Online Bargaining and Interpersonal Trust
Charles E. Naquin
University of Notre Dame
Gaylen D. Paulson
University of Texas at Austin
The presented study explores the effect of interacting over the Internet on interpersonal trust when
bargaining online. Relative to face-to-face negotiations, online negotiations were characterized by (a)
lower levels of pre-negotiation trust and (b) lower levels of post-negotiation trust. The reduced levels of
pre-negotiation trust in online negotiations (i.e., before any interaction took place) demonstrate that
negotiators bring different expectations to the electronic bargaining table than to face-to-face negotia-
tions. These negative perceptions of trust were found to mediate another aspect of the relationship,
namely, desired future interaction. Those who negotiated online reported less desire for future interac-
tions with the other party. Online negotiators also were less satisfied with their outcome and less
confident in the quality of their performance, despite the absence of observable differences in economic
outcome quality.
The Internet has significantly influenced the ways in which
people communicate and exchange information both within and
between organizations (Katsh & Rifkin, 2001; Kiesler & Sproull,
1992). It has been estimated that approximately 80% of business
organizations now rely on electronic messaging (e.g., e-mail) as a
critical means of communication for everyday operations (Overly,
1999). For example, at Intel (the popular computer chip manufac-
turer) employees reportedly spend an average of 2.5 hr per day
interacting via e-mail, generating over 3 million messages per day
(Overholt, 2001). On a broad level, it is clear that with increasing
globalization and the emergence of “virtual workplaces” and rap-
idly changing economic landscapes, e-mail communication repre-
sents an indispensable business and managerial tool.
How do these changing modes of communication affect orga-
nizations and their members? Recognizing that managers spend a
good deal of their time negotiating and dealing with conflicts, it
comes as no surprise that practitioners and scholars alike have
sought to explore the ramifications of using the Internet when
crafting deals and resolving disputes (see Katsh & Rifkin, 2001;
Landry, 2000; McKersie & Fonstad, 1997; Thompson, 2001).
Conferences and workshops devoted to negotiating and resolving
disputes in the online context have been sponsored by such orga-
nizations as the Federal Trade Commission, the U.S. Department
of Commerce, and the European Union (Katsh & Rifkin, 2001).
From a more scholarly perspective, online negotiations have been
compared with traditional face-to-face interactions in such areas as
outcome quality (Barsness & Tenbrunsel, 1998; Rangaswamy &
Shell, 1997), time on task (Hiltz, Johnson, & Turoff, 1986; Kiesler
& Sproull, 1992), and the tone of the process (Sproull & Kiesler,
1991).
One common thread in the electronic communication literature
is that messages sent via e-mail are, in their current text-based
format, lacking when it comes to conveying certain types of
information (Kiesler, 1986; Trevino, Daft, & Lengel, 1990). Spe-
cifically, in the absence of nonverbal cues, e-mail users may find
it challenging (though not impossible) to send and receive affec-
tive or relational information, particularly in new relationships
(Walther, 1995; Walther & Burgoon, 1992). In e-mail negotia-
tions, these problems might become evident as negotiators attempt
to establish a working relationship and to build the trust that is
often considered necessary to create an efficient and satisfying
agreement (Carnevale & Probst, 1997; Kramer & Tyler, 1996;
Purdy, Nye, & Balakrishnan, 2000). Indeed, trust is often cited as
one of the critical hurdles to overcome in online negotiations
(Katsh & Rifkin, 2001; Keen, Ballance, Chan, & Schrump, 2000)
and in online communication in general (e.g., Duarte & Snyder,
1999; Keen et al., 2000; Overly, 1999; Rosenoer, Armstrong, &
Gates, 1999). International consulting firms such as Ernst &
Young and Deloitte & Touche now offer expert advice in the
development of trust within online settings. Academic research has
also begun to focus on ways negotiators might overcome the
challenges associated with the online context (see Moore,
Kurtzberg, Thompson, & Morris, 1999; Morris, Nadler, Kurtzberg,
& Thompson, 1999), with the goal of enabling users to reap the
benefits offered by the efficiency of the medium (i.e., overcoming
distance and time restrictions) while minimizing the potential
costs.
The present research seeks to extend the understanding of (a)
negotiations and (b) the social–psychological consequences of
interacting via the Internet by empirically examining how negoti-
ating online influences interpersonal trust. Much of the theory and
research on negotiations have focused on economic outcomes, and
only recently have studies begun to explore the more relational or
Charles E. Naquin, Mendoza College of Business, University of Notre
Dame; Gaylen D. Paulson, McCombs School of Business, University of
Texas at Austin.
