him’ (Xenakios, 1610—Cyprus)

(8)

1 (γιος

ε'πε

τους

o aVios

ipE

tus

the saint–Nom sg

say–3sg Past

they–IOpl WP

‘The saint said to them’ (Xenakios, 1610—Cyprus)

Surprisingly though, the pattern of pronoun placement that one finds

in the poetic texts of the same period is very different from the one ob-
served above. As Table 7.3 shows, there is no major difference in pro-
noun placement between 17

th

century poetry and the Later Medieval

Greek texts, a fact that can easily be determined if one compares the data
from the Cretan Renaissance poems with the early 15

th

century poems by

Phalieros which were also written in Crete. Even if one uses only the
Cretan prose texts in this comparison, the difference between the two
styles of writing is very conspicuous and indicates a clear distinction in
pronoun placement for finite verb-forms. Although postverbal placement
is still available, and even preferred for certain syntactic contexts in the
poetry (see examples 9-12), this is not the case for the prose, where there
is a strong tendency for preverbal placement even in such environments
as clause initial position and after a temporal expression.

22

(9)

ξúπνουν

τηνε

κι /λεγá

τση

Eksipnun

tine

ki ElEVa

tsi

awake–1sg Impf

she–DOsg

and say–1sg Impf

she–IOsg WP

‘I used to awake her and say to her’ (Boskopoula, 373)

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

121

(9)

1 Θεòς

γρικ

το

o TEos

Vrika

to

the god–Nom sg

hear–3sg Pres

‘God hears it’ (Thysia, 22)

(9)

πáντα

προφτευγá

το

panda

profitevVa

to

always

foretell–1sg Impf

it–DOsg WP

‘I used to awake her and say to her’ (Boskopoula, 373)

(10)

;Η χáρη σου

μο

τò

4πεψε

i xari su

mu

to

pEpsE

the grace your

I–IOsg WP

it–DOsg WP

send–3sg Past

και πáλι

μο

τò

παíρνει

kE pali

mu

to

pErni

and again

I–IOsg WP

it–DOsg WP

take–3sg Pres

‘Your grace sent it to me, and again takes it from me’ (Thysia, 225)

Table 7.3

Pronoun placement in 17

th

century Cretan poetry

Text

Æ

Thysia

Ero

¤

phile

¤

Boskopoula

Phalieros (15

th

c.)

Factor

Ø Pre Post Pre Post Pre

Post

Pre

Post

Initial

4

23

13

9

6

22

15

73

Red Obj

1

0

2

1

0

0

1

1

Fctn wd

136

2

89

3

49

1

155

0

Frd cnst

13

0

23

4

11

2

32

3

Subject

20

2

7

0

6

0

12

4

Temp

6

1

6

1

2

2

2

0

Impv

4

39

1

6

2

4

1

29

Gerund

0

0

0

3

0

0

0

0

Total

184

67

141

27

76

31

218

110

Another major difference that one observes in the pattern of weak pro-

noun placement in the prose texts concerns the periphrastic tense con-
structions, especially the future. Unlike the pattern that is found in the
Cretan Renaissance poems, and in the Later Medieval Greek texts dis-
cussed in previous chapters, in the prose texts there is a tendency to
place the pronoun between

θéλω and the infinitive, even in contexts

where one would expect the pronoun to appear to the left

θéλω, as in

examples (13) and (14).

background image

122

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(13)

πẁς θéλομε

τò

πáθει

pos

TElomE

to

paTi

that

want–1pl Pres

it–DOsg WP

suffer–Infin

‘That we will suffer it’ (Santorinaios, 1660—Santorine)

(14)

τíνας

Wθελε

τò

%ρνηθε

tinas

iTElE

to

arniTi

who

want–3sg Past

it–DOsg WP

deny–Infin

‘Who would deny it?’ (Loukare¤s, 1610-1635—Crete)

This pattern is quite striking, especially if Mackridge’s explanation is

brought to mind, i.e., that the pronoun is always the syntactic argument
of

θéλω and its position is determined according to the rules for non-

periphrastic finite verb-forms. Under those constraints, one would expect
the pronoun in these two examples to appear before

θéλω and not after it

since the immediately preceding element is a function word. I will return
to these examples at the end of the next section in order to provide an
explanation for this surprising pattern of pronoun placement.

Faced with such stylistic differences, one wonders which of the two

styles was closest to the ‘vernacular’. This is not an easy question to
answer, because both the poems and the prose writings are popular in
character. Most of the poems in this era are actually theatrical plays, and
as such would have been performed at some point or another for a large
and diverse audience. The prose writings are either brief histories, or
sermon-like pieces that are clearly meant for a wide audience, as they
draw analogies from farming, fishing and other lower-class occupations.
Some of them even discuss the matter of writing in a language that eve-
ryone can understand, and emphasize the importance of translating the
Bible into such a ‘vernacular’. All indications suggest that both styles
were accessible to a wide audience. Nevertheless, given the larger tradi-
tion of popular poetry in the Byzantine era, which must have served as
the foundation for the Cretan poems of the 17

th

century, and the fact that

the style seen in the prose texts does not have such an obvious base, it is
reasonable to conclude that, although accessible to the masses, the lan-
guage of the poems (at least as far as pronoun placement is concerned)
does not truly reflect the spoken language of the time. Instead, it seems
to present a compromise between what the spoken norm was and the
linguistic framework of the genre to which the composition aspired to
belong.

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

123

Indeed, such a view is also held for Byzantine popular poetry (which

is the content of the LMG texts researched in this study). The most noted
champions of this view, Elizabeth and Michael Jeffreys (1986: 506) pro-
pose the following:

… we should see in the popular poetry of Byzantium the written re-
mains of a tradition of oral poetry. This statement must be very care-
fully qualified. We think it most unlikely that any of the surviving
texts are the verbatim record of creative oral performances, taken
down by the methodology of the ‘oral dictated text’ (see Lord
1953)—though in one or two cases this possibility cannot be ex-
cluded, as will be discussed later. We believe, on the other hand, that
it is almost impossible to explain many features of the language, me-
ter, and style of this genre of poems without assuming that they de-
rive in a fairly direct way from a language, meter, and style developed
by oral poets for use in oral poetry. We would suggest, therefore, that
Byzantine popular poetry was produced by means, which approxi-
mate to those of conventional literature, but in a genre most of whose
products were orally composed and disseminated. This genre was the
only one available to poets who wished to write in a way which
would be immediately intelligible to the uneducated majority of their
audiences.

Mackridge (1993: 333), on the other hand, claims that the variation in

pronoun placement in the LMG texts cannot be ascribed to a literary
language (‘Kunstsprache’), but is rather representative of the ‘vernacu-
lar’. The first of his three arguments is that the rules that he proposes are
‘not only logical, but are followed quite consistently by the versifiers
and copyists.’ His second argument is that his proposal does not only
cover the poetic texts of Later Medieval Greek, it can also account for
the prose texts that were written in Cyprus and even the facts about pro-
noun placement in present day Cypriot. The final argument is based on
the pattern of pronoun placement found in the Medieval Romance lan-
guages. According to Mackridge, their similarity to the facts from LMG
provides corroborating evidence that the variation is a phenomenon of
natural language and not a contrivance.

As far as the first argument is concerned, adherence to reason and de-

scriptive adequacy are the least one would expect from an account; they
cannot be held up as arguments for the account’s validity, nor can it be

background image

124

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

asserted that only a spoken language can be described in a logical way.
With respect to the second argument, it has already been demonstrated in
Chapter 5 that the Cypriot texts that Mackridge refers to do not show the
same pattern as the verse texts. Furthermore, simply asserting that Mod-
ern Cypriot follows ‘broadly the same rules’ does not provide any sup-
port for his argument without a closer investigation of how similar or
different the rules really are. Finally, there are some crucial differences
between pronoun placement in the Medieval Romance languages and
pronoun placement in LMG, most notably the fact that interpolation was
possible in the former but not in the latter. Moreover, the similarity be-
tween the two can be used to corroborate the view that the weak pronoun
variation was a stylistic effect just as easily as it can support
Mackridge’s claim, especially when some of the LMG works are trans-
lations from Romance originals.

Since Mackridge’s arguments are not particularly convincing, and

since it is clear that at least in the early 17

th

century, pronoun placement

did vary according to the style of the text, one can only speculate how
far back into time this stylistic distinction can be carried. It is conceiv-
able that even the earliest compositions (Digene¤s, for example) were
created in the language of a genre and not in the ‘people’s tongue’. In
this respect, certain systematic counterexamples to postverbal placement
like the

σè λéγω formula in Moreas or preverbal pronouns after καí in

the Rimada (cf. Chapter 5) may not be indications of the inception of a
dialectal change in weak object pronoun placement but rather manifesta-
tions of the controlled mixing of two different styles. Such speculations,
however, should not be considered an endorsement of the view that the
language of these texts is merely an artifice, disjointed from the spoken
language of its time. Instead, as Joseph (2000: 317) says, in arguing for
the authenticity of infinitival forms and constructions in Later Medieval
Greek: ‘Since many of these texts were pieces of popular literature, in-
tended for a general audience … it must be assumed that the forms and
constructions in them were generally accessible to the audience’, i.e., at
least their passive competence can be tapped. In a similar vein it is
maintained here that the main pattern of variation in pronoun placement
was indeed a reflection of the ‘vernacular’, if we are to judge from the
evidence of Modern Greek and the various regional dialects, but that
there was also a number of constructions that the composer had more
poetic license with.

23

Such leeway would have been available especially

if the spoken language of the period in which this genre was being cre-

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

125

ated (most likely around the 10

th

century—cf. Jeffreys 1986) was under-

going change with respect to the placement of pronouns.

Examining the pattern of variation from a diachronic per-
spective

The previous section sought to establish—through a comparison be-
tween poetic and prose writings of the 17

th

century—that the stability

seen in the pattern of pronoun placement during the period of Later Me-
dieval Greek may be partly due to the fact that the available texts from
the period are all written in a particular style. It is further surmised that
the changes that are so evident in the prose texts must have begun much
earlier, although in the absence of extensive prose writings from the
LMG period, the exact starting point and route of the change may never
become clear. Nevertheless, I believe that there is enough information
available to formulate a hypothesis about the development that took
place in weak object pronoun placement in Greek.

In light of the available evidence, perhaps the most plausible account

is one in which the placement of weak object pronouns in the language
of the Cypriot chronicles is considered as the immediate predecessor of
the more complex variation pattern witnessed in the other LMG texts.
There are several advantages to such an approach. The first one is that
the Cypriot texts are in prose and thus less likely to be affected by sty-
listic considerations. They provide a ‘baseline’, as it were, against which
to judge other developments. Secondly, from the middle of the 7

th

cen-

tury Cyprus was becoming increasingly isolated from the rest of Byz-
antium, especially Constantinople, the cultural center of the empire. This
isolation was not only political and cultural but also linguistic. It started
with the Arab conquest of the island, which set off a period of political
instability, during which the island kept changing from Arab to Byzan-
tine control and back, and was finalized by the conquest of Cyprus by
Richard the Lionheart in 1191. In 1192, the island was sold to the French
who ruled it up until 1489, at which time the Venetians took control of
it. In 1571, Cyprus came under Ottoman rule. The isolation from the rest
of the Byzantine Empire would account both for the use of the vernacu-
lar in prose writings, and lend credence to the proposal that the Cypriot
texts may indeed be conservative with respect to weak object pronoun
placement.

24

The third advantage of this approach is that the variation in

the Cypriot texts has a much clearer pattern than that of the LMG texts,
and, thus is a better prospect for a structural explanation.

background image

126

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

As has already been discussed in Chapter 5, weak object pronoun

placement in the Cypriot texts varies as follows. The pronoun appears
postverbally in all situations except when the verb complex is immedi-
ately preceded by a function word, a cover term used in this study to
signify a wh-phrase, a negative marker other than

ο, a number of tem-

poral and clausal conjunctions and the subjunctive marker

νá. This pat-

tern was described in detail in Chapter 5, and examples (15) through (18)
illustrate its key characteristics.

(15)

Λαλε

το

1 Γιáκουμος

lali

tu

o Viakumos

speak–3sg Pres

he–IOsg WP

the Jacomo–Nom sg

‘Jacomo spoke to him’ (Boustro¤nios, §83)

(16)

καì

1 ργας

/ρισéν

τον

kE

o riVas

ErisEn

ton

and

the king–Nom sg

order–3sg Past

he–DOsg WP

‘And the king ordered him’ (Boustro¤nios, §84)

(17)

καì

ε8ς ]λíγαις $μéραις

βγáλαν

τον

kE

is oliVEs imErEs

EvValan

ton

and

in a few days

take out–3pl Past

he–DOsg

‘And in a few days they took him out’ (Boustro¤nios, §75)

(18)

καì

θαφíον

δèν το

ποκαν

kE

Tafion

DEn

tu

Epikan

and

grave–DOsg

not

he–IOsg WP

make–3pl Past

‘And they did not make a grave for him’ (Boustro¤nios, §88)

Although this pattern of variation may, at first sight, seem more ame-

nable to a structural explanation, none of the analyses presented in
Chapter 6 can be employed in this case either. For accounts like those of
Klavans (1982, 1985), Anderson (1992, 1993), and Halpern (1995), the
problematic aspects of strict attachment to the verb and variation be-
tween proclisis and enclisis are still present. If, on the other hand, one
attempts to form a Principles and Parameters or Minimalist style analy-
sis, which—à la Wanner (1991)—seeks to employ the mechanism of
verb movement in order to account for the position of the pronoun in the
Cypriot data, the proposal would have to maintain that the pronoun can-

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

127

not be the first realized form after a CP boundary. This constraint would
force the verb to move to the Comp position. In order to adopt such a
view, however, one is obliged to also maintain that preverbal elements
such as subjects, adverbs, objects, etc. are positioned to the left of the CP
boundary since they are all associated with postverbal pronoun place-
ment. This, in turn, implies that in neutral word order the subject is not
the first element, and further predicts that in those dependent clauses
which are introduced by a conjunction or a complementizer, a subject
should not be able to appear between the conjunction and the verb. In
other words, *Comp–S–V.

Contrary to this prediction, however, there are several examples of

such an ordering, where the subject appears after the conjunction and
before the verb. This can be seen in examples (19) and (20):

(19)

%φF ^ν

οJ κατáδικοι …

πραν τò ;Ιεροσóλυμαν

afon

i kataDiki

piran

to iErosoliman

Comp

Subj

Verb

since

the enemies …

took

the Jerusalem

‘Ever since the enemies … took over Jerusalem’ (Makhairas, §324)

(20)

μπως καì οJ Βενετíκοι

ποíσουν

ριμοúρι

mipos kE i vEnEtiki

pisun

rimuri

Comp Subj

Verb

in case the Venetians–Nom pl

make–3pl Pres

a stir–DOsg

‘In case the Venetians would make a stir’ (Makhairas, §325)

The above analysis highlights another aspect of the pattern of variation

that would be problematic for any account based on the structural prop-
erties of syntactic trees, as verb movement explanations are prone to do.
The difficulty lies in the fact that such accounts require that at least four
different categories of elements (wh-phrases, complementizers, modal
markers, negators) for which there is ample evidence that they behave
differently and should be assigned different positions in a phrase struc-
ture tree, should be treated similarly with respect to pronoun place-
ment.

25

Alternatively, pronoun placement has to be explained as an

epiphenomenon of extremely complex structural maneuvers that are for
the most part unmotivated. Such attempts, when carried out for the sake
of theoretical considerations and despite the linguistic evidence, have
two adverse affects on our understanding of grammar: they multiply

background image

128

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

abstract categories beyond what is necessary, and they shift the focus of
complexity onto areas of the grammar that need not be complex.

If, on the other hand, the data is evaluated from a diachronic perspec-

tive, the situation is not unprecedented. The long list of disparate ele-
ments that are associated with preverbal pronoun placement in the lan-
guage of the Cypriot chronicles (namely, those elements listed under
factor function word in this study) can be viewed as the result of an
analogical process of change. In fact, if this were a phenomenon of mor-
phophonological variation in which a number of various and sundry en-
vironments had—surprisingly—the same effect, analogy would be the
‘prime suspect’ (Hock 1991: 171). Given that all the accounts reviewed
in Chapter 6 have been unable to provide a synchronic account for the
phenomenon, mainly because there is no way to prove complementary
distribution, the best (and perhaps only) account is to postulate that the
patterns of pronoun placement variation seen in these texts are freeze-
frames during the period of an analogical repositioning of weak object
pronouns.

This type of explanation is not new. It was first proposed by Ramsden

(1963: 112-133) for Late Latin and Early Romance. According to Rams-
den, in the Late Latin period, preverbal weak pronouns are favored in
subordinate clauses due to a rhythmical change in the language. This, he
proposes, led to the ‘… analogical extension of pronoun anteposition on
the model of those cases where it was being (or had been) established for
rhythmic reasons’ (Ramsden 1963: 117). A similar analysis is briefly
proposed in Hock (1991: 611-618). Although this type of explanation
has been dismissed by those working in the GB framework—Wanner
(1991: 320) describes Ramsden’s proposal as ‘quite suspect’—it pre-
sents itself as the only meaningful way of understanding the variation
seen in LMG. In particular, Wanner was suspicious of Ramsden’s refer-
ence to ‘sentence rhythm’ as the cause of the change from postverbal to
preverbal. It must be noted, however, that Wanner (1996) adopts a per-
spective similar to Ramsden’s since he looks for an understanding of the
synchronic complexity through a diachronic perspective.