Portions of this research were presented at the annual conference of the
International Association of Conflict Management, Cergy, France, June
2001. We are indebted to Michael Roloff, James Schmidtke, and Trexler
Proffitt for their assistance at various stages of this project.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Charles
E. Naquin, Mendoza College of Business, University of Notre Dame, Notre
Dame, Indiana 46556. E-mail: charles.naquin.1@nd.edu
Journal of Applied Psychology
Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
2003, Vol. 88, No. 1, 113–120
0021-9010/03/$12.00
DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.113
113
social aspects of the negotiation process. This study provides
further insight into these latter elements, which can be critical for
long-term relationships. To do so, we first explore theory and
research regarding trust on a general level and then move to
consider variations that derive from the online environment.
Perspectives on Trust in Negotiations
In recent years, scholars have begun to give more attention to
the social and relational forces at work within negotiations (e.g.,
Kramer & Messick, 1995; Oliver, Balakrishnan, & Barry, 1994;
Thompson, 2001), with interpersonal trust often cited as a central
component to promoting cooperation and goodwill (Mayer, Davis,
& Schoorman, 1995; McAllister, 1995). Whereas its importance is
frequently noted, trust has historically proved to be an elusive
construct with multiple interpretations (Lewicki, McAllister, &
Bies, 1998).
In this article, we adopt the cross-disciplinary view of trust
provided by Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, and Camerer (1998), who
defined trust as “a psychological state comprising the intention to
accept vulnerability based upon positive expectations of the inten-
tions or behaviors of another” (p. 395). From this perspective, trust
becomes most poignant when there exists potential for significant
loss on the part of the one who is trusting. Any benefits of
exhibiting trust in a negotiation (e.g., cooperating) must therefore
be weighed against the potential risk for exploitation or loss
(Deutsch, 1958; Tyler & Kramer, 1996). As such, significant time
and energy may go into assessing the trustworthiness of negotia-
tion opponents. In addition, persons may also spend time and
energy toward encouraging others to place trust in them.
Scholars have long recognized that there may be many reasons
people choose to trust one another (Deutsch, 1973). Building from
work in organizational relationship development, Lewicki and
Wiethoff (2000) suggested that there are two primary routes to
trust development (see also Lewicki & Bunker, 1995, 1996; Sha-
piro, Sheppard, & Cheraskin, 1992): one that is contextually based
and another that is more relationally based. In “deterrence-based”
trust (also referred to as “calculus-based” trust), negotiators essen-
tially engage in a contextual risk analysis of sorts when determin-
ing whether to trust the other party. This analysis is typically
rooted in the extent to which one has the ability to punish or deter
the undesirable behaviors that constitute trust violations (Butler &
Cantrell, 1984; Lewicki & Bunker, 1996; Rempel & Holmes,
1986; Ring & Van de Ven, 1992; Shapiro et al., 1992; Williamson,
1993). For example, if a negotiator can impact his or her oppo-
nent’s reputation or social viability, exploitation by the other party
may be perceived as less likely to occur. Trustworthy behavior,
therefore, can be reliably predicted to the extent that it represents
the most rational choice for the opponent in pursuit of his or her
own self-interest. As such, deterrence-based trust incorporates a
contextual analysis whereby negotiators assess, and perhaps ma-
nipulate, external pressures guiding behavioral choices.
The second route to establishing trust, termed “identification-
based” trust, is more relationally based and extends beyond that
which can be explained by a contextual analysis. The foundation
for this perspective is consistent with social identity theory (Tajfel,
1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1981), which emphasizes the perception of
oneness with or belongingness to another. Research in social
identity theory suggests that individuals define themselves partly
in terms of salient group memberships, such as organizational
affiliation, religion, shared interests, and so forth. Such a shared
sense of identity among negotiators may promote a deeper under-
standing of the other individual’s thoughts and actions and thereby
enhance perceived predictability. In addition, shared identity can
create a sense of empathy and concern for the outcomes of the
other. In essence, a shared sense of identity yields a more trusting
relationship (Lewicki & Wiethoff, 2000; Tyler & Kramer, 1996).
In both deterrence- and identification-based trust, negotiators
seek to eliminate or minimize levels of uncertainty. Although high
levels of trust may be hard to attain, attempting to do so from a
distance—such as over the Internet—may be especially challeng-
ing. We now turn our attention to these challenges.
Trust and the Online Environment
There is a growing stream of research examining the effects of
Internet-based communication, such as e-mail, on social interac-
tion and decision making. Because of its almost exclusive reliance
on text-based messaging, the current e-mail system constitutes a
fairly “lean” form of media, with important cues rendered unavail-
able for senders and receivers (Daft, 1984; Trevino et al., 1990).
Negotiators in these contexts must overcome significant limita-
tions not faced by those interacting in a traditional face-to-face
setting.
In particular, whereas e-mail provides an excellent means for
conveying information and task-based content, it is not optimal for
conveying relational messages. Indeed, relational information may
be most efficiently conveyed via nonverbal channels (i.e., those
not translatable into text), with verbal-based channels serving as an
uncomfortable alternative (Watzlavick, Beavin, & Jackson, 1967).
Given that judgments of trust are often viewed, at least in part, as
relational in nature (Lewicki & Bunker, 1995; Shapiro et al.,
1992), making judgments regarding trust exclusively by way of a
text-based channel may feel awkward and constrained. Though
negotiators may attempt to overcome the relational limitations of
this text-based channel (see Thompson, 2001), efforts here may be
difficult or inappropriate within certain organizational settings
(e.g., use of typed symbols to represent an emotional state, com-
monly referred to as “emoticons”). Thus, we predict that negoti-
ators who exclusively communicate over the Internet via e-mail
will be less trusting of others than those who interact face-to-face
as well.
Hypothesis 1: Negotiators who bargain online will exhibit
lower levels of trust than those who interact face-to-face.
Indeed, past research lends support to the first hypothesis re-
garding trust. Specifically, persons interacting at a distance report
lower levels of trust following a negotiation (Fortune & Brodt,
2000; Moore et al., 1999). However, much of the prior research
and theory has focused on trust as being primarily reactive, such
that the process of interacting serves as the means by which trust
differences are created. In contrast, we argue that negotiators
actually have a more proactive approach when assessing trust in
online negotiations and that it is these initial expectations (i.e.,
pre-negotiation) that may influence the relationship between par-
ties. We base this prediction on three interrelated streams of
research that, all combined, suggest that pre-negotiation percep-
114
NAQUIN AND PAULSON
tions of trust may be influenced by Internet-based communication
media.
First, the difficulties resulting from the communication channel
restriction lead to another challenge that involves the expected
ease of encoding, and expected difficulties in decoding, deceptive
messages. Although verbal inconsistencies may be an important
factor for determining the veracity of messages, people rely
heavily on visual indicators of arousal and nonverbal leakage when
assessing sincerity (e.g., Anderson, Ansfield, & DePaulo, 1999;
Ekman, 1985). Where nonverbal information is unavailable, as is
the case with text-based messages, negotiators may find it easier to
engage in exaggerations, bluffs, and outright lies (Valley, Moag, &
Bazerman, 1998). If a negotiator recognizes that his or her oppo-
nent has these sinister opportunities available, feelings of uncer-
tainty regarding the other party’s behaviors may increase and the
generation of trust may be inhibited.
In addition, it is also recognized that the potential for a broad
array of unethical and hurtful behaviors increases when a person is
removed, physically or psychologically, from the victim (Kelman
& Hamilton, 1989). Thus, whereas the potential to exploit another
individual in a face-to-face negotiation exists, such potential be-
havior may be perceived as being even greater when dealing with
a “faceless” opponent (Walther, 1995).
Finally, these uncertainties and anxieties regarding exploitation
are further aggravated by the “sinister attribution error.” As
Kramer (1994, 1995) noted, people facing diverse challenges and
stress may develop a heightened sense of paranoia. This could
make any initial concern about negotiating online an even more
salient obstacle to overcome.
Taken together, these three interrelated lines of research suggest
that negotiating online encourages the perception that opportuni-
ties are ripe for unethical behavior and, as such, the risk for being
taken advantage of is high. In essence, the online medium yields
significant pre-negotiation cause for concern and leads us to the
following prediction:
Hypothesis 2: Negotiators who bargain online will exhibit
lower levels of pre-negotiation trust (i.e., before any interac-
tion) than those who interact face-to-face.
In a similar fashion, we expected that the reduced levels of
interpersonal trust within the online context would have an influ-
ence in another relational aspect of the negotiation, specifically,
desired future interaction. In this regard, we expected that the
increased uncertainty presented in the e-mail context (as discussed
above) would result in negotiators being less satisfied with the
quality of their interpersonal relationship than those who negotiate
face-to-face. As such, we predicted that the hypothesized low
levels of trust in online negotiations would result in strained
relations such that online negotiators would have less desire to
have future interactions with the other party than those who
negotiate face-to-face.