Any account that refers to analogical change as an explanation needs

to establish two things: the model upon which the analogical process was
based, and the existing surface similarities that allowed speakers to ex-
tend the model to other contexts. The marker

νá appears to be the most

likely candidate as the source of the change from postverbal to preverbal
pronoun placement. Unlike other elements which introduce subordinate

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

129

clauses,

νá is in a sense bound to the verb since only the negative

marker

μν and the weak object pronouns can appear between it and the

verb. Furthermore, as the phonological distinction between the indicative
and the subjunctive (and optative) was lost due to regular sound changes,
νá seems to assume this role of distinguishing feature and, consequently,
also assumes the unique quality of finely shading the mood of the verb
into subjunctive, hortative, conditional or even future meanings (cf. Jo-
seph 1981, 1983a, 1985, Horrocks 1995, 1997: 167, 208-211, 230).
These two characteristics, especially the second one, could mean that in
the

νá–verb cluster of LMG the head is not the verb anymore but νá, a

position that is also taken by Veloudis and Philippaki-Warburton (1983)
and Philippaki-Warburton (1994) for

νá as a subjunctive marker in

Modern Greek. In the latter account, the subjunctive mood is realized as
a node in a phrase structure tree, and that node is filled by

νá; that part

of the analysis, however, is not assumed here.

Thus, if one accepts Horrocks’ description of Early Medieval Greek

pronoun placement as being mainly post-head (i.e., after nouns and after
verbs—see Chapter 1) then the preverbal placement of the pronoun
when the complex is preceded by

νá can be explained as an instance of

canonical post-head placement except the head is no longer the verb.
This proposal, then agrees with Horrocks (1990) that the

νá construc-

tions were the change from postverbal pronoun placement to preverbal
pronoun placement was triggered. However, he does not identify

νá as a

head, only as the initial element in the verbal complex (see Chapter 6).
This change is also a reflection of Wackernagel’s Law, especially once
the

νá–verb form could be a main-clause predicate (see discussion of

Nevis and Joseph 1993 in the final chapter). Due to the greater frequency
of

νá constructions in the language this pattern would have gained

prominence rather quickly.

Therefore, if one makes the reasonable assumption that the string

νá–pronoun–verb was the model that influenced the preverbal placement
in other contexts, there remains the question of how it came to be the
norm when negative markers, complementizers, and wh-expressions
immediately preceded the verb complex. One very plausible explanation
can be based on Kathol’s (2000) account for the appearance of verbal
morphology on complementizers and fronted wh-expressions in embed-
ded questions in certain Dutch and German dialects. Consider the fol-
lowing examples highlighted by Kathol (2000: 61-62, 112):

background image

130

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(21) South Hollandic
dat–(t)–e

ze

com–(m)–e

that–pl

they

come–pl

(22) Brabantish
dat–de

gullie

komt

that–2pl

you

come–2pl

(23) Bavarian
wenn–st

du

kumm–st

if–2pl

you

come–2pl

(24) Colloquial German
a.

wenn–ste

komm–st

if–2sg

come–2sg

b.

warum–ste/wann–ste

kommst

why–2sg/when–2sg

come–2sg

Kathol states that these peculiar distributional characteristics, which

are—strangely enough—reminiscent of the pattern of pronoun place-
ment in the Cypriot chronicles,

26

cannot be explained via a pure phrase

structure encoding as in a GB approach:

… any attempt to explain inflected complementizers in terms of the
mechanisms underlying verb movement fails to extend to those cases
in which the special inflection is seen on a clause-initial wh-phrase,
such as in relative and embedded interrogative clauses (Kathol 2000:
63).

Instead, Kathol proposes that the appearance of verbal morphology on

the complementizers and wh-expressions is due to analogical change,
and that the link upon which the analogical extension travels is the linear
position of the elements in question.

… the emergence of special morphology on complementizers has to
be thought of as an analogical process in which the shape alternations
seen with verbs in frontal position end up being associated not with
particular syntactic categories, but rather with the linear position it-

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

131

self … these shape alternations that originally only made proper sense
as part of verbal morphology … get carried over to other elements
occupying the same linear position … (Kathol 2000: 62-63).

A similar case can be made for the spread of preverbal pronoun

placement between Early and Later Medieval Greek. Since the negative
markers

δéν and μν also become attached to the verb, it is most likely

that negative constructions were the next step in the change to preverbal
placement. From this stage it is foreseeable that the pattern would be
generalized to include other elements that were both similar in shape
(i.e., short) and typically appeared immediately before the verb, in effect
creating the category function word. This is the pattern found in the Cyp-
riot chronicles. A further development would have led to the preverbal
placement of pronouns when the complex was preceded by ‘larger’ ele-
ments as well. In fact, the preverbal placement of the pronoun in redu-
plicated object constructions with

λος was probably based not only on

the ambiguous status of the pronoun

τους between a partitive or an ob-

ject pronoun but also on the fact that the partitive constructions provided
a linear model of preverbal pronoun placement which was then extended
to the reduplicated object constructions as well (see Chapter 4). The final
step would have been the preverbal placement of pronouns in all situa-
tions, even when the verb was clause initial. It is hypothesized here that
the alternation in pronoun position between affirmative and negated
main clauses seen in (25) must have played a crucial role in this final
extension of preverbal pronoun placement.

(25)

a.

δíνω το

b.

δèν τò δíνω

‘I give it’

‘I do not give it’

At this point, one might ask if a synchronic account of weak object

pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek (or some sub-period of this
stage) can be provided. After all, as stated in Chapter 1, one of the main
goals of this thesis was to arrive at a generalization that would capture
the facts, and provide a better insight into the nature of the grammar than
Mackridge’s list of rules did. However, this attempt has not produced the
expected—one might even say desired—result, but not for lack of trying,
or of willingness to test a wide range of approaches. Rather, the main
difficulty arises from the nature of the data usually involved in historical
linguistics: to paraphrase Labov, historical linguists must make the best

background image

132

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

of imperfect data since much of the necessary information is oftentimes
missing. This has also been the case for this project; the fact that almost
all available texts are written in a specific poetic style, which is demon-
strably different from the few available prose texts, makes it difficult to
identify a specific set of linguistic facts with a specific period in time,
and provide a synchronic account. Furthermore, even in the language of
the Cypriot chronicles, which approximates the above-stated desidera-
tum, the elements associated with categorical preverbal placement are
too diverse to be captured under a single generalization. Even though
this conclusion was unexpected, it is, nonetheless, significant, especially
as it correlates with the more general question of what synchronic
grammars can and should account for, an issue that will be revisited in
the final chapter of this book.

Further implications for Later Medieval Greek

There are several aspects of this analysis that need to be explained in
further detail. First, there is the matter of determining the time in which
this change began. The lack of a sufficient amount of ‘vernacular’
documents from the Early Medieval Greek period will always present a
serious obstacle to such an effort. The one observation that can be made
is that the change must predate the period of the composition of the Ac-
clamations
(7

th

-10

th

century) for in them we find the following construc-

tions:

(26)

μ7

μè

δεíρεις

mi

mE

Diris

not

I–DOsg WP

beat–2sg Pres

‘Do not beat me’ (Acclamations, 10: 31)

(27)

1 Μáρτης

σè

διẃκει

o martis

sE

Dioki

the march–Nom sg

you–DOsg WP

chase–3sg Pres

‘March is chasing you’ (Acclamations, 10: 15)

According to Horrocks (1997: 256) ‘These fragments of a more every-

day language are unfortunately neither common nor extensive, but they
do serve to confirm that popular spoken Greek in the early and middle
Byzantine periods was developing strongly in the direction of the mod-
ern language in tems of grammar and the lexicon.’ It is claimed here that
weak object pronoun placement was one of these features.

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

133

Another issue that needs clarification is the placement of the pronoun

that is an argument of a gerund or imperative, for one may ask why the
pattern of preverbal placement was not extended to these verb-forms as
well. Following Joseph’s (1978/1990, 1983a) generalization that, in
SMG, pronoun placement is determined by the finiteness of the verb-
form, one might hypothesize that the status of these verb-forms as non-
finite was solidified in Later Medieval Greek before preverbal pronoun
placement became categorical for the finite verb-forms, and that post-
verbal placement with non-finite forms was obligatory. This hypothesis,
however, is contradicted by the fact that there are some examples of pre-
verbal pronoun placement with the imperatives and other non-finite
forms such as the infinitives and active participles (cf. Chapter 4).

Although the facts of Standard Modern Greek pronoun placement may

be best described if the imperative is considered non-finite, non-finite-
ness is not considered here to have been the cause of postverbal pronoun
placement becoming the norm with imperatives and gerunds. Instead, it
is claimed that the fact that imperative and gerund constructions pre-
dominantly have the verb in clause-initial position gave the postverbal
placement pattern much more prominence in these circumstances than
this had when the verb-form was indicative or subjunctive. Especially in
the case of the imperative, it is equally important that it cannot be ne-
gated (cf. Chapters 4 and 6). Thus, the lack of a robust preverbal pro-
noun pattern within imperative and gerund constructions must have led
to the division seen in SMG, where pronouns are placed preverbally with
the indicative and the subjunctive verb-forms and postverbally with the
imperative and the gerund. The added fact that the imperative of SMG
shows extremely limited inflection has resulted in a situation that is best
described by Joseph’s generalization, which treats the imperative as a
non-finite form.

Finally, one has to consider the placement of the pronouns when they

are arguments of periphrastic tense constructions (mainly the

θéλω +

infinitive future). In Chapter 4, it was shown that the pattern of pronoun
placement in these situations indicates that the pronoun is syntactically
the argument of

θéλω although semantically it remains the argument of

the infinitive. Accordingly, one would expect that as pronoun placement
became categorically preverbal, the pronouns would appear invariably to
the immediate left of

θéλω. Contrary to this expectation, however, the

data from the prose texts made available at the beginning of this chapter
indicate the tendency of the pronoun to appear between

θéλω and the

background image

134

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

infinitive even when the construction is immediately preceded by a fac-
tor that is otherwise associated with preverbal placement, such as a com-
plementizer or an interrogative pronoun (see examples 3 and 4).

But these would be counterexamples only if one assumes that there

has been no change in the periphrastic tense constructions. However, in
the case of the future periphrastic tense (which makes for the vast ma-
jority of examples) it has been well documented that it was not stable
during the period of Early Modern Greek. Instead, it has been estab-
lished for over a century now (cf. Psichari 1884, Meillet 1912, Bànescu
1915) that the

θéλω + infinitive constructions were undergoing a slow

but steady change which within two centuries would replace the content
word

θéλω with the function word θá. This change is chronicled in full

detail in Pappas and Joseph (2001, 2002). For the moment, it will suffice
to mention that one of the crucial steps in the change is the reanalysis of
the infinitive as a third person singular verb-form (after the regular loss
of final –

ν–) and the subsequent appearance of person marking on both

verb-forms of the periphrasis, which served as a competing variant. This
change must have taken place at least by the 14

th

century for in the Pou-

lologos there is the following example:

(28)

θéλουν

σè

ραβδíσουσιν

TElun

sE

ravDisusin

want–3pl Pres

you–DOsg WP

cane–3pl Pres

‘They will cane you’ (Poulologos, 618)

Thus, it can be maintained that by the time the authors of the prose

texts were writing (17

th

century), the status of

θéλω as the content (and

governing) verb in the periphrastic construction would have been weak-
ened, as a result of all the other variants of the construction in which
what used to be an infinitive came to have full finite verb morphology
(for a more detailed account of the development of the future periphrasis
with

θéλω and the counterfactual periphrasis with Wθελα see Pappas

1999, 2001b). This, coupled with the fact that the weak object pronoun
had always been the semantic argument of the lower verb (i.e., the in-
finitive), would have led to the perception that pronouns to the left of
θéλω were not adjacent to the verb that selected them, and, consequently
the order pronoun–

θéλω–infinitive was disfavored, much like pro-

noun–

θá–verb is not acceptable in Modern Greek, in accordance with

Behaghel’s third law: ‘What belongs together, in a mentalist sense, is

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

135

placed together’ (in Collinge 1985). In other words, it is not so much that
the pronouns in the prose texts are to the right of

θéλω, as they are to the

left of the infinitive.

Conclusion

The diachronic account provided in this chapter for the phenomenon of
weak object pronoun placement variation in Later Medieval Greek texts
is not only an attempt to clarify a problematic part of the history of the
Greek language and to identify the source and course of the change. It
also coherently explains why those who have attempted to provide syn-
chronic accounts of the phenomenon have not been able to account for
all the facts, by demonstrating that the key factor affecting pronoun
placement was not the phrase-structure relationships of the elements
involved—as they have assumed—but rather their linear position with
respect to each other. Furthermore, it was shown that this account pro-
vides a reasonable analysis not only for the main body of the variation
(i.e., that concerning the indicative and subjunctive verb-forms) but also
for aspects of the phenomenon that have been considered peripheral up
until now, namely the placement of the pronoun with imperatives and
gerunds, and in periphrastic constructions. All of these cases can be suc-
cessfully integrated as the various manifestations of the same process of
change, which was based on linearly determined models of word order.

background image

136

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

22

Jannsen (1998), however, remarks that in three non-literary documents

from the 17

th

century, which she examined, the pattern of pronoun

placement does not differ significantly from that found in Thysia tou
Abraam
. Nonetheless, she does admit that there are robust examples of
preverbal pronouns in clause-initial position and in ‘doubling pronoun’
constructions. Since Janssen does not provide the specific numbers, it is
difficult to compare her results to the ones presented here.

23

Such flexibility can be found in SMG verse as well. In a popular song

of the 1990’s,

Δε?ν πιστευςω ‘I don’t believe’, Helene¤ De¤mou sings:

(

[)

οZλ
α

στα? μεςτρα σου

κ ο μ μ ε ς ν α κ α ι ?
ραμμεςνα

ει\χε¨

ola

sta mEtra su

komEna kE ramEna

ixEs

all

to your measurement

cut and tailored

you had

‘You had everything according to your standards’

This line violates the constraint that the adjective

οZλο¨ when

functioning as an object must be reduplicated by a weak pronoun, in this
case

τας (see Mackridge 1985 and Chapter 4).

24

Indirect confirmation of this hypothesis about the conservative

character of the Cypriot dialect, at least with respect to pronoun
placement, comes from medieval Pontic, another variety of Greek that
became isolated from Byzantine Greek in the beginning of the LMG
period. Condoravdi and Kiparsky (2001) report the following
construction from a deed dated 1260, which they found in Ouspensky
and Bénéchevitch (1927).

(#)

ton de tópon

edókamén

soi

the Part place

give–1pl Past

you–IOsg WP

‘We gave you this land’

background image

A Diachronic Perspective

137

Here, as in the Cypriot data, the pronoun is placed postverbally even
though the immediately preceding element is a fronted constituent,
which in LMG is associated with preverbal pronoun placement.

25

This problem is also recognized by Rivero and Terzi’s (1995) article

on Modern Cypriot Greek. Their attempt to solve it is indicative of how
vexing this problem is for accounts in the GB/Minimalist approach:
‘Intuitively, the constituent before the clitic is of the type that counts as
first position. More technically, the strong feature is licensed because
the clitic is contained in the internal domain of an A-bar head made
visible in PF via the phonological content in either X

o

or Spec.’

Condoravdi and Kiparsky (2001: 8) rightly comment that this is ‘a
strange syntactic licensing requirement’.

26

It must be noted, however, that in the German and Dutch examples the

verbal morphology appears on the preceding elements. This is not the
case in Cypriot where the weak pronouns are attached to the following
verb.

background image

136

8

Theoretical Implications

In this final chapter, I discuss the theoretical implications of the conclu-
sions reached in the previous chapters. First, now that the facts about
pronoun placement have been exposed in full detail, I turn my attention
to the question of their status as clitics, a term that has been used by
other researchers to describe them. This question is considered within
the broader context of the necessity of the tripartite classification of
grammatical elements into words, clitics and affixes, and it is determined
that the weak object pronouns of Later Medieval Greek are best charac-
terized by the term ‘atypical affix’. Determining the status of the pro-
nouns has direct implications on the hypothesis of ‘unidirectionality’,
which is a seminal component of the theory of ‘grammaticalization’,
and, by extension, on whether we treat this theory as a separate mecha-
nism of linguistic change or as the aggregate of other processes. This
issue is discussed in the second section of the chapter. Another hypothe-
sis that is directly impacted by the results of this study is Kroch’s (1989)
claim that in order for disparate parts of a grammar to undergo the same
process of change, they must share abstract, not surface characteristics.
This claim is antithetical to the analysis of weak object pronoun place-
ment that was given in Chapter 7. The two opposing views are discussed
in the third section. The final section of the chapter discusses what is
perhaps the most surprising result of this study, namely, that there is no
generalization, or even a set of generalizations, that can account for the
behavior of weak object pronouns in LMG. Although such a claim may
go against the traditional view of what the objectives of linguistic analy-
sis should be, there have been several instances in which researchers
have had to propose grammars with less-than-perfect generalizations.
These case-studies are discussed in the final section and their conclu-
sions are used to illuminate the issue at hand.

background image

Theoretical Implications

137

Are the weak object pronouns of LMG clitics or affixes?