Hypothesis 3a: Negotiators who have bargained online will
have less desire for future interaction with their opponent than
those who have interacted face-to-face.
Hypothesis 3b: The desire for future interaction will be me-
diated by perceptions of an opponent’s trustworthiness.
Method
Participants and Research Design
Participants were 134 full-time graduate-level business students who
participated in the study as part of a negotiation class assignment. The
experimental design had one manipulation: mode of communication,
wherein negotiations were conducted either exclusively over the Internet
via e-mail or face-to-face. Negotiating dyads were randomly assigned to
one of the two communication media and served as the primary unit of
analysis for our hypothesis tests. In total, 35 dyads negotiated via e-mail,
and 32 dyads interacted face-to-face.
Participants were also assigned roles and partners randomly, with the
restriction that no participant negotiate with another from the same class
section. This cross-class context not only made the need for e-mail com-
munication credible, it also ensured that participants had not completed any
previous negotiation-class simulations together. In addition, whereas in
some cases participants may have known their opponent, any potential
effects here were controlled for by the random assignment of participants
across conditions.
Procedure
Each individual was provided with a packet of material containing the
negotiation case information, confidential role instructions, and an un-
sealed envelope containing a questionnaire. In the confidential instructions,
participants in the e-mail condition were told that their negotiation was to
take place exclusively via e-mail, and they were provided with the name
and e-mail address of the other party. Similarly, in the face-to-face con-
dition, participants were instructed that the negotiation was to be done
exclusively face-to-face and were provided with the name of the other
party and a phone number by which to arrange a meeting.
Participants were informed both verbally and in the written instructions
that their goal in this negotiation was to maximize their economic outcome.
Participants were motivated to perform well on account of two sets of
reasons: Intrinsically, students indicate an enjoyment of these cases and
show enthusiasm toward achieving high-quality outcomes. Extrinsically,
course routine indicated that negotiated outcomes would be posted and
discussed during post-negotiation case debriefings. As such, the anticipa-
tion of a “public” display of individual performance relative to others
provided the incentive to perform well as a matter of competitive pride.
Participants were given either a pre- or post-negotiation questionnaire,
but not both. Those who received post-negotiation questionnaires were not
also given a pre-negotiation questionnaire because of concerns that pre-
negotiation questions might prime participants’ trust levels. Trial runs of
the experimental manipulation raised concerns and comments that the
pre-negotiation trust measure actually led participants to be less trusting of
the other party during the negotiation. Consequently, this was addressed by
having participants who completed a post-negotiation questionnaire forgo
the completion of a pre-negotiation questionnaire. In addition, because we
were working in a classroom setting and were concerned about the appear-
ance of parallel treatment, each person received only a single envelope of
material to complete. Consequently, those who received pre-negotiation
questionnaires did not receive the post-negotiation questionnaire. Pre- and
post-negotiation questionnaires were assigned randomly to dyads across
the two conditions. Thirty-three dyads received pre-negotiation question-
naires (n
⫽ 17 dyads in the e-mail condition, n ⫽ 16 dyads in the
face-to-face condition), and 34 dyads received post-negotiation question-
naires (n
⫽ 18 dyads in the e-mail condition, n ⫽ 16 dyads in the
face-to-face condition).
Those who were assigned pre-negotiation questionnaires were instructed
to open and complete the pre-negotiation questionnaire after reading the
case material and approximately 1 hr before commencing the negotiation.
They were then instructed to seal the pre-negotiation questionnaire in the
provided envelope. In a similar fashion, participants who were given a
115
ONLINE BARGAINING AND INTERPERSONAL TRUST
post-negotiation questionnaire were instructed to complete and seal the
post-negotiation questionnaire within 30 min of completing the negotia-
tion. All questionnaires were returned, along with outcome information,
after the negotiation was completed. Participants were allotted 3 weeks to
complete the negotiation, and outcomes and questionnaire material could
be turned in at any time during those 3 weeks.