As was made clear in Chapter 6, the researchers that have put forward
proposals to account for the facts in Later Medieval Greek have all re-
ferred to the weak object pronouns as clitics (or clitic object pronouns).
However, that discussion also showed that the term clitic is interpreted
differently in each account sometimes without the appropriate argumen-
tation. For example, Wanner (1991) follows the guidelines of Kayne
(1975) and Rizzi (1986) and treats Old Spanish weak object pronouns as
clitics that adjoin to a head, even though there are, according to his own
analysis, several arguments that support treating them as clitics that ad-
join to phrasal categories. Fontana (1993, 1996, 1997) on the other hand,
argues that the same elements can only be phrasal category clitics. The
differences are even greater among the proposals that deal specifically
with the LMG facts. Mackridge’s (1993) view of clitichood seems to
rely mostly on the phonological dependence of these elements on their
hosts. Philippaki-Warburton (1995) assumes that they are phrasal cate-
gory clitics which are adjoined to the Tense/Agreement Phrase, without
offering any justification for this approach, or even considering their
status as enclitics or proclitics. Similarly, Condoravdi and Kiparsky
(2001) adopt Fontana’s treatment and analyze LMG pronouns as X

max

enclitics without, however, applying his argumentation for the specific
case. Finally, Horrocks (1997) treats them as clitics that project their
own phrasal category (ClP), yet, he too offers no support for this ap-
proach even though his analysis is unconventional.

Let’s begin the discussion about the nature of weak object pronouns in

Later Medieval Greek by revisiting Fontana’s argumentation for Old
Spanish. The reader is reminded that the crucial evidence in that case
was the presence of interpolation in the Old Spanish documents. For
Fontana, interpolation is evidence that the weak object pronouns of Old
Spanish are not necessarily bound to the verb, and thus can be treated as
‘true’ clitics or phrasal affixes. In arguing so, Fontana is indirectly fol-
lowing the criteria that Zwicky and Pullum (1983) and Zwicky (1985)
have proposed for examining the clitic status of grammatical elements.
For ease of exposition, I present a summary of these criteria based on
Nevis and Joseph (1993). According to these criteria, non-words are
distinguished from words by their strict ordering and their phonological
dependence, whereas words have relatively free ordering and are pho-
nologically independent. Furthermore, clitics can be told apart from af-
fixes because a) affixes show a higher degree of host selection than
clitics; b) the set of combinations between affix and stem is much more
likely to have arbitrary gaps than the set of combinations between clitic
and word; c) affixes are more likely to display morphophonological and

background image

138

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

semantic idiosyncrasies; d) affixes cannot be manipulated by syntactic
operations whereas clitics can be; and e) affixes are internally positioned
within the word whereas clitics are externally positioned. Clearly, the
possibility of interpolation in Old Spanish is evidence that the weak pro-
nouns do not show ‘high degree of host selection’ and would be consid-
ered clitics even in this typology.

The same cannot be said of Later Medieval Greek weak object pro-

nouns, however. Mackridge (1993) and Horrocks (1990, 1997) are in
agreement with the results of this study that weak object pronouns are, in
the vast majority of tokens, attached to the verb that selects them. Those
rare cases of interpolation in LMG texts can be shown to be instances of
archaic usage. More importantly, the weak pronouns of LMG are also
phonologically dependent on the verb. Unfortunately, the other criteria
cannot be tested. Even this partial information, however, suggests that
they behave more like the weak pronouns in Modern Greek and the
Modern Romance languages than the weak pronouns of Old Spanish.
Indeed, the high selectivity of host that they exhibit could lead us to
classify them as verbal affixes, a position that is taken for Standard
Modern Greek (Joseph 1988, 1989, 1990, 1992, 2001b, 2002, Halpern
1995), Modern French (Miller and Sag 1997, Halpern 1995), and Mod-
ern Spanish (Klavans 1985, Fontana 1993, 1996, 1997). Unlike these
languages though, Later Medieval Greek has alternation between prever-
bal and postverbal placement of the weak pronouns that is not deter-
mined by the finiteness of the verb-form. According to Zwicky, (1985:
§2.34) elements that exhibit free order with adjacent words ‘cannot be
affixes and even clitics do not normally show freedom of order with
their host.’ Later Medieval Greek weak pronouns, then, embody a set of
contradicting characteristics as far as their morphological classification
is concerned.

This problem of classification may only be a consequence of the at-

tempt to carve up morphosyntactic space into three independent parts, in
order to accommodate the terms ‘word’, ‘clitic’, and ‘affix’. Such a par-
tition, however, may not be necessary. In fact, as early as his 1985 article
‘Clitics and Particles’, Zwicky is uncertain that clitics should be elevated
to the same categorial status as words and affixes: ‘clitics are more
marked than either inflectional affixes or independent syntactic units
(i.e., words)’ (1985: §2.6). Zwicky (1994) goes even further, maintaining
that ‘clitic … is an umbrella term, not a genuine category in grammatical
theory. Umbrella terms are names for problems, for phenomena that pre-
sent “mixed” properties of some kind, not names of theoretical con-
structs’ (1994: xiii).

Following the lead of Zwicky, Joseph (2002: §2) rejects the term

‘clitic’ arguing that the three-way analysis of morphological/syntactic

background image

Theoretical Implications

139

primitives into ‘affix’ vs. ‘clitic’ vs. ‘word’ is uninformative. Instead, he
opts for a simple bipartite division of affix vs. word together with the
recognition that ‘there are typical (i.e., “core”) and atypical (i.e., “mar-
ginal” or “marked”) members.’ The elements usually referred to as
clitics could be divided into two categories: atypical words (cf. Zwicky’s
‘bound words’), and atypical affixes (cf. Klavan’s phrasal affixes).

If anything has been made clear in this study, it is that the weak pro-

nouns in LMG are atypical. But are they atypical words or atypical af-
fixes? The answer to this question depends crucially on the characteriza-
tion of the modal markers

νá, and ς, and the negative markers μν and

δéν. For if these are shown to be affixes, then the weak pronouns, which
must appear between these elements and the verb cannot be words. This
holds, of course, if we assume that ‘lexical integrity’, the general prop-
erty of words according to which a syntactic operation cannot refer ex-
clusively only to a part of a word (Spencer 1989), is a constraint that
even atypical forms cannot violate.

Following the argumentation of Joseph (1988, 1990), where the affixal

character of these same elements for Standard Modern Greek is estab-
lished, it can also be argued that these four elements are not word-like in
LMG. First, they too show high selectivity of host, appearing always
before verbs. Furthermore, the linear position of these elements is fixed
both with respect to the host (always preverbal) and with respect to each
other (

νá before μν, also ς before μν). Next, there is the arbitrary

exclusion of

δéν as a negator after both νá and ς (i.e., *νà δéν) and the

fact that none of these elements appears independently. There is no ex-
ample of *

UΑς! ‘Let’s!’ Both of these arguments, of course, cannot be

absolutely substantiated since there is no direct evidence that the hypo-
thetical sequences were not allowed. However, the fact that there are,
literally, hundreds of examples of

νà μν and ς μν, but no examples

of *

νà δéν or *ς δéν should be taken as proof that the latter were not

grammatical. Finally, with respect to morphophonological idiosyncrasies
there are some tokens of the form

νáν in the prose texts found in Baletas

(1949), one of which is shown in example (1):

(1)

διà

νàν

τοùς

φουρκíσει

Dja

nan

tus

furkisi

in order to

Subjun

he–DOpl WP

hang–3sg Pres

‘In order to hang them’ (Soumake¤s, 1616—Zakynthos)

The presence of the final –

ν– on νáν cannot be explained by any

regular synchronic (morpho)phonological alternations, nor can it be mo-
tivated from a diachronic perspective; there was never a final –

ν– on να

and, indeed, final –

ν– was eliminated from the language during the mid-

background image

140

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

dle Byzantine period (6

th

-12

th

century, cf. Joseph 1978/1990, 1983a,

Horrocks 1997). However, this observation intersects crucially with the
well-known fact (cf. Householder, Kazazis, and Koutsoudas 1964, Jo-
seph 1988, 1989) that for some speakers of Modern Greek the initial /t/
of the third person weak pronoun becomes voiced when preceded by the
future marker

θá or the modal marker νá. Thus νà τò κáνω /na to

kano

/ is pronounced [na do kano], although such intervocalic voicing is

otherwise unattested in Greek. The sporadic appearance of final –

ν– on

the marker

νá may be a written representation of the irregular voicing of

the following voiceless consonant, and could be used to support the view
that this cluster of preverbal elements manifested morphophonological
idiosyncrasies as early as the 17

th

century. Alternatively, one may con-

sider that these irregular spellings of

νá indicate the historical origin of

the irregular voicing that has been observed. It is possible that the linear
position similarities between

δéν, μν, θá, and νá, lead to the analogical

(but sporadic) extension of the final –

ν– to the latter two particles, and

the series nasal + voiceless stop yielded a voiced stop (also sporadic).

If these markers were indeed affixes in Later Medieval Greek, the

weak object pronouns of the same period should also be considered af-
fixes since they appear between the verb and the marker. All evidence
supports the view that weak pronouns in LMG were affixes, albeit atypi-
cal ones, since they could appear either immediately to the right of the
verb or immediately to the left of it. However, they are not unique. In
fact, a very similar atypical affix can be found in Lithuanian. Nevis and
Joseph (1993) discuss the reflexive element –s(i)–, which typically ap-
pears postverbally at the end of non-prefixed verbs and verbal deriva-
tives, but, when one or more preverbal prefixed elements are attached to
the verb, the reflexive appears after the first such element. The distribu-
tion is illustrated in the examples below (2 and 3), which are taken from
Nevis and Joseph (1993: 95).

(2)

laikau$

laikau$

–si

‘I consider’

‘I get along’

(3)

is*

–laikau$

is*

–si–laikau$

‘I preserve, withstand’

‘I hold my stand’

Nevis and Joseph further demonstrate that this reflexive exhibits high

degree of host selection, some unpredictable gaps in its combinatory
possibilities, and morphophonological idiosyncrasies; it also does not
participate in any syntactic rules separately from the verb. Based on this
evidence they conclude that although mobility is highly marked for an
affix, Lithuanian reflexive –s(i)– is best treated as such, because the al-

background image

Theoretical Implications

141

ternative (treating it as a clitic) would require violating ‘lexical integ-
rity’; they also propose that –s(i)– is a ‘Wackernagel affix’ (Nevis and
Joseph 1993: 97):

An affix-and-word Wackernagel’s Law parallel to the clitic-and-sen-
tence and the clitic-and-phrase versions of Wackernagel’s Law is ac-
tually expected on theoretical grounds … and on diachronic grounds,
given that syntax so often develops into morphology over time … ac-
cordingly, Wackernagelian syntax ought to be able to give rise to
Wackernagelian morphology.

The results of this research into the pattern of pronoun placement

variation in LMG texts fit well into a framework with just a two-way
division between word and affix as primary morphological elements.
The LMG evidence suggests that such a framework may be better suited
for understanding the subtle differentiations between forms that occupy
the morphosyntactic continuum than the three-way partition that in-
cludes a separate category for clitics. Certainly, to the extent that clitics
are a marked phenomenon (cf. Zwicky 1985), if the facts can be ac-
counted for with a more restrictive framework, that is to be preferred.
Later Medieval Greek therefore, offers another example of a mobile af-
fix, an entity that has rare attestations, but may prove to be more com-
mon once the umbrella category of clitic is abandoned.

The development of ‘atypical affixes’ in LMG and the theory
of ‘grammaticalization’

The proposal that the weak object pronoun forms of Later Medieval
Greek are best considered mobile affixes, has further implications for
proposals which claim the existence of such a mechanism of change as
‘grammaticalization’ (also referred to as ‘grammaticization’, or even
‘grammatization’). This is a mechanism by which lexical items lose their
independent phonological and syntactic status, as well as their specific
semantic meaning and become more grammatical in nature (cf. Camp-
bell and Janda 2001 for an overview of the different interpretations of
the term and related bibliography). In Hopper and Traugott (1993: 132,
135) a subsort of grammaticalization, ‘morphologization’ is defined as
‘that part of grammaticalization that primarily involves the second and
third parts of the cline: lexical item > clitic > affix’, while later they state
that ‘Morphologization involves the creation of a bound morpheme (i.e.,
an affix) out of an independent word by way of cliticization. The final
stage of this process, the uniting of the affix with its stem, is referred to
as “univerbation”.’

background image

142

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

Whether the change discussed here is viewed in the traditional way

(i.e., LMG clitic > SMG affix), or according to the analysis in the first
section as a change from LMG mobile affix > SMG affix, it is obvious
that it would be considered as an instantiation of the process of mor-
phologization and therefore of the more general process of grammati-
calization. As such, the evidence discussed in Chapters 3-5, and the
analysis of the phenomenon proposed in Chapter 7 offer two significant
observations.

The first one concerns the hypothesis of ‘unidirectionality’, which

according to Hopper and Traugott (1993) means that the process of
grammaticalization can only lead from a less grammatical item to a more
grammatical item and not vice versa, while Herring (1991: 253) de-
scribed it as a ‘linear and irreversible process’. However, several coun-
terexamples to this hypothesis have been identified (some of them by
Hopper and Traugott 1993, but see also Joseph and Janda 1988, Camp-
bell 1991, 2001, Janda 1995, 2001, Norde 2001, Ramat 1998), to which
the results of this study add yet another one. As was mentioned in
Chapter 7, Horrocks (1990, see also Chapter 1) gives a description of
Koiné and Early Medieval Greek pronoun placement in which postver-
bal placement is more or less categorical. As was argued above, this type
of immobility of an element is considered typical behavior for an affix.
In fact, in the language of Hopper and Traugott, the reported fixed posi-
tion of weak object pronouns to the immediate right of the verb could be
interpreted as the result of univerbation, which, according to them, is the
final stage of grammaticalization. If this description is indeed accurate,
then in the change from Early Medieval Greek to Later Medieval Greek,
there is evidence of these elements ‘freeing’ themselves from the post-
verbal position and, in essence, reversing the process by becoming
atypical affixes, after having been completely grammaticalized. Al-
though the movement is not grand in scale (since we are dealing with
affixes in both Early and Later Medieval Greek), it is still movement
away from a well-behaved type of affix to an atypical one. Since the
supporters of grammaticalization and unidirectionality view the differ-
ence between words and affixes as a cline, the change in the status of
weak object pronouns in Medieval Greek must be interpreted as a
change in the direction of more word-like behavior, which according to
their usual claims should not be possible. Thus, depending on the accu-
racy of Horrock’s description of EMG pronoun placement, the LMG
data may provide yet another counterexample to the unidirectionality
hypothesis.

Furthermore, as Joseph (2001a: 166) has argued, the use of unidirec-

tionality as a constraint on grammaticalization implies that the latter is a
separate process of language change:

background image

Theoretical Implications

143

In particular, a constraint requiring movement only in one direction
could be entertained if grammaticalization is a separate and distinct
process or mechanism, since other processes or mechanisms or
change seem not to be constrained in that way; i.e., other recognized
mechanisms of change, especially sound change, analogy, or
reanalysis, do not seem to be subject to a constraint like unidirection-
ality … if grammaticalization is a process in and of itself, a mecha-
nism of change that is separate and distinct from other mechanisms of
change, then it could in principle be subject to a constraint like unidi-
rectionality …

In the same article, however, Joseph, examines two instances of change
in the history of Greek, namely the emergence of weak subject pronouns
(

τος, τη, το, see also Chapter 1), and the formation of the future marker

θá, and shows that, although these two cases may appear on the surface
to be typical instantiations of grammaticalization, a closer look reveals a
much more complex development, which can be accounted for by refer-
ence to already known processes of change. In a similar vein, Joseph
(2001a) and Pappas and Joseph (2001) argue that grammaticalization is
best thought of as an epiphenomenon, a concise way of describing the
result of other processes rather than as a separate mechanism of change.

The analysis of the rise of preverbal pronoun placement from Later

Medieval to Early Modern Greek further supports this view of gram-
maticalization as an epiphenomenon. For, on the one hand, the change,
when viewed from the perspective of the point of departure and the end
point, appears to be a normal case of morphologization in which clitics
(or atypical affixes) become affixes. The detailed examination of the
change, however, given in Chapter 7, revealed that the appearance and
rise of preverbal pronoun placement can be explained by reference to
reanalysis (of the marker

νá as the verbal head) and analogical spread of

the ensuing word-order pattern according to the linear position of the
relevant elements. Crucially, there is no evidence that a change in the
character of the weak pronouns (e.g., from atypical to less atypical af-
fixes) preceded the change in their placement. Thus, although the term
‘grammaticalization’ may be a convenient way to refer to the result of
the change, it does not offer any insight into how the change actually
took place.

Concrete contexts of linguistic change

In 1989, Anthony Kroch published an overview of a number of cases of
syntactic change—including his own reexamination of the emergence of
English periphrastic do (Kroch 1982/1989)—in which he challenged the

background image

144

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

traditional view of language change that ‘those contexts in which the
new form is more common are those in which the form first appears and
in which it advances most rapidly’ (1989: 199). In particular, Kroch has
proposed that in order to gain an accurate understanding of language
change, raw frequency data should be modeled by some nonlinear
mathematical function:

Given the S-shape of curves of change, we cannot use raw frequency
data to give us a single parameter for the rate of increase in the use of
the innovative form. This is because at the beginning and end of a
linguistic change, the rate of increase is low, while in the middle of a
change it is very high. In order to associate such a nonlinear evolution
with a single rate, it is necessary to model it with some nonlinear
mathematical function. I chose the logistic, as it is the simplest ap-
propriate function and the one commonly used to model cases of
competition in other fields. (Kroch 1997: 142)

As a result, the S-shaped curve is transformed to a straight line in

which the rate of change is encoded as the slope of this line. Thus, if
lines representing change in different contexts have the same slope, then
this can be taken as proof that the rate of change is the same for all con-
texts. This has proven to be the case not only for the studies surveyed in
Kroch’s (1989) article but also in a number of other studies, including,
Fontana (1993), Santorini (1993), Taylor (1994), and Pintzuk (1995).
Kroch has identified it as the ‘Constant Rate Effect’ (also referred to as
the ‘Uniform Rate Hypothesis’) ‘in all linguistic contexts in which an
innovative form is in competition with a conservative one, the use of the
innovative form increases over time at the same rate, when this increase
is measured on the logistic scale’ (Kroch 1997: 140).

One of the important conclusions of Kroch’s (1982/1989) study of the

rise of periphrastic do in English was that its use did not spread from
context to context (e.g., from negative declarative to affirmative de-
clarative sentences); instead it was actuated simultaneously in all con-
texts. On the other hand, it has been argued here that during the rise of
preverbal pronoun placement in the history of Greek, this pattern did not
appear simultaneously in all contexts, but spread analogically, especially
if the data from the Cypriot chronicles is interpreted not simply as a re-
flection of dialectal differentiation but also as indicative of an early pe-
riod in the process of change. However, it would be wrong to interpret
this finding as a direct contradiction of the CRE, because Kroch’s con-
clusion is not solely based on the discovery of the effect.

Instead, his claim about the spread of do is based on the evidence pro-

vided by Ellegård that do appeared simultaneously in all contexts. Ac-

background image

Theoretical Implications

145

cording to Kroch (1997: 143) it is ‘… this evidence, in conjunction with
the CRE, [that] leads to the conclusion that there is no spread of do from
context to context’ [emphasis added]. The logistic model itself in which
the CRE is detected ‘is neutral between an interpretation under which
one context precedes another in time in its adoption of the innovative
form and one under which the first context only uses the innovative form
more often than the other’ (ibid: 142). Thus, although the change from
postverbal to preverbal pronoun placement in the history of Greek, as
analyzed here, differs in essential ways from Kroch’s analysis of the rise
of English periphrastic do, it does not directly contradict the hypothesis
of the CRE. Such a challenge could have been mounted only if the data
from the LMG texts allowed for a test of the rate of change to be con-
ducted. However, this is not possible, both because the data cannot be
accurately dated, as has been discussed in Chapter 2, and because almost
all of the texts are written in verse, a style that appears to have remained
conservative with respect to pronoun placement, a point that was demon-
strated in Chapter 7.

Nonetheless, the results of this study do present a challenge for the

way that Kroch uses the CRE to shed light on the mechanism of syntac-
tic change. As he states in his article (1989: 201):

… our results show that the grammatical analysis that defines the
contexts of a change is quite abstract. We see that the set of contexts
that change together is not defined by the sharing of a surface prop-
erty, like the appearance of a particular word or morpheme, but rather
by a shared syntactic structure whose existence can only be the prod-
uct of an abstract grammatical analysis on the part of the speakers.

In the case of periphrastic do the particular abstract grammatical

analysis concerns which types of verbs could be raised to Infl; all verbs
(conservative structure), or auxiliaries only (innovative structure). Ac-
cording to Kroch it is the competition between these two structures that
brings about the pattern of variation in the usage of periphrastic do in the
transition from Middle to Modern English.

However, the analysis presented in this study has demonstrated that

the variation in weak object pronoun placement in LMG cannot be ex-
plained by the type of abstract structural differences that Kroch makes
reference to. In Chapters 6 and 7, it was shown that all such explanations
which have been proposed so far fail to capture the facts, precisely be-
cause they refer to the mechanism of verb movement and the abstract
structural relationships that can or cannot license it. Instead, it was ar-
gued there that the change to preverbal pronoun placement evident in the
history of Greek, although initiated by a change in the abstract analysis

background image

146

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

of morphemes (i.e., the interpretation of

νá as the head of the subjunc-

tive), was spread due to a surface similarity, namely the linear position
of the relevant elements, and that this spread took place gradually. Thus,
the evidence from Later Medieval Greek weak object pronoun placement
supports the view that the contexts of a syntactic change can be concrete
as well as abstract, an observation that is in direct contrast with Kroch’s
(1989) proposal.

As a final point, it must be noted that, in the type of studies conducted

by Kroch and his students, and also in the one presented here, the pat-
terns revealed by the variationist analysis concern the language of the
written texts primarily and not colloquial speech. And although it can be
argued that certain texts may approximate the vernacular of a period
more than others, textual evidence should not be used as the basis for
such strong hypotheses as the CRE. For the act of writing, regardless of
subject matter, always involves considerably more care than casual
speech. Furthermore, as Janda and Joseph (2003) observe, ‘the order in
which specific changes appear in written language need not reflect the
order in which they first appeared in colloquial speech.’ This point was
illustrated in Chapter 7 where it was argued that a particular genre of
composition (poetry) preserved a pattern of variation for a period of five
or more centuries. As a consequence, the results of this study reinforce
the point made by Janda and Joseph that ‘empirical verification of the
Uniform Rate Hypothesis [=CRE] will not be forthcoming until students
of syntactic change begin to carry out serious long-term investigations of
ongoing developments in contemporary colloquial speech.’

Grammars with less-than-perfect generalizations

For some readers, the most surprising conclusion reached in this study

may be that there is no generalization that captures the facts about weak
object pronoun placement in Later Medieval Greek. In fact, as has been
stated already in Chapters 1 and 7, the search for such a generalization
has been the main driving force behind this project. Despite, however,
the vigorous pursuit of this goal, it appears impossible to write a rule that
makes appeal to general categories of the grammar and accurately de-
scribes the facts. Instead, the diachronic evidence points toward a change
from postverbal to preverbal pronoun placement that spread from one
class of elements to another, yielding intermediate stages for which no
generalization can be stated. Generativists will, of course, ask how it is
possible to have a grammar (or a subsystem of a grammar) without gen-
erative rules, and the answer that a part of the grammar may be deter-

background image

Theoretical Implications

147

mined according to a list of lexical items will, most probably, seem theo-
retically unappealing to them.

Nonetheless, and despite how unappealing it may seem, such a situa-

tion is not unprecedented. Gross and his colleagues spent a number of
years trying to construct a transformational-generative grammar of
French. This, however, proved to be impossible. Gross (1979: 860)
states that ‘… if we compare the syntactic properties of any two lexical
items—it is observed that no two lexical items have identical syntactic
properties.’ As a result, he and his researchers ‘… were forced to con-
clude that [they] could obtain no generalization without a reasonably
complete study of the lexical items of the language and their syntactic
uses’ (ibid: 861). This conclusion is even more extreme than the one
reached in this study since in the case of Later Medieval Greek pronoun
placement one can at least identify the major parameters that play a role
in the pattern of variation.

In another study, Gropen et al. (1989) investigated the acquisition by

children of the dative alternation (e.g., I give him money vs. I give
money to him). They also conducted an experiment in which adults had
to rate the acceptability of the two constructions with nonce words (e.g.,
norp, pell, orgulate). The results of the first study showed that while the
acquisition of this alternation by children is not strictly conservative, that
is there is evidence of productivity in a child’s use, these instances are
only a ‘tiny minority of children’s usages’.

… the vast majority of the verbs that they used in both dative con-
structions could have been based on forms actually used by the par-
ticular adults that interacted with the children. In the experiments,
children always used verbs more often in the constructions they heard
them used in than in related construction requiring the application of
a rule, and extensive exposure to a construction with other verbs was
required to get them to use that construction productively at all. (Gro-
pen et al. 1989: 239)

Even more significantly, the authors point out that even adults show

several exceptions to this rule that pose an embarrassing question. Ex-
amples (4) through (7) (ibid) pair verbs that are quite similar semanti-
cally and morphophonologically, yet one (a) can be used in the dative
construction, while the other (b) cannot. If the dative alternation is gov-
erned by a generalization, then why do these exceptions exist? Gropen et
al. could not find a set of criteria that can account for these differences.

(4)

a.

John threw Bill the box

b.

*John pulled Bill the box

background image

148

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

(5)

a.

Sam told John the story

b.

*Sam shouted John the story

(6)

a.

Becky promised Bill the money

b.

*Becky credited Bill the money

(7)

a.

Irv found her a dress

b.

*Irv chose her a dress

More recently, Joseph (1997) examined the nature of generalizations

in phonological, morphological and syntactic constructions. He draws
from the results of previous work (Neikirk 1996, Kim 1996, Gross 1979,
and others) as well as introducing new observations (e.g., the formation
of plural for borrowed nouns in English, and the accent pattern of Greek
neuter nouns that end in –

ι /i/). In all of these cases, a close examination

of the facts reveals that none of the constructions can be captured with a
generalization that can account for all possibilities. Accordingly, Joseph
(1997: 150) reaches the conclusion that:

… while linguistic generalizations are important and to some extent
reflect linguistic competence, … we should not be surprised to find
behavior that is contradictory to apparent generalizations, not only
across speakers but within speakers as well … the type of information
that speakers have access to again and again seems to be facts about
particular lexical items … this lexically particularized knowledge,
gives us crucial insights into the nature of what speakers know about
their language.

Furthermore, Joseph (ibid: 157) proposes that most generalizations

should have ‘restricted scope’ and be ‘sufficiently localized’, and that
this new type of generalizations can be described synchronically by us-
ing the constructs ‘constellations’ and ‘(partial) meta-templates’. These
concepts have been described by Joseph and Janda in several publica-
tions (Janda and Joseph 1986, 1989, 1992, and in preparation, as well as
Joseph and Janda 1988, and Joseph and Janda 1999) and defined as
follows:

1. The constellation: a group of elements which share at least one
characteristic property of form but are distinguished by individual
idiosyncrasies, both of form and of function, that prevent their being
collapsed with one another.

background image

Theoretical Implications

149

2. The meta-redundancy-statement, or ‘(partial) meta-template’,
which equates (or ‘parses’) all relevant instances of a particular for-
mal configuration, (Joseph and Janda 1999: 342).

Gropen et al. have a similar proposal that accounts for the com-

plexities of the use of dative alternation. They suggest that the facts are
best captured by the synergy of two types of rules. First, there is a
‘broad-range’ rule which ‘dictates the kinds of properties that a double-
object form must have if it exists’. However, a verb will not necessarily
appear in both constructions simply by virtue of satisfying these condi-
tions. For this, the authors suggest that speakers also possess a set of
‘narrow range’ rules, each of which applies to ‘narrow sets of verbs with
similar kinds of meanings’. These narrow range rules, which are remi-
niscent of Joseph’s lexically particularized rules are the existence-pre-
dicting rules that allow the formation of particular dative constructions.

Another study that found evidence for the existence of lexically de-

fined rules is that of Bybee and Slobin (1982). In their investigation of
the development and use of English past-tense forms, they found that
although many of these forms are memorized and stored in the lexicon,
there are still classes of verbs described by ‘schemas’, which are formed
on the basis of the past-tense form. Even more interesting is Bybee and
Slobin’s observation that these schemas describe ‘product-oriented’
classes, that is the specific verbs are grouped together because they have
the same vowel in the past-tense form. The base form of the verb is not
as relevant. As a concrete example, the authors offer the verbs strike,
stick, sneak which have different vowels in the base form but the same
one in the past-tense form—struck, stuck, snuck.

Such approaches can also be employed in the explanation of the Later

Medieval Greek data, and the case of reduplicated object constructions
that involve the adjective

λος is a good example of the interaction of

the different types of rules. The reader is reminded that, although redu-
plicated object constructions are overwhelmingly associated with post-
verbal pronoun placement, when the reduplicated object is the adjective
λος, the pronoun is placed postverbally in only 14 out of 38 tokens (cf.
Chapter 4). This peculiar pattern can be viewed as the result of tension
between a broad-range rule that positions pronouns after a verb when the
immediately preceding element is a reduplicated object, and a lexically
particularized rule that places pronouns preverbally if the immediately
preceding element is

λος. More specifically, since this narrow rule is

most likely the result of an analogical change that is based on the model
of pronoun placement associated with the

λος + possessive pronoun

construction, it can be treated as a product-oriented schema in the sense
of Bybee and Slobin (1982). Despite the fact that such an analysis does

background image

150

Variation and Morphosyntactic Change in Greek

not offer a grand solution for the phenomenon of weak object pronoun
placement in Later Medieval Greek, it does provide a viable account as
to why certain exceptions exist. Given that under other proposals such
exceptions are simply swept under the rug, this perspective undoubtedly
sheds more light on the phenomenon than any other previous account.

It must be stressed—though it should already be clear—that these con-

siderations do not mean abandoning the search for generalizations. This
remains a powerful heuristic tool that has led to better understanding of
what constitutes ‘knowledge-of-language’ and will, undoubtedly, con-
tinue to do so. The real issue here is our preconceived notions of what a
grammar should look like. It has already been mentioned in Chapter 2
that the structuralist ideal of homogeneity, which also underlies the gen-
erativist program through the use of the concept of ‘idiolect’, is vigor-
ously challenged by the fact that languages change. Weinreich, Labov
and Herzog (1968: 101) proposed that in order to solve this paradox, one
must ‘break down the identification of stucturedness with homogeneity’
and argued that ‘the possibility of describing orderly differentiation in a
language serving a community’ is essential for understanding how lan-
guages can change without losing any of their communicative efficiency.
However, the construct that Weinreich, Labov and Herzog proposed to
explain structured heterogeneity, namely the ‘variable rule’, has been
proven to have adequate descriptive power but no explanatory force (cf.
Romaine 1981, Dittmar 1996, Fasold 1996). On the other hand, con-
structs such as the notion of ‘constellations’ described above provide not
only a way of describing orderly differentiation within the language of
an individual speaker, but also a model for understanding how speakers
are able to treat certain groups of linguistic elements as being ‘the same’
in some constructions but ‘different’ in others.

Conclusion

The above discussion shows that, although the main focus of this study

has been the detailed examination of the variation in weak object pro-
noun placement in Later Medieval Greek, the results of this research
have significant implications for hypotheses about the nature of morpho-
syntactic change, such as ‘unidirectionality’ and the ‘constant rate ef-
fect’. This study also provides more material for consideration in the on-
going debate over the nature of clitics and whether or not they should be
considered a separate primary morphological element, and, on a more
theoretical level, questions some of the established assumptions about
the nature and scope that generalizations should have in the grammars
that we construct. Furthermore, the discussion here together with the
analysis in Chapter 7 have highlighted the need for an equally thorough

background image

Theoretical Implications

151

examination of weak pronoun placement in Early Medieval Greek, be-
cause it appears that many crucial conclusions have hinged on Horrocks’
decidedly brief description of these facts. Finally, it must be noted that
an exploration of pronoun placement variation in non-standard dialects
of Greek (especially the southeastern ones—Khios, Rhodos, Crete, Cy-
prus) is also needed. For, although some descriptions exist, they are for
the most part epigrammatic and quite uninformative, when one considers
how complex the pattern of Later Medieval Greek pronoun placement
has turned out to be. The full range of facts, however, would prove to be
very insightful, because the process of the change will be fully under-
stood only when all of its outcomes are catalogued in detail.

background image

152

Appendix

Table A.1

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors initial and coordinat-

ing conjunction

Factor

Æ

Initial

Coordinating

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

1

25

3

69

Pto

¤

khoprod.

0

18

0

41

Glykas

0

15

0

4

Spaneas

0

12

0

18

Kallimakhos

2

37

2

11

Lybistros

5

117

0

47

Achilleid

0

59

2

49

Belisarios

0

5

0

21

Thre

¤

nos

0

18

0

13

Poulologos

0

24

1

39

Paidiophr.

0

20

0

16

Physiologos

0

11

1

19

Spanos

1

22

2

7

Aito

¤

los

0

5

3

1

Moreas

17

14

1

33

Tokkoi

0

30

1

56

Rimada

4

51

18

47

Gioustos

3

4

7

3

Depharanas

3

8

7

4

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

1

26

4

30

Phalieros

11

42

4

31

Homilia

1

3

0

6

Apokopos

5

35

0

4

Apollo

¤

nios

1

35

1

35

Phlo

¤

rios

2

48

1

29

Rhodos

1

7

0

13

Katalogia

1

28

0

35

Total

59

719

58

681

background image

Appendix

153

Table A.2

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors

διóτι and τι

Factor

Æ

διóτι

τι

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

0

0

1

3

Pto

¤

khoprod.

0

0

0

1

Glykas

0

0

0

0

Spaneas

0

0

0

0

Kallimakhos

0

0

0

0

Lybistros

0

0

0

2

Achilleid

0

0

0

1

Belisarios

0

0

0

0

Thre

¤

nos

0

0

0

0

Poulologos

0

0

0

1

Paidiophr.

0

0

0

0

Physiologos

0

0

0

2

Spanos

0

0

0

0

Aito

¤

los

0

0

0

0

Moreas

0

0

2

2

Tokkoi

1

0

2

1

Rimada

2

0

0

1

Gioustos

0

0

0

2

Depharanas

0

0

0

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

0

0

1

0

Phalieros

1

0

1

0

Homilia

0

0

0

0

Apokopos

0

0

0

0

Apollo

¤

nios

0

0

0

1

Phlo

¤

rios

0

0

0

3

Rhodos

0

0

0

1

Katalogia

0

0

1

9

Total

4

0

8

30

background image

154

Appendix

Table A.3

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors

ο!κ", δé and redupli-

cated object

Factor

Æ

οκ

δé

Red. Object

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

0

11

2

1

1

5

Pto

¤

khoprod.

0

6

0

1

1

1

Glykas

0

4

0

0

0

0

Spaneas

0

3

0

1

0

1

Kallimakhos

0

1

0

0

0

3

Lybistros

0

6

0

1

2

10

Achilleid

0

6

1

2

0

6

Belisarios

0

1

0

0

0

1

Thre

¤

nos

0

1

0

2

0

6

Poulologos

0

3

0

0

2

7

Paidiophr.

0

0

0

0

0

6

Physiologos

0

0

0

2

0

1

Spanos

0

0

0

0

1

0

Aito

¤

los

0

0

0

0

5

0

Moreas

0

2

0

0

0

1

Tokkoi

0

1

0

0

5

4

Rimada

0

1

0

0

8

4

Gioustos

0

0

0

0

4

4

Depharanas

0

0

0

0

2

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

0

0

0

0

2

0

Phalieros

0

0

0

0

1

1

Homilia

0

1

0

0

1

0

Apokopos

0

1

0

0

0

1

Apollo

¤

nios

0

1

0

1

0

4

Phlo

¤

rios

0

3

0

0

2

5

Rhodos

0

0

0

0

2

3

Katalogia

0

5

0

0

0

5

Total

0

57

3

11

39

79

background image

Appendix

155

Table A.4

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors relative pronoun,

negation, and interrogative pronoun

Factor

Æ

Rel. Pron

Negation

Q-pronoun

Text

Ø

Pre V Post V Pre V Post V Pre V Post V

Digene

¤

s

9

0

34

0

21

0

Pto

¤

khoprod. 12

0

17

1

14

0

Glykas

5

0

10

0

0

0

Spaneas

7

0

38

0

6

0

Kallimakhos 6

0

2

1

3

1

Lybistros

21

0

18

0

13

0

Achilleid

20

0

23

1

4

1

Belisarios

0

0

5

0

3

0

Thre

¤

nos

7

1

18

0

5

0

Poulologos

8

0

14

0

12

0

Paidiophr.

1

0

3

0

1

0

Physiologos 0

0

3

0

0

0

Spanos

3

1

5

0

1

0

Aito

¤

los

5

0

6

0

4

0

Moreas

32

0

8

0

11

0

Tokkoi

18

0

11

0

6

0

Rimada

25

0

35

0

9

0

Gioustos

16

0

30

0

12

0

Depharanas 14

0

19

0

5

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

12

0

14

0

9

0

Phalieros

12

0

36

0

4

0

Homilia

6

0

4

0

2

0

Apokopos

7

0

9

0

3

0

Apollo

¤

nios

7

0

4

0

0

0

Phlo

¤

rios

7

0

14

0

5

0

Rhodos

6

1

11

0

3

0

Katalogia

9

0

40

0

8

0

Total

275

3

431

3

164

2

background image

156

Appendix

Table A.5

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors

νá, να, and ς

Factor

Æ

νá

να

ς

Text

Ø

Pre V Post V Pre V Post V Pre V Post V

Digene

¤

s

95

2

8

0

10

0

Pto

¤

khoprod. 73

0

1

0

6

0

Glykas

11

0

0

0

2

0

Spaneas

50

0

1

0

1

0

Kallimakhos 15

0

0

0

2

0

Lybistros

129

0

0

0

4

0

Achilleid

82

0

10

0

6

0

Belisarios

10

0

0

0

0

0

Thre

¤

nos

76

1

0

0

2

1

Poulologos

25

0

0

0

0

0

Paidiophr.

14

0

0

0

1

0

Physiologos 9

0

2

0

0

0

Spanos

15

0

1

0

1

0

Aito

¤

los

23

0

0

0

0

0

Moreas

98

0

0

0

6

0

Tokkoi

102

0

1

0

1

0

Rimada

75

0

0

0

1

0

Gioustos

48

0

0

0

2

0

Depharanas 54

0

0

0

0

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

59

0

0

0

1

0

Phalieros

80

0

0

0

6

0

Homilia

14

0

0

0

1

0

Apokopos

20

0

0

0

0

0

Apollo

¤

nios

60

0

0

0

2

0

Phlo

¤

rios

63

0

0

0

3

0

Rhodos

38

0

0

0

3

0

Katalogia

98

0

0

0

4

0

Total

1436 3

24

0

65

1

background image

Appendix

157

Table A.6

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors temporal/comparative

conjunction,

áν, ν, and πẃς

Factor

Æ

Temp/Compr. conj

áν, ν

πẃς

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V Post V Pre V Post V

Digene

¤

s

22

0

11

0

0

0

Pto

¤

choprod. 8

1

10

0

0

0

Glykas

1

0

0

0

0

0

Spaneas

6

0

11

0

0

0

Kallimakhos 2

0

2

0

0

0

Lybistros

12

0

5

0

0

0

Achilleid

6

0

1

0

0

0

Belisarios

6

0

2

0

0

0

Thre

¤

nos

7

0

3

0

0

0

Poulologos

3

0

3

0

0

0

Paidiophr.

1

0

0

0

0

0

Physiologos 0

0

0

0

0

0

Spanos

5

0

0

0

0

0

Aito

¤

los

3

0

0

0

0

0

Moreas

45

0

1

0

1

0

Tokkoi

9

0

4

0

0

0

Rimada

27

0

5

0

1

0

Gioustos

10

0

0

0

0

0

Depharanas 2

0

0

0

0

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

7

0

3

0

1

0

Phalieros

14

0

3

0

0

0

Homilia

3

0

0

0

0

0

Apokopos

10

0

1

0

0

0

Apollo

¤

nios

6

0

0

0

0

0

Phlo

¤

rios

7

0

0

0

0

0

Rhodos

3

0

3

0

0

0

Katalogia

12

0

16

1

0

0

Total

237

1

84

1

3

0

background image

158

Appendix

Table A.7

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors object, prepositional

phrase, and non-temporal adverb

Factor

Æ

Object

Prep phrase Adverb

Text

Ø

Pre V Post V Pre V Post V Pre V Post V

Digene

¤

s

55

1

17

2

52

2

Pto

¤

khoprod. 21

1

6

2

4

6

Glykas

1

0

0

0

4

1

Spaneas

4

0

5

1

3

1

Kallimakhos 5

2

3

2

2

2

Lybistros

31

3

16

4

5

3

Achilleid

18

1

18

3

17

5

Belisarios

10

0

7

1

5

2

Thre

¤

nos

6

1

6

0

2

0

Poulologos

12

1

6

2

6

2

Paidiophr.

1

0

0

0

6

0

Physiologos 3

0

5

3

1

1

Spanos

4

0

5

3

0

3

Aito

¤

los

2

0

2

0

2

1

Moreas

35

0

19

0

55

1

Tokkoi

44

0

21

0

31

0

Rimada

17

1

22

0

23

0

Gioustos

2

0

8

0

6

0

Depharanas 5

1

8

1

8

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

3

0

8

4

3

1

Phalieros

10

1

12

2

10

0

Homilia

1

0

1

1

3

1

Apokopos

10

0

11

0

7

0

Apollo

¤

nios

18

1

9

1

9

1

Phlo

¤

rios

19

3

17

1

22

0

Rhodos

2

2

3

0

3

0

Katalogia

18

2

11

3

6

0

Total

357

21

246

36

295

33

background image

Appendix

159

Table A.8

Raw counts of pronoun placement for factors subject, and temporal

expression

Factor

Æ

Subject

Temporal expression

Text

Ø

Pre V Post V Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

28

5

14

9

Pto

¤

khoprod. 7

5

1

3

Glykas

2

5

1

1

Spaneas

2

5

3

2

Kallimakhos 4

3

1

6

Lybistros

17

10

4

10

Achilleid

34

6

3

4

Belisarios

3

4

5

2

Thre

¤

nos

19

3

3

2

Poulologos 17

6

1

3

Paidiophr.

1

1

1

0

Physiologos 2

4

2

1

Spanos

6

17

0

3

Aito

¤

los

13

2

1

1

Moreas

17

2

4

1

Tokkoi

27

4

7

1

Rimada

49

3

16

1

Gioustos

10

3

2

0

Depharanas 15

1

3

1

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

4

2

1

0

Phalieros

12

4

2

0

Homilia

3

3

1

0

Apokopos

6

0

3

0

Apollo

¤

nios

10

10

1

4

Phlo

¤

rios

12

6

5

3

Rhodos

5

3

0

2

Katalogia

10

13

1

3

Total

334

130

86

63

background image

160

Appendix

Table A.9

Raw counts of pronoun placement in the contexts

ν ο and ο μ

Factor

Æ

ν ο

ο μ!

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

2

0

3

0

Pto

¤

khoprod.

3

0

1

0

Glykas

0

0

0

0

Spaneas

0

0

5

0

Kallimakhos

0

0

0

0

Lybistros

0

0

1

0

Achilleid

5

1

9

0

Belisarios

2

0

0

0

Thre

¤

nos

0

0

0

0

Poulologos

3

1

5

0

Paidiophr.

0

0

0

0

Physiologos

0

0

0

0

Spanos

0

0

0

0

Aito

¤

los

0

0

0

0

Moreas

0

0

0

0

Tokkoi

0

0

0

0

Rimada

0

0

0

0

Gioustos

0

0

0

0

Depharanas

0

0

0

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

0

0

0

0

Phalieros

0

0

1

0

Homilia

0

0

0

0

Apokopos

1

0

1

0

Apollo

¤

nios

0

0

0

0

Phlo

¤

rios

0

0

0

0

Rhodos

0

0

0

0

Katalogia

3

0

1

0

Total

19

2

27

0

background image

Appendix

161

Table A.10

Raw counts concerning the interaction between the presence of

λος and pronoun placement

Red. Object with

λος

without

λος

Text

Ø

Pre V Post V Pre V Post V

Digene

¤

s

1

5

0

4

Pto

¤

khoprod. 1

1

0

1

Glykas

0

0

0

0

Spaneas

0

1

0

1

Kallimakhos 0

3

0

3

Lybistros

2

10

1

7

Achilleid

0

6

0

6

Belisarios

0

1

0

1

Thre

¤

nos

0

6

0

6

Poulologos

2

7

1

6

Paidiophr.

0

6

0

6

Physiologos 0

1

0

1

Spanos

1

0

1

0

Aito

¤

los

5

0

1

0

Moreas

0

1

0

1

Tokkoi

5

4

2

2

Rimada

8

4

3

4

Gioustos

4

4

0

3

Depharanas 2

0

1

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

2

0

0

0

Phalieros

1

1

1

1

Homilia

1

0

1

0

Apokopos

0

1

0

0

Apollo

¤

nios

0

4

0

2

Phlo

¤

rios

2

5

1

4

Rhodos

2

3

2

1

Katalogia

0

5

0

5

Total

39

79

15

65

background image

162

Appendix

Table A.11

Raw counts concerning the interaction between

θéλω periphrastic

constructions and pronoun placement

Envrn.

Æ

Preverb Environ.

Postverb Environ.

Neutral Environ.

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

5

0

0

1

0

0

Pto

¤

khoprod.

1

0

0

0

0

0

Glykas

1

0

0

2

1

0

Spaneas

0

0

0

0

0

0

Kallimakhos

1

0

0

2

0

0

Lybistros

4

0

0

4

1

2

Achilleid

3

0

0

3

0

2

Belisarios

0

0

0

1

0

0

Thre

¤

nos

0

0

0

0

0

1

Poulologos

1

0

0

3

0

0

Paidiophr.

0

0

0

0

0

0

Physiologos

0

0

0

0

0

0

Spanos

0

0

0

0

0

0

Aito

¤

los

0

1

0

0

0

0

Moreas

4

0

0

0

0

0

Tokkoi

3

0

2

2

1

0

Rimada

4

0

0

4

1

2

Gioustos

3

0

1

2

0

0

Depharanas

1

0

1

1

0

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

1

0

0

1

0

0

Phalieros

7

1

0

1

0

0

Homilia

0

0

0

0

0

0

Apokopos

0

0

0

2

0

0

Apollo

¤

nios

1

0

0

1

0

0

Phlo

¤

rios

1

0

0

4

0

0

Rhodos

1

1

0

1

0

0

Katalogia

2

0

0

2

0

0

TOTAL

44

3

4

37

4

7

background image

Appendix

163

Table A.12

Raw counts concerning the interaction between imperative verb-

forms and pronoun placement

Environ.

Æ Preverb Environ. Postverb Environ. Neutral Environ.

Text

Ø

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Pre V

Post V

Digene

¤

s

2

0

1

21

1

1

Pto

¤

khoprod.

2

4

0

46

3

4

Glykas

1

2

0

40

2

4

Spaneas

0

0

1

10

0

2

Kallimakhos

0

0

0

17

0

1

Lybistros

0

0

0

40

0

3

Achilleid

1

1

0

16

1

0

Belisarios

0

0

0

4

0

0

Thre

¤

nos

5

1

0

7

0

1

Poulologos

0

0

0

8

0

0

Paidiophr.

0

0

0

0

0

0

Physiologos

0

0

0

0

0

0

Spanos

1

0

0

24

0

3

Aito

¤

los

0

0

0

4

0

0

Moreas

0

0

0

4

0

0

Tokkoi

0

0

0

1

0

0

Rimada

0

3

0

11

0

0

Gioustos

0

3

0

14

0

0

Depharanas

2

1

0

4

0

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

0

0

0

3

0

0

Phalieros

0

0

0

26

1

3

Homilia

0

0

0

4

0

0

Apokopos

0

0

0

7

0

0

Apollo

¤

nios

0

0

0

6

0

0

Phlo

¤

rios

1

2

0

26

0

2

Rhodos

0

0

0

9

0

0

Katalogia

0

0

0

18

0

1

Total

15

17

2

263

8

25

background image

164

Appendix

Table A.13

The effects of metrical requirements on preverbal placement for

factors fronted constituent vs. subject and temporal expression

Factor

Æ

Fronted Constituent

Subject&Temp. Expr.

Metric. Constrained

Ø

Yes

No

Yes

No

Digene

¤

s

108

16

40

2

Pto

¤

choprod.

25

6

7

1

Glykas

4

1

2

1

Spaneas

8

4

2

2

Kallimakhos

10

0

5

0

Lybistros

36

16

18

3

Achilleid

42

11

35

2

Belisarios

14

8

6

0

Thre

¤

nos

10

4

22

0

Poulologos

15

9

17

1

Paidiophr.

7

0

2

0

Physiologos

9

0

4

0

Spanos

EXCLUDED

Aito

¤

los

5

1

5

1

Moreas

81

29

16

5

Tokkoi

72

24

34

0

Rimada

55

7

60

3

Gioustos

14

2

12

0

Depharanas

14

7

18

0

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s

11

3

5

0

Phalieros

20

12

9

4

Homilia

5

0

4

0

Apokopos

23

5

9

0

Apollo

¤

nios

28

8

10

1

Phlo

¤

rios

56

2

16

1

Rhodos

5

3

7

0

Katalogia

31

4

12

1

Total

708

182

377

28

background image

Appendix

165

Table A.14

Verb position by text in Later Medieval Greek

Verb Position

Æ

Initial

Second

Medial

Final

Text

Ø

Digene

¤

s 1182-1202

6

12

2

0

Pto

¤

khoprod. 12-39

13

6

0

1

Spaneas 12-29

11

4

3

2

Lybistros 1635-1670

13

4

3

0

Achilleid 68-91

9

11

0

0

Belisarios 239-258

9

9

2

0

Poulologos 12-45

9

7

3

1

Moreas 1410-1438

4

13

3

0

Tokkoi 1082-1100

9

10

1

0

Rimada 847-864

4

11

5

0

Apokopos 5-26

6

10

4

0

Phlo

¤

rios 484-508

9

9

0

2

Apollo

¤

n. 607-629

5

10

3

2

Rhodos 10-34

12

6

0

2

Katalogia 9-30

7

11

1

1

Total

126

133

30

11

background image

166

Notes

1.

When the noun is preceded by an adjective, the weak object pronoun may
appear after the adjective and before the noun, a pattern that Horrocks views
as a residual of the ‘clitic second’ tendency which has now been transferred
to the phrasal level.

2.

Mackridge’s exact formulation is ‘when the verb-phrase is preceded by an
object with the same referent as that of the clitic pronoun (i.e., when the
pronoun is resumptive or doubling).’ However, by ‘verb-phrase’ Mackridge
means the verb–pronoun complex, not the VP of generative syntax. It must
also be noted that, according to Haberland and van der Auwera (1990) dou-
bling and resumptive clitics (pronouns) are not the same. They reserve the
term ‘resumptive’ for those pronouns that appear in relativized clauses rep-
resenting the antecedent, and use the term ‘doubling’ for the situation that
Mackridge describes.

3.

The velar stop is retained only before vowels and becomes a velar fricative

if in Classical Greek the following vowel was aspirated. Later Medieval
Greek orthography still reflects this change (

οχ ε#ρíσκω τον, Achilleid

1616) even though the vowels are no longer aspirated.

4.

For the loss of the infinitive see Joseph (1983a), while for the future and
perfect periphrases see Horrocks (1997) and Joseph and Pappas (2002).

5.

Syntacticians traditionally mark ungrammatical sentences with a star (*),
whereas generally accepted sentences are left unmarked. When they want to
indicate that they are not sure what the grammaticality status of the sentence
is, they mark with a question mark (?) or several of them (???) reflecting
several degrees of ‘uneasiness’ with a sentence. Hence the term ‘question
mark’ judgement.

6.

This, of course, is based on the assumption that written accents, as they
appear in the manuscripts, can be taken at face value in interpreting exam-
ples. According to Holton (electronic communication–via Mackridge,
11/27/2000) ‘accents are clearly written, at least in the best manuscripts …
enclitics are often joined to the preceding noun or verb … [and] there are
very few ambiguities of the type [mentioned here].’ Since I am not exam-
ining the manuscripts themselves, however, I remain cautious about what
can be inferred from the presence or absence of the accents.

7.

These accents are not indications of stress, just orthographic indicators
about the status of the pronoun. Similarly, in Standard Modern Greek
monosyllabic verbs are not written with an accent, even though they are ac-
cented phonologically e.g.,

θα τον δω ‘I will see him’ not θα τον δ (pho-

netically [TA ton "Do]).

8.

Jannaris (1968: §1827-1828) argues that

μ in this case is a ‘corruption’ of

μν, a conjunction expressing ‘confindent expectation’ and that ‘copiers
[after 300 AD] … mistook it for another negative intended to strengthen

ο,

and so changed it to the classical and familiar

μ.’

background image

Notes

167

9.

In Pappas (2000, 2001a), I suggested that

ο enclitizes to ν in these cases.

Both hypotheses yield the same result—a new morphological unit that can
carry stress—but this one is more consistent with what is already known
about the status of

ο and ν in Ancient Greek.

10. According to Mackridge (1985)

λος exhibits some idiosyncrasies in Stan-

dard Modern Greek as well, since it is the only adjective that has to be dou-
bled by a pronoun.

11. Joseph (1978/1990, 1983a), and Nevis and Joseph (1993) mention that the

past passive participle may have a weak pronoun argument in some rare
cases. The example they cite is (1), but there are no such examples in this
database of LMG texts.

(1)

δεχóμενóς

το

DExomEnos

to

accept–Past Pass Prcle

it–DOsg WP

‘Accepting it’

12. According to Joseph (1983a: 64, 2000), Horrocks (1997: 304) the present

perfect periphrasis with the present tense of

/χω ‘I have’ was modeled on

the pluperfect form at a much later time.

13. Since the imperative in LMG appears only in the second singular and plural

forms with rare, highly stylized, and presumably consciously archaizing
uses of the third person singular or plural, it would seem appropriate to ex-
clude all non-second person forms from the finite verb-form database. It is,
however, a reasonable assumption that the person of the form does not af-
fect the placement of the weak object pronoun, and so it is not necessary to
do so.

14. Although I do not necessarily agree with the use of the term ‘clitic’ in these

accounts, I will not engage, for the moment, in a discussion about the status
of the weak pronouns as ‘clitics’ or ‘affixes’. This issue will be taken up in
a later chapter. Here I will focus on the problematic aspects of these analy-
ses, even if their assumption that the pronouns are clitics in LMG is granted.

15. Even though Horrocks refers to the weak object pronouns as ‘clitics’ his use

here of the term ‘word-like complex’ to refer to the string composed of the
various verbal markers, the object pronouns and the verb suggests that he
considers these elements to be affixes, as Joseph (1988, 1989, 1990, 2002)
has argued.

16. Wanner (1981a) has also written about the situation in Old Italian, where

the pattern of variation in pronoun placement is quite similar to LMG. One
finds postverbal placement when the verb (finite—including imperative) is
clause-initial, or following certain paratactic conjunctions (e.g., e ‘and’ or
ma ‘but’, though not o ‘or’, peró ‘however’), and when the verb is of non-
finite form (infinitive, gerund, absolute participial constructions). He ac-
counts for these facts by proposing that in Old Italian there were lineariza-
tion principles that were constrained syntactically: ‘clitics could not stand
after a surface clause boundary.’ Furthermore, he notes that the system of
clitic placement was ‘mixed’, since the non-finite forms had to be morpho-
syntactic exceptions while the conjuctions e and ma also had to be lexically
marked as associated with postverbal placement. In a (1996) article, Wan-

background image

168

Notes

ner suggests that ‘clitic position’ in the Medieval Romance languages can
be better understood as a transition phenomenon, as the languages moved
from a second position schema of pronoun placement (à la Wackernagel’s
Law) to a schema in which the pronoun is attached to the verb.

17. According to Wanner (1991, 1996), though, Old Spanish interpolation was

a much more complex phenomenon than indicated in Fontana’s (1993) ac-
count.

18. This constraint is poorly worded, for if taken literally it means that any

phenomenon that is restricted to root clauses is a NI structure. This would
make V2 in German, and tag-questions in English NI structures, clearly an
unwanted conclusion. Rather, the constraint seems to mean ‘NI structures
are only a root phenomenon.’ I thank Martin Jansche and Shravan Vasishth
for helping me sort out the logical implications of the constraint.

19. This is not an uncontroversial assumption. Halpern mentions that ‘they may

at times be proclitics (Ewen 1979: 5, 6) but it does seem that they are al-
ways subject to a constraint which prevents them from being clause-initial.’
In Old French, however, there are some instances of clause-initial clitics
(Adams 1989).

20. This, in fact, is only true of the (1980/1995) dissertation. Already in the

introduction to the revised version of 1982, Klavans limits the set of pa-
rameters to three, keeping P3, P4 and P5. The set of five parameters is used
here for ease of exposition.

21. Proclisis vs. Enclisis, according to Anderson (1992: 203) ‘refers to the

properties (and consequences of) a particular language’s rule(s) of Stray
Adjunction.’

22. Jannsen (1998), however, remarks that in three non-literary documents from

the 17

th

century, which she examined, the pattern of pronoun placement

does not differ significantly from that found in Thysia tou Abraam. None-
theless, she does admit that there are robust examples of preverbal pronouns
in clause-initial position and in reduplicated object constructions. Since
Janssen does not provide the specific numbers, it is difficult to compare her
results to the ones presented here.

23. Such flexibility can be found in SMG verse as well. In a popular song of the

1990’s,

Δèν πιστεúω

‘I don’t believe’, Eleni Dimou sings:

(2)

λα

στà μéτρα σου

κομμéνα καì ραμμéνα ε'χες

ola

sta mEtra su

komEna kE ramEna ixEs

all

to your measurement

cut and tailored you had

‘You had everything according to your standards’

This line violates the constraint that the adjective

λος when functioning as

an object must be reduplicated by a weak pronoun, in this case

τá (see

Mackridge 1985 and note 10).

24. Indirect confirmation of this hypothesis about the conservative character of

the Cypriot dialect, at least with respect to pronoun placement, comes from
medieval Pontic, another variety of Greek that became isolated from Byz-
antine Greek in the beginning of the LMG period. Condoravdi and Kiparsky
(2001) report the following construction which was found in a deed dated
from 1260 AD.

background image

Notes

169

(3)

ton de tópon

edókamén

soi

the Part place

give–1pl Past

you–IOsg WP

‘We gave you this land’

Here, as in the Cypriot data, the pronoun is placed postverbally even though
the immediately preceding element is a fronted constituent, which in LMG
is associated with preverbal pronoun placement.

25. This problem is also recognized by Rivero and Terzi’s (1995) article on

Modern Cypriot Greek. Their attempt to solve it is indicative of how vexing
this problem is for accounts in the GB/Minimalist approach: ‘Intuitively, the
constituent before the clitic is of the type that counts as first position. More
technically, the strong feature is licensed because the clitic is contained in
the internal domain of an A-bar head made visible in PF via the
phonological content in either X

o

or Spec.’ Condoravdi and Kiparsky

(2001: 8) rightly comment that this is ‘a strange syntactic licensing re-
quirement’.

26. It must be noted, however, that in the German and Dutch examples the ver-

bal morphology appears on the preceding elements. This is not the case in
Cypriot where the weak pronouns are attached to the following verb.

background image

170

References

1. Primary Sources.
Acclamations = in Maas 1912.
Achilleid =

Διγησις 4Αχιλλéως. In W. Wagner (ed.), Trois Poémes Grecs du

moyen-age inédit, Berlin 1881.

Agapios = in Baletas (1949: 249-250).
Aito

¤

los =

Γεωργíου Α8τωλο. In N. Ba~nescu (ed.), Un poéme Grec Vulgaire re-

latif á Piérre Le Boiteux de vallachie, Bucarest 1906.

Apollo

¤

nios =

Μεταγλẃττισμα %πò Λατινικς ε8ς Ρωμαïκν, διγησις πολυ-

παθος 4Απολλωνíου το Τυρíου. In W. Wagner (ed.), Medieval Greek
Texts
, Amsterdam 1970.

Apokopos =

4Απóκοπος. In Στυλιανòς 4Αλεξíου (ed.),

Μπεργαδς

:

4Απóκο-

πος

,

;Η βοσκοποúλα

, Athens 1971.

Assizes = in Baletas (1949: 1-4).
Belisarios =

;Ιστορíα το Βελισαρíου (version χ). In W. F. Bakker and A. F.

Gemert (eds.),

;Ιστορíα το Βελισαρíου

, Athens 1988.

Boskopoula =

;Η βοσκοποúλα. In Στυλιανòς 4Αλεξíου (ed.),

Μπεργαδς

:

4Α-

πóκοπος

,

;Η βοσκοποúλα

, Athens 1971.

Boustro

¤

nios =

Διγησις κρóνικας Κúπρου

Nicosia 1989.

Depharanas I =

Μáρκου Δεφαρáνα, Jστορíα κ το Δανιλ περì τς Σω-

σáννης. In Γ. Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1],

Athens.

Depharanas II =

Μáρκου Δεφαρáνα, λóγοι διδακτικοì πατρòς πρòς τòν υJ-

óν. In Γ. Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

Digene

¤

s =

Βασíλειος Διγεν7ς 4Ακρíτης !κατà τò χειρóγραφο το 4Εσκο-

ριáλ". In Στυλιανòς 4Αλεξíου (ed.),

Βασíλειος Διγεν7ς 4Ακρíτης !κατà

τò χειρóγραφο το 4Εσκοριáλ" καì τò σμα το 4Αρμοúρη

, Athens 1985.

Ero

¤

phile

¤

=

4Ερωφíλη. In

Γεωργíου Χορτáτζη

,

4Ερωφíλη !Κρητικòν

θéατρον

,

Μεσαιωνικá"

, Athens 1970.

Gioustos =

Πéνθος θανáτου, ζως μáταιον καì πρòς Θεòν πιστροφ. In Γ.

Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

Glykas =

Στíχοι γραμματικο Μιχα7λ το Γλυκ ο.ς /γραψε καθF ^ν κα-

τεσχéθη καιρòν κ προσαγγελíας χαιρεκáκου τινóς. In Γ. Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

Homilia =

;Ομιλíα το νεκρο βασιλι. Μ. Ι. Μανοúσακας,

4Επιστημονικ7

4Επετηρìς Öιλολογικς Σχολς Πανεπιστημíου Θεσσαλονíκης

, 8: 295-

314.

background image

References

171

Kallimakhos =

Καλλíμαχος καì Χρυσορρóη. In Michel Pichard (ed.), Le ro-

man de Callimaque et de Chrysorrhoé, Paris 1956.

Katalogia =

Καταλóγια, Στíχοι περì /ρωτος καì %γáπης. In D. C. Hesseling

and Hubert Pernot (eds.), Erotopaignia (Chansons d'amour), Paris 1913.

Loukare

¤

s = in Baletas (1949: 140-149).

Lybistros =

Τò κατà Λúβιστρον καì Ροδáμνην. In W. Wagner (ed.), Trois

Poé-mes Grecs du moyen-age inédit, Berlin 1881.

Makhairas =

4Εξγησις τς γλυκεíας χẃρας Κúπρου. In R. M. Dawkins (ed.),

Leontios Makhairas: Recital concerning the Sweet Land of Cyprus entitled
‘Chronicle’
, Oxford 1932.

Moreas =

Χρονικòν το Μορéως (codex Havniensis). In John Schmitt (ed.),

Chronicle of Morea, Groeningen 1967.

Paidiophrastos =

Διγησις παιδιóφραστος τν τετραπóδων ζẃων. In Γ.

Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

Phalieros I =

;Ιστορíα καì UΟνειρο. In A. F. Gemert (ed.),

Μαρíνου Öαλιé-

ρου 4Ερωτικà Dνειρα

, Thessaloniki 1980.

Phalieros II =

Λóγοι διδακτικοì το πατρòς πρòς τòν υJóν. In W. F. Bakker

and A. F. Gemert (eds.), The

Λóγοι Διδακτικοì

of Marinos Phalieros, Lei-

den 1977.

Phlo

¤

rios =

Öλẃριος καì Πλατζιαφλẃρα. In W. Wagner (ed.), Medieval Greek

Texts, Amsterdam 1970.

Physiologos =

4Εκ το Öυσιολóγου. In F. Sbordone (ed.), Physiologus, Milan

1936.

Poulologos =

;Ο Πουλολóγος. In 4Ισαβéλλα Τσαβáρη (ed.),

;Ο Πουλολóγος

,

Athens 1987.

Pto

¤

khoprodromos =

Πτωχοπροδρομικá. In Hans Eideneier (ed.), Ptochopro-

dromus, Cologne 1991.

Rhodos =

4Εμμανου7λ Γεωργιλ, τò θανατικòν τς Ρóδου. In W. Wagner

(ed.), Medieval Greek Texts, Amsterdam 1970.

Rimada=

Διγησις το 4Αλεξáνδρου. In David Holton (ed), The tale of Alex-

ander =

Διγησις το 4Αλεξáνδρου

the rhymed version, critical edition

with an introduction and commentary, Thessalonike 1974.

Santorinaios = in Baletas (1949: 130-133).
Soumake

¤

s = in Baletas (1949: 119-122).

Spaneas =

Σπανéας, 4Αλεξíου Κομνηνο, ποíημα παραινετικóν. In Γ .

Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

Spanos =

Σπανóς. In Hans Eideneier (ed.), Spanos: eine byzantinische Satire in

der Form einer Parodie, Berlin 1977.

Thre

¤

nos =

Θρνος τς Κωνσταντινουπóλεως. In W. Wagner (ed.), Medieval

Greek Texts, Amsterdam 1970.

Thysia =

;Η θυσíα το 4Αβραáμ. In UΑγγελος Τερζáκης (ed.),

Βιτσéνζος Κο-

ρνáρος

,

;Η θυσíα το 4Αβραáμ

, Athens 1979.

background image

172

References

Tokkoi =

Τò Χρονικò τν Τóκκων. In Giuseppe Schiro (ed.), Cronaca dei

Tocco di Cefalonia, Rome 1975.

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s I =

4Ιακẃβου Τριβẃλη, Jστορíα το Ρè τς Σκωτíας. In Γ. Ζẃρας

(ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

Tribo

¤

le

¤

s II =

4Ιακẃβου Τριβẃλη, Jστορíα το Ταγιαπéρα. In Γ. Ζẃρας (ed.),

Βυζαντιν7 Ποíησις

, [Vasiki Vivliothiki, 1], Athens.

2. Secondary Sources.
Abeille, Anne and Daniéle Godard. 1996. The Complementation of French Aux-

iliaries. Unpublished ms. Université de Paris 7.

Adams, Marianne. 1989. Verb Second Effects in Medieval French. In Carl

Kirschner and Janet DeCesaris (eds.), Studies in Romance Linguistics. Am-
sterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 1-31.

Anderson, Stephen R. 1992. A-Morphous morphology. Cambridge; New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Anderson, Stephen R. 1993. Wackernagel’s Revenge: Clitics, Morphology, and

the Syntax of Second Position. Language 69: 68-98.

Androulakis, Anna. 1998. Wh- and Direct Object Clitics Revisited. In Brian D.

Joseph, Geoffrey C. Horrocks and Irene Philippaki-Warburton (eds.), The-
mes in Greek linguistics II
. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 131-
168.

Arvaniti, Amalia and Brian D. Joseph. 1999. Variation in Voiced Stop Nasaliza-

tion in Greek. OSUWPL 53: 203-234.

Arvaniti, Amalia and Brian D. Joseph. 2000. Variation in voiced stop prenasali-

zation in Greek. Glossologia 11-12: 131-166.

Bakker, Willem F. and Arnold F. van Gemert. 1988. Historia tou Belisariou.

Athens: Morpho¤tiko Hidryma Ethnike¤s Trapeze¤s.

Baletas, Geo¤rgios. 1949. Anthologia te

¤

s De

¤

motike

¤

s Pezographias. Athens: Petros

Ranos.

Bànescu, Nikolai. 1915. Die Entwicklung des griechischen Futurums von der

frühbyzantinischen Zeit bis zur Gegenwart. Königliche Hofbuchdruckerei F.
Göbl Söhne, Bucharest.

Beck, Hans-Georg. 1993. Historia te

¤

s Byzantine

¤

s de

¤

mo

¤

dous logotekhnias. Nike¤

Eideneier (transl). Athens: Morpho¤tiko Hidryma Ethnike¤s Trapeze¤s.

Blake, Renee. 1997. Defining the Envelope of Linguistic Variation: The Case of

‘Don’t Count’ Forms in the Copula Analysis of African American Vernacu-
lar English. Language Variation and Change 9: 57-79.

Browning, Robert. 1983. Medieval and Modern Greek. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Bybee, Joan, L. and Dan I. Slobin. 1982. The development and use of the Eng-

lish past tense. Language 58: 265-289.

background image

References

173

Campbell, Lyle. 1991. Some grammaticalization changes in Estonian and their

implications. In E. C. Traugott and B. Heine (eds.), Approaches to Gram-
maticalization
, vol. 1. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 285-299.

Campbell, Lyle. 2001. What’s wrong with grammaticalization? Language Sci-

ences 23: 113-161 [Special issue on Grammaticalization: A Critical Assess-
ment
(Lyle Campbell, ed.)].

Campbell, Lyle and Richard Janda. 2001. Introduction: conceptions of gram-

maticalization and their problems. Language Sciences 23: 93-112 [Special
issue on Grammaticalization: A Critical Assessment (Lyle Campbell, ed.)].

Cardinaletti, Anna and Ian G. Roberts. To appear. Clause Structure and X-sec-

ond. In W. Chao and G. Horrocks (eds.) Levels, Principles and Processes:
The Structure of Grammatical Representations
. Dordrecht: Foris.

Chesire, Jenny. 1987. Syntactic variation, the linguistic variable and sociolin-

guistic theory. Linguistics 25: 257-282.

Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT

Press.

Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Collinge, N. E. 1985. The laws of Indo-European. Amsterdam; Philadelphia:

Benjamins.

Condoravdi, Cleo and Paul Kiparsky. 2001. Clitics and clause structure. Journal

of Greek Linguistics 2: 1-39.

Dittmar, Norbert. 1996. Descriptive and Explanatory Power of Rules in So-

ciolinguistics. In Rajendra Singh (ed.), Towards a critical sociolinguistics.
Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 115-150.

Drachman, Gabriel. 1994. Verb Movement and Minimal Clauses. In Irene

Philippaki-Warburton, Katerina Nicolaidis, Maria Sifianou (eds.), Themes in
Greek linguistics: papers from the first International Conference on Greek
Linguistics, Reading, September 1993
, Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins,
pp. 45-52.

Dressler, Wolfgang. 1966. Von altgriechischen zum neugriechischen System der

Personalpronomina. Indogermanische Forschungen 71: 39-63.

Ewen, Robert. 1979. A Grammar of Bulgarian Clitics. Doctoral dissertation,

University of Washington.

Fasold, Ralph W. 1972. Tense Marking in Black English: A Linguistic and So-

cial Analysis. Arlington: Center for Applied Linguistics.

Fasold, Ralph W. 1996. The Quiet Demise of Variable Rules. In Rajendra Singh

(ed.), Towards a critical sociolinguistics. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benja-
mins, pp. 79-98.

Fontana, Josep M. 1993. Phrase structure and the syntax of clitics in the History

of Spanish. Unpublished Dissertation, IRSC Report: 93-24.

Fontana, Josep M. 1996. Phonology and Syntax in the Interpretation of the

Tobler-Mussafia Law. In Aaron L. Halpern and Arnold M. Zwicky (eds.),

background image

174

References

Approaching second: second position and related phenomena. Stanford,
California: CLSI Publications, pp. 41-84.

Fontana, Josep M. 1997. On the integration of second position phenomena. In

Ans van Kemenade and Nigel Vincent (eds.), Parameters of morphosyntac-
tic change
. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 207-
250.

Gemert, Arnold F. van. 1980. Marinou Phalierou Ero

¤

tika oneira. Thessalonike¤:

Typogr. St. and I. Sphakianake¤.

Germano, Girolamo. 1622. See Pernot 1907.
Gropen, Jess, Steven Pinker, Michelle Hollander, Richard Goldberg and Ronald

Wilson. 1989. The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in
English. Language 65: 204-257.

Gross, Maurice. 1979. On the Failure of Generative Grammar. Language 55:

859-885.

Haberland, Hartmut and Johan van der Auwera. 1990. Topics and Clitics in

Greek Relatives. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 22: 127-158.

Halpern, Aaron. 1995. On the Placement and Morphology of Clitics. Stanford:

CSLI Publications.

Halpern, Aaron. 1998. Clitics. In A. Spencer and A. Zwicky (eds.), The hand-

book of morphology. Oxford; Malden, Mass.: Blackwell, pp. 101-124.

Halpern, Aaron and Josep Fontana. 1994. X

0

and X

max

clitics. WCFFL 12: 251-

266.

Harris, John. 1984. Syntactic variation and dialect divergence. Journal of Lin-

guistics 20(2): 303-327.

Herring, S. 1991. The grammaticaliztion of rhetorical questions in Tamil. In E.

C.Traugott, and B. Heine (eds.), Approaches to Grammaticalization, vol. 1.
Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 253-284.

Hesseling, D. 1892. Essai Historique sur l’infinitif Grec. In J. Psichari (ed.),

Études de Philologie NéoGreque. Paris, pp. 1-43.

Hock, Hans H. 1991. Principles of historical linguistics. Berlin; New York:

Mouton de Gruyter.

Hopper, Paul J. and Elizabeth C. Traugott. 1993. Grammaticalization. Cam-

bridge: Cambridge University Press.

Horrocks, Geoffrey C. 1983. The Order of Constituents in Modern Greek. In

G.Gazdar, E. Klein and G. K. Pullum (eds.), Order, Concord and Constitu-
ency
. Dordrecht: Foris, pp. 95-111.

Horrocks, Geoffrey C. 1990. Clitics in Greek. A diachronic review. In M. Rous-

sou and S. Panteli (eds.), Greek Outside Greece II. Athens: Diaspora Books,
pp. 35-52.

Horrocks, Geoffrey C. 1995. On Condition…: Aspect and Modality in the His-

tory of Greek. Proceedings of the Cambridge Philological Society 41: 153-
173.

background image

References

175

Horrocks, Geoffrey C. 1997. Greek: A history of the language and its speakers.

London; New York: Longman Linguistics Library.

Householder, Fred W., Kostas Kazazis and Andreas Koutsoudas. 1964. Refer-

ence grammar of literary Dhimotiki. Bloomington: Indiana University.

Janda, Richard D. 1995. From agreement affix to subject ‘clitic’— and bound

root: -mos>-nos vs. (-)nos(-) and nos-otros in New Mexican and other re-
gional Spanish dialects. In Papers from the Parasession on Clitics. Chicago
Linguistic Society, Chicago, pp. 118-139.

Janda, Richard D. 2001. Beyond ‘pathways’ and ‘unidirectionality’: on the dis-

continuity of language transmission and the counterability of grammaticali-
zation. Language Sciences 23: 265-340 [Special issue on Grammaticaliza-
tion: A Critical Assessment
(Lyle Campbell, ed.)].

Janda, Richard and Brian D. Joseph. 1986. One rule or many? Sanskrit redupli-

cation as fragmented affixation. OSUWPL 34: 84-107.

Janda, Richard and Brian D. Joseph. 1989. Further defence of a non-

phonological account for Sanskrit root-initial aspiration alternations. In
ESCOL ’88, Proceedings of the Fifth Eastern States Conference on Linguis-
tics
. Columbus: Ohio State University, pp. 246-200.

Janda, Richard and Brian D. Joseph. 1992. Meta-Templates and the underlying

(dis)-unity of Sanskrit reduplication. In ESCOL ’91, Proceedings of the Fifth
Eastern States Conference on Linguistics
. Columbus: Ohio State University,
pp.160-173.

Janda, Richard and Brian D. Joseph. In preparation. On constellations in mor-

phology: Crosslinguistic evidence and a case study from Modern Greek. Un-
published Ohio State University ms.

Jannaris, Antonius N. 1968. Historical Greek Grammar. Georg Olms Ver-

lagsbuchhandlung: Hildesheim.

Janse, Mark. 1994. Son of Wackernagel: The Distribution of Object Clitic Pro-

nouns in Cappadocian. In Irene Philippaki-Warburton, Katerina Nicolaidis,
Maria Sifianou (eds.), Themes in Greek linguistics: papers from the first In-
ternational Conference on Greek Linguistics, Reading, September 1993
.
Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 435-442.

Janse, Mark. 1998. Cappadocian Clitics and the Syntax-Morphology Interface.

In G. Horrocks, B. D. Joseph and I. Philippaki Warburton (eds.), Themes in
Greek Linguistics II
. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 257-282.

Janssen, Marjoline. 1998.

;Η πρóταξη καì πíταξη το %δúνατου τúπου τς

προσωπικς %ντωνυμíας τ7ν ποχ7 το 4Ερωτóκριτου καì τς Θυσíας
το 4Αβραáμ. Cretan Studies 6: 129-143.

Jeffreys, Elizabeth and Michael Jeffreys. 1986. The Oral Background of Byzan-

tine Popular Poetry. Oral Tradition 1: 504-547.

Joseph, Brian D. 1978/1990. Morphology and universals in syntactic change:

evidence from medieval and modern Greek. New York: Garland Pub. (Up-

background image

176

References

dated version of 1978 Harvard Univeristy PhD. Dissertation, printed and
distributed by Indiana University Linguistics Club).

Joseph, Brian D. 1981. On the synchrony and diachrony of Modern Greek NA.

Byzantine and Modern Greek Studies 7: 139-154.

Joseph, Brian D. 1983a. The synchrony and diachrony of the Balkan infinitive : a

study in areal, general, and historical linguistics. Cambridge; New York:
Cambridge University Press.

Joseph, Brian D. 1983b. Relativization in Modern Greek: Another look at the

accessibility hierarchy constraints. Lingua 60: 1-24.

Joseph, Brian D. 1985. Complementizers, Particles, and Finiteness in Greek and

the Balkans. Folia Slavica 7.3: 390-411.

Joseph, Brian D. 1988. Pronominal Affixes in Modern Greek: The Case Against

Clisis. In D. Brentari et al. (eds.), Papers from the 24th Regional Meeting.
Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 203-215.

Joseph, Brian D. 1989. I erminía merikón voríon típon tis prostaktikís katá ti

simeriní morfolojikí qeoría [The Interpretation of Several Northern Forms of
the Imperative According to Current Morphological Theory]. Eliniki
Dialektolojía
1: 21-26.

Joseph, Brian D. 1990. The benefits of morphological classification: on some

apparently problematic clitics in Modern Greek. In W. U. Dressler
(ed.), Comparative Morphology. Berlin; New York: Mouton de Gruyter, pp.
171-182.

Joseph, Brian D. 1992. I morfosíndaksi tu neoelinikú rimatikú sinólu san mor-

folojía ke óxi síndaksi [The Morphosyntax of the Modern Greek Verbal Unit
as Morphology and not Syntax]. Studies in Greek Linguistics 12: 33-44.

Joseph, Brian D. 1997. How General are our Generalizations? What Speakers

Actually Know and What They Actually Do. In Anthony D. Green and V.
Motopanyane et al., (eds.), ESCOL ’96, Proceedings of the Thirteenth East-
ern States Conference on Linguistics
. Ithaca: Cascadilla Press, pp. 148-160.

Joseph, Brian D. 2000. Textual Authenticity: Evidence From Medieval Greek. In

S. Herring, P. van Reenen, and L. Schoesler, (eds.), Textual Parameters in
Ancient Languages
. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 309-329.

Joseph, Brian D. 2001a. Is there Such a Thing as “Grammaticalization”? Lan-

guage Sciences 23: 163-186 [Special issue on Grammaticalization: A Criti-
cal Assessment
(Lyle Campbell, ed.)].

Joseph, Brian D. 2001b. Dialect Evidence bearing on the Definition of ‘Word’ in

Greek. In A. Ralli, B. Joseph and M. Janse (eds.), Proceedings of the First
International Conference of Modern Greek Dialects and Linguistic Theory
(Patras, Greece, Oct. 12-14, 2000). Patras: University of Patras, pp. 89-104.

Joseph, Brian D. 2002. The Word in Modern Greek. In R. M. W. Dixon and A.

Aikhenvald (eds.) Word: A Cross-Linguistic Typology. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press, pp. 243-265.

background image

References

177

Joseph, Brian D. In preparation. The Modern Greek Weak Subject Pronoun tos -

Its Origins and Implications for Language Change and Language Structure:
A Study in Grammatical Change
. Unpublished Ohio State University ms.

Joseph, Brian D. and Irene Philippaki-Warburton. 1987. Modern Greek. London;

Wolfeboro, NH: Croom Helm.

Joseph, Brian D. and Panayiotis A. Pappas. 2002. On some recent views con-

cerning the development of the Greek future system. Byzantine and Modern
Greek Studies
26: 247-273.

Joseph, Brian D. and Richard D. Janda. 1988. The How and Why of Diachronic

Morphologization and Demorphologization. In M. Hammond and M.
Noonan (eds.), Theoretical Morphology: Approaches in Modern Linguistics.
New York: Academic Press, pp. 193-210.

Joseph, Brian D. and Richard D. Janda. 2003. On Language, Change, and Lan-

guage Change – Or, Of History, Linguistics, and Historical Linguistics. To
appear in B. Joseph and R. Janda (eds.), Handbook of Historical Linguistics.
Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, pp. 3-18.

Kaisse, Ellen M. 1982. On the Preservation of Stress in Modern Greek. Linguis-

tics 20.1/2: 59-82.

Kathol, Andreas. 2000. Linear syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kayne, Richard S. 1975. French Syntax: The Transformational Cycle. Cam-

bridge: MIT Press.

Kazazis, Kostas and Joseph Pentheroudakis. 1976. Reduplication of Indefinite

Direct Objects in Albanian and Modern Greek. Language 52: 398-403.

Kim, Hyeree. 1996. The Synchrony and Diachrony of English Impersonal Verbs:

A Study in Syntactic and Lexical Change. PhD. Dissertation, The Ohio State
University.

Klavans, Judith L. 1980/1995. On clitics and cliticization: the interaction of

morphology, phonology and syntax. New York: Garland Pub. (Updated ver-
sion of 1980 University College of London PhD. Dissertation).

Klavans, Judith L. 1982. Some problems in a theory of clitics. Bloomington,

Ind.: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

Klavans, Judith L. 1985. The independence of syntax and phonology in cliticiza-

tion. Language 61: 95-120.

Kontosopoulos, Nikolaos G. 1981. Dialektoi kai idio

¤

mata te

¤

s neas Hellenike

¤

s.

Athens: [s.n.].

Kroch, Anthony S. 1982/1989. Function and Grammar in the History of English:

Periphrastic Do. In Ralph Fasold, and Deborah Schiffrin (eds.) Language
Change and Variation
. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 133-172.

Kroch, Anthony S. 1989. Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change.

Language Variation and Change 1: 99-244.

Kroch, Anthony S. 1997. Comments on ‘syntax shindig’ papers. Transactions of

the Philological Society 95:1.

background image

178

References

Labov, William. 1963. The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19: 273-

309.

Labov, William. 1982. Building on Empirical Foundations. In Winfred P. Leh-

mann and Yakov Malkiel. Perspectives on Historical Linguistics. Amster-
dam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp.17-92.

Labov, William. 1994. Principles of Linguistic Change: Internal Factors. Ox-

ford; Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell.

Lightfoot, David W. 1979. Principles of Diachronic Syntax. Cambridge: Cam-

bridge University Press.

Maas, Peter. 1912. Metrische Akklamationen der Byzantiner. Byzantinische

Zeitschrift 21: 28-51.

Mackridge, Peter. 1985. The Modern Greek Language: A Descriptive Analysis of

Standard Modern Greek. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.

Mackridge, Peter. 1993. An editorial problem in the medieval greek texts: the

position of the object clitic pronoun in the Escorial Digenes Akrites. In N.
M. Panayiotakis (ed.), Arkhes te

¤

s neoelle

¤

nike

¤

s logoteknias. Venice, pp. 325-

342.

Mackridge, Peter. 1995.

;Η θéση το %δúνατου τúπου τς προσωπικς %-

ντωνυμíας στ7 Μεσαιωνικ7 Δημẃδη ;Ελληνικ. Studies in Greek Lin-
guistics
15: 906-929.

Mackridge, Peter. 2000. The position of the weak object pronoun in Medieval

and Modern Greek. Yazyk i Rechevaya Deyatel’nost’ 3: 133-151.

Meillet, Antoine. 1912. L’évolution des formes grammaticales. Scienta (Rivista

di Scienza) 12, No. 26, 6.

Méndez Dosuna, J. 2000. Review of Horrocks 1997. Journal of Greek Linguis-

tics 1: 274-295.

Meyer, W. 1889. Simon Portius. Grammatica Linguae Graecae Vulgaris. Re-

production de l’Édition de 1638. Paris: F. Vieweg, Librairie-Éditeur.

Mihevc!-Gabrovec!, Erika. 1973. L’Infinitif temporel en Grec du moyen-age et en

Slovene. Linguistica: 219-229.

Miller, Philip H. and Ivan A. Sag. 1997. French Clitic Movement without Clitics

or Movement. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 15: 573-639.

Milroy, James. 1992. Linguistic Variation and Change: On the Historical So-

ciolinguistics of English. Oxford; Cambridge, Mass.: Blackwell.

Milroy, Lesley. 1987. Language and Social Network. Oxford; Cambridge,

Mass.: Blackwell.

Mis#eska-Tomic!, Olga. 1996. The Balkan Slavic Clausal Clitics. Natural Lan-

guage and Linguistic Theory 14: 811-872.

Mussafia, Alfredo. 1898. Enclisi o proclisi del pronome personale atono quale

oggetto. Romania 27: 145-6.

Neikirk, Jill M. 1996. From Adaptation to Nativization: A Synchronic Analysis

of the Category of Aspect in Borrowed Verbs in Russian, Bulgarian and Ma-
cedonian
. PhD. Dissertation, The Ohio State University.

background image

References

179

Nevis, Joel A. and Brian D. Joseph. 1993. Wackernagel Affixes: Evidence from

Balto-Slavic. Yearbook of Morphology 5: 93-111.

Newton, Brian. 1972. The Generative Interpretation of Dialect. A Study of Mod-

ern Greek Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Norde, Muriel. 2001. Deflexion as a counterdirectional factor in grammatical

change. Language Sciences 23: 231-264 [Special issue on Grammaticaliza-
tion: A Critical Assessment
(Lyle Campbell, ed.)].

Oikonomide¤s, De¤me¤trrios. 1958. Grammatike

¤

te

¤

s Helle

¤

nike

¤

s Dialektou tou

Pontou. Athens: Akade¤mia Athe¤no¤n.

Pangalos, G. 1955. Peri tou glo

¤

ssikou idio

¤

matos te

¤

s Kre

¤

te

¤

s. Athens.

Papadopoulos, Th. 1977. Nikolaou Sophianou, Grammatike

¤

t e

¤

s koine

¤

s to

¤

n

Helle

¤

no

¤

n Glo

¤

sse

¤

s. Athens: Kedros.

Pappas, Panayiotis A. 1997. Dispelling chaos: pronoun-verb placement in Later

Medieval Greek. Paper presented at the 18th annual conference for Greek
Linguistics; Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece, April 1997.

Pappas, Panayiotis A. 1999. From thelo

¤

graphein/e

¤

thela graphein to tha gra-

pho

¤

/tha egrapha: Comparing the development of futures and counter-factu-

als in Early Modern Greek. OSUWPL 52: 15-40.

Pappas, Panayiotis A. 2000. Unus testis, nullus testis? The significance of a sin-

gle token in a problem of Later Medieval Greek Syntax. OSUWPL 54: 171-
176.

Pappas, Panayiotis A. 2001a.

4Αριθμοí καì Διαíσθηση. Μéθοδοι γιà τ7 μελé-

τη το συντακτικο τν Μεσαιωνικν ;Ελληνικν. Studies in Greek
linguistic
s 21: 597-605.

Pappas, Panayiotis A. 2001b. The Microcosm of a Morphological Change:

Variation in the thelo

¤

+ infinitive futures and e

¤

thela + infinitive counterfac-

tuals in Early Modern Greek. Diachronica 18.1: 59-92.

Pappas, Panayiotis A. and Brian D. Joseph. 2001. The development of the Greek

future system: setting the record straight. In Proceedings of the 4th Interna-
tional Conference on Greek Linguistics
, University of Cyprus, University
Studio Press, pp. 354-359.

Pernot, H. 1907. Girolamo Germano. Grammaire et Vocabulaire du Grec Vul-

gaire publiés d’après l’Édition de 1622. Paris.

Philippaki-Warburton, Irene. 1977. Modern Greek Clitic Pronouns and the ‘Sur-

face Structure Constraints’ Hypothesis. Journal of Linguistics 13: 259-281.

Philippaki-Warburton, Irene. 1985. Word order in Modern Greek. Transactions

of the Philological Society 83: 113-143.

Philippaki-Warburton, Irene. 1994. Verb Movement and Clitics in Modern

Greek. In I. Philippaki-Warburton, K, Nicolaidis, and M. Sifianou (eds.),
Themes in Greek Linguistics: papers from the first International Conference
on Greek Linguistics, Reading, September 1993
. Amsterdam, Philadelphia:
Benjamins, pp. 53-61.

background image

180

References

Philippaki-Warburton, Irene. 1995.

Διαχρονικ7 θεẃρηση τς θéσης τν γ-

κλιτικν μéσα στ7ν πρóταση. Studies in Greek Linguistics 15: 123-134.

Pichard, Michel. 1956. Le roman de Callimaque et de Chrysorrhoé. Paris: Les

Belles-Lettres.

Pintzuk, Susan. 1995. Variation and Change in Old English Clause Structure.

Language Variation and Change 7: 229-60.

Portius, Simon. 1638. See Meyer 1889.
Psichari, Jean. 1884. Essai de Phonétique Néo-Grecque. Futur Composé du Grec

Moderne. Paris: Imprimerie Nationale.

Ramat, Anna G. 1998. Testing the boundaries of grammaticalization. In A. G.

Ramat and P. J. Hopper (eds.), The limits of grammaticalization. Amster-
dam; Philadelphia: Benjamins.

Ramsden, Herbert. 1963. Weak-pronoun position in the early Romance lan-

guages. Manchester: University Press.

Rickford, John R., Arnetha Ball, Renee Blake, Raina Jackson, and Nomi Martin.

1991. Rappin on the Copula Coffin: Theoretical and Methodological Issues
in the Analysis of Copula Variation in African-American Vernacular Eng-
lish. Language Variation and Change 3: 103-132.

Rivero, Maria-Louise. 1993. Clause Structure and V-movement in languages of

the Balkans. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 12: 63-120.

Rivero, Maria-Louise and Arhonto Terzi. 1995. Imperatives, V-Movement and

Logical Mood. Journal of Linguistics 31: 301-332.

Rizzi, Luigi. 1986. On the Status of Subject Clitics in Romance. In Jaeggli,

Osvaldo and Carmen Silva-Corvalan (eds.), Studies in Romance Linguistics.
Dordrecht: Foris, pp. 391-419.

Rollo, Antonio 1989. L’uso dell’enclisi nel Greco Volgare dal XII al XVII

secolo e la legge Tobler-Mussafia. Italoellinika 2: 35-146.

Romaine, Suzanne. 1981. The Status of Variable Rules in Sociolinguistic The-

ory. Journal of Linguistics 17: 93-119.

Romaine, Suzanne. 1984. On the problem of syntactic variation and pragmatic

meaning in sociolinguistic theory. Folia Linguistica 18: 409-437.

Santorini, Beatrice. 1993. The Rate of Phrase Structure Change in the History of

Yiddish. Language Variation and Change 5: 257-283.

Sigur∂sson, Halldór A. 1990. V1 declaratives and Verb raising in Icelandic. In J.

Maling and A. Zaenen (eds.), Modern Icelandic syntax (Syntax and Seman-
tics
24: 41-69).

Smyth, Herbert W. 1956. Greek Grammar. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Unive-

rsity Press.

Sophianos, Nikolaos. 1555. See Papadopoulos 1977.
Spencer, Andrew. 1991. Morphological Theory. Oxford; Cambridge Mass.:

Blackwell. (Reprinted 1995).

background image

References

181

Stavrou, Melita. 1986.

;Η κλιτικ7 %ντωνυμíα στìς περιοριστικèς %ναφορικèς

προτáσεις μεσου %ντικειμéνου ποù ε8σáγονται με το «ποú». Studies in
Greek Linguistics
6: 151-160.

Taylor, Ann. 1990. Clitics and Configurationality in Ancient Greek. Doctoral

Dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

Taylor, Ann. 1994. The change from SOV to SVO in Ancient Greek. Language

Variation and Change 6: 1-37.

Thráinsson, Hoskuldur. 1986. V1, V2, V3 in Icelandic. In Hubert Haider and

Martin Prinzhorn (eds.), Verb Second Phenomena in Germanic Languages.
Dordrecht: Foris, pp. 169-194.

Tobler, Adolf. 1906 (=1889). Pronominales Objekt zu Infinitiv oder Partizipium.

Vermischte Beiträge zur französischen Grammatik 2: 93-103. Leipzig:
Hirzel.

Veloudis, Giannis and Irene Philippaki-Warburton. 1983.

;Η #ποτακτικ7 στà

νéα Kλληνικá. Studies in Greek Linguistics 3: 151-168.

Wackernagel, Jacob. 1892. Über ein Gesetz der indogermanischen Wortstellung.

Indogermanische Forschungen 1: 333-436.

Wanner, Dieter. 1981a. Clitic Placement from Old to Modern Italian: Morpholo-

gization of a Syntactic Rule. In William W. Cressey and Donna Jo Napoli
(eds.), Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages 9, pp. 331-348.

Wanner, Dieter. 1981b. Pragmatics and Syntax in Portuguese Clitic Placement.

In James P. Lantolf and Gregory B. Stone (eds.), Current Research in Ro-
mance Languages
. Bloomington, Ind.: Indiana University Linguistics Club,
pp. 194-206.

Wanner, Dieter. 1987. The development of Romance clitic pronouns: from Latin

to Old Romance. Berlin; New York: M. de Gruyter.

Wanner, Dieter. 1991. The Tobler-Mussafia Law in Old Spanish. In Campos,

Hector and Fernando Martinez-Gil, (eds.), Current Studies in Spanish Lin-
guistics
. Washington, DC: Georgetown UP, pp. 313-378.

Wanner, Dieter. 1996. Second position clitics in Medieval Romance. In Aaron L.

Halpern and Arnold M. Zwicky (eds.), Approaching Second: second position
clitics and related phenomena
. CSLI Publications, pp. 537-578.

Warburton, Irene P. (=Philippaki-Warburton). 1970. Rules of accentuation in

Classical and Modern Greek. Glotta 48: 107-121.

Warburton, Irene P. (=Philippaki-Warburton). 1975. The Passive in English and

Greek. Foundations of Language: 563-78.

Weinreich, Uriel, William Labov, and Marvin I. Herzog. 1968. Empirical foun-

dations for a theory of language change. In Winfred P. Lehmann and Yakov
Malkiel (eds.), Directions for historical linguistics. Austin: University of
Texas Press, pp. 97-195.

West, Martin L. 1982. Greek metre. Oxford; New York: Clarendon Press.
Winford, Donald. 1984. The linguistic variable and syntactic variation in creole

continua. Lingua 62: 267-288.

background image

182

References

Winford, Donald. 1990. Copula variability, accountability, and the concept of

‘Polylectal’ grammars. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 5: 223-252.

Winford, Donald. 1992. Another Look at the Copula in Black English and Car-

ibbean Creoles. American Speech: A Quarterly of Linguistic Usage 67: 21-
60.

Winford, Donald. 1993. Variability in the Use of Perfect Have in Trinidadian

English: A Problem of Categorial and Semantic Mismatch. Language Varia-
tion and Change
5: 141-87.

Woods, A., P. Fletcher, and A. Hughes. 1986. Statistics in language studies.

Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.

Zwicky, Arnold M. 1977. On clitics. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguis-

tics Club.

Zwicky, Arnold M. 1985. Clitics and Particles. Language 61: 283-305.
Zwicky, Arnold M. 1994. What is a clitic? In Joel A.Nevis, Brian D. Joseph,

Dieter Wanner, and Arnold M. Zwicky (eds.), Clitics: A Comprehensive
Bibliography 1892-1991
. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. xii-xx.

Zwicky, Arnold M. and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 1983. Cliticization vs. Inflection:

English n’t. Language 59: 502-513.

background image

183

Index

A

accent 27, 53, 66ff, 74ff, 80, 101,
148
accent shift 98
accountability, principle of 22ff
accusative, use of 2-3, 27
accusative-for-genitive 2
adjunction 103
affix 13, 106, 137ff

atypical, 137, 139, 141ff
in Modern Greek 138
phrasal 107, 139
verbal 138
‘Wackernagel’ 141

affixhood, criteria for 137
analogical change 2, 63, 117,
128ff, 140, 143ff, 149
analogy, four-part 54, 143
Ancient Greek 3, 37, 47, 61, 115
aphaeresis 4, 97ff
argument incorporation 62ff
attraction, of pronouns 11, 57ff

B

broad-range rule 149
Bulgarian 111ff

C

caesura 73, 82ff, 114
clitic 137ff

directional 102, 112
lexical 13
non-directional 102, 112
simple 12
special 13
syntactic 13

true 137
verbal, 13, 111

clitic climbing 62ff
clitic group 111
clitichood, criteria for 137
conditional conjunction 9, 32, 48,
98, 103
conditional periphrasis 64
constant rate effect 144ff
constellations 148, 150
Cretan Greek 2, 21, 118
Cretan Renaissance 22, 120
Cypriot Greek 2, 22, 31, 84ff, 120,
123ff, 144

D

dative alternation 147ff
dativization rule 147
definite article 2, 27-28, 64
diachronic development 55, 125ff,
139, 147
diachronic syntax 16
ditransitive constructions 51
do 143
Dutch dialects 130

E

Early Medieval Greek 3, 5, 129,
132
emphasis 3, 11, 55ff, 99
English 25

F

focus-topic distinction 57ff, 100,
102
French, Modern 138, 147

background image

184

Index

French, Montreal 25
French, Old 8, 104, 111,
future periphrasis 12, 19, 64ff, 134
future marker 9, 50, 95, 140, 143

G

generalizations 11, 37, 132, 146ff
genitive, use of 2
German dialects 130
gerund 6, 40, 60, 63ff, 70, 85, 90,
95, 99, 104, 119, 133
grammaticalization 141ff

H

Heptanese Greek 21, 88, 119
Homeric Greek 3

I

Icelandic, Old 107
imperative 6, 28, 60, 70ff, 80ff, 82,
99, 105, 113, 119, 133
imperative morpheme 94ff
indicative 30, 70, 104, 129
indicative morpheme 94
Indo-European 3
infinitive 72, 90, 104, 133
infinitive complement 64
infinitive in periphrastic tenses
65ff, 121, 133
infinitive morpheme 95
infinitive, articular 64
infinitive, circumstantial 19
interpolation 31, 109, 124, 137
Italian, Old 104

K

Koiné Greek 3, 5, 16, 20, 142

L

left dislocation 109
lexically particularized rule 149

linear precedence model 131ff
linguistic competence 15, 23
Lithuanian 140

M

meta-templates 148
meter 74ff, 123
morphological analysis 114

N

narrative inversion 107
narrow-range rule 149
negation 8, 40, 44ff, 63, 70, 93ff,
103, 119, 126ff, 139, 144

O

optative 129

P

poetry, Cretan see Cretan
Renaissance
poetry, oral 123
poetry, romantic 18
participles 62-63, 133
partitive construction 53
passive competence 124
perfect periphrasis 62
pluperfect periphrasis 62
politikos verse 73
Pontic Greek 50
product-oriented rule 149
pronouns, full 4

possessive 3, 26
relative 27
weak 2

proparoxytone 27, 66, 101
prosodic analysis 114
prosodic inversion 100, 102, 111

S

second position 4, 45, 93, 107, 111

background image

Index

185

Spanish, Old 104ff
stylistic operation 106
subjunctive 32, 70, 104, 129, 133
subjunctive marker 94, 99, 103,
126, 129, 146
synchronic syntax 17, 132

T

Tobler-Mussafia Law 104, 107
Trinidad Creole 25

U

unidirectionality 147
univerbation 148

V

variable rules 150
variable, linguistic 22ff
variant 22
variationist approach 22
verb movement 95, 106, 145
verb-second language 107

asymmetric 107
symmetric 107

vernacular 12, 22, 48, 73, 122, 132

W

Wackernagel Law 4, 141


Document Outline


Wyszukiwarka

Podobne podstrony:
Notto R Thelle Buddhism and Christianity in Japan From Conflict to Dialogue, 1854 1899, 1987
Far Infrared Energy Distributions of Active Galaxies in the Local Universe and Beyond From ISO to H
ebooksclub org Women and Race in Contemporary U S Writing From Faulkner to Morrison American Literat
Armstrong; From Huponoia to Paranoia On the Secular Co optation of Homeric Religion in Vico, Feuerba
John N Duvall Race and the White Identity in Southern Fiction From Faulkner to Morrison
12 Naturalness and Morphological Change
Key Concepts in Language and Linguistics
Adaptive Filters in MATLAB From Novice to Expert
Zizek From politics to Biopolitics and back
Phonetics in language and linguistics
Russian and Chinese Methods of going from Communism to?moc
Phonetics and Phonology 11 12 13, Gimson Chapter 5 Sounds in Language
Key Concepts in Language and Linguistics
From Bosnia to Baghdad The Evolution of U S Army Special Forces from 1995 2004
T A Chase Darkness and Light 2 From Slavery to Freedom
5 Diversity and Stability in Language
Unsolved Mysteries An Exhibition of Unsolved Mysteries and Enigmatic Findings in the History of Hum
Graham Cole & Michael Schluter FROM PERSONALISM TO RELATIONISM COMMONALITIES AND DISTINCTIVES
Phonetics in language and linguistics

więcej podobnych podstron