Negotiation Task
The negotiation task involved two managers representing separate divi-
sions of the same company negotiating for an intra-organizational transfer
of technology (Bazerman & Brett, 1997). This simulation, “El-Tek,” in-
volves multiple issues that vary in importance for each party and, as such,
contains both fixed-sum and variable-sum elements. All outcome alterna-
tives have explicitly defined payoffs ranging from a maximum joint payoff
of $15.1 million that is distributed between the two parties (as determined
in the negotiation) to the impasse payoff in which the seller gets $5 million
and the buyer gets $0.
Dependent Variables
Pre- and post-negotiation trust.
To assess individuals’ level of trust in
their counterparts, we used the Organizational Trust Inventory—Short
Form (OTI–SF) developed and statistically validated by Cummings and
Bromiley (1996). This scale contains 12 items geared toward assessing
three dimensions of trust, including the reliability, honesty, and good faith
of the other party with respect to fulfilling their commitments. In addition,
it also assesses both an affective and a cognitive component for each
dimension. We modified the OTI–SF slightly to be more appropriate for a
dyadic negotiation setting by asking individually based questions (e.g., “I
think” rather than “We think”) and by substituting the words “the other
party” where the original questionnaire stated the name of the “other
department” or “unit.” The reliability for this measure in the present study
was acceptable, with Cronbach’s
␣ ⫽ .70. The modified OTI–SF, as was
used to measure post-negotiation trust, is presented in the Appendix. The
pre-negotiation OTI–SF that was used was equivalent to the one in the
Appendix, with the exception that it was phrased in the future tense.
Outcome quality.
Both the economic outcome and participants’ per-
ceptions of the negotiated outcome were assessed. To assess objective
outcome quality we used an economic measure of success, assessed with
dollar gains. The El-Tek simulation provides an explicitly defined payoff
schedule (included in the case material given to each participant) that
readily yields quantifiable payoffs as a function of the negotiated deal.
These outcomes were analyzed with respect to participants’ ability to
capture all available gains (i.e., integrativeness or efficiency) and also with
respect to the distribution of those gains. Integrativeness was measured
with total joint gains, represented by the sum of both parties’ outcomes. For
the distribution of gains, we analyzed the dispersion of the agreements. In
this case, higher levels of variation in one condition relative to the other
might be indicative of a greater willingness to claim gains at the other’s
expense, whereas a smaller variation might suggest a tendency toward
more “balanced” deals. We measured subjective perceptions of outcome
quality by participants’ responses to two questions. First, participants’
perceptions of the quality of their performance was assessed by responses
to the question, “How well do you think you performed in this negotiation
compared to others also playing your role? I did better than
% of the
others playing my role.” Second, participants’ overall satisfaction with the
negotiated outcome was evaluated on the basis of responses to the follow-
ing question: “How satisfied are you with the negotiated outcome?” (1
⫽
not satisfied at all, 7
⫽ very satisfied).
Desired future interaction.
From a relational perspective, we measured
participants’ willingness to have a continued relationship through re-
sponses to this question: “Based upon your experience in this negotiation
with the other side, to what degree are you willing to have future dealings
(i.e., negotiations) with them? Please give your response on a scale of 1 to
100, with 1 being not at all and 100 being without hesitation.”
Results
Our reported analyses were conducted at the dyadic level. No-
tably, the reported pattern of results was found to be equivalent
whether the analysis was conducted at an individual or a dyadic
level. However, because dependent variables were highly corre-
lated within dyads, we combined individual measures to form
dyadic measures in order to eliminate any dependency concerns
within the data (Kenny, 1995).
Post-negotiation trust.
Our first hypothesis examined post-
negotiation trust levels. Consistent with our prediction, and as
illustrated in Figure 1, a significant difference was observed in
post-negotiation trust levels based on our manipulation of com-
munication medium. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) on these
data indicated that participants who negotiated online via e-mail
reported significantly lower post-negotiation trust levels (M
⫽
3.66, SD
⫽ 0.28) as compared with those who communicated
exclusively face-to-face (M
⫽ 4.56, SD ⫽ 0.18), F(1, 32) ⫽
118.23, p
⬍ .01,
2
⫽ .78. The means, standard deviations, and
intercorrelations between post-negotiation variables are shown in
Table 1.
Pre-negotiation trust.
Supporting our second hypothesis re-
garding pre-negotiation trust, a significant difference in trust was
also evident for negotiators who completed pre-negotiation ques-
tionnaires. In particular, online negotiators reported significantly
lower pre-negotiation trust (M
⫽ 3.41, SD ⫽ 0.20) than did their
face-to-face counterparts (M
⫽ 4.29, SD ⫽ 0.29), F(1, 31) ⫽
100.22, p
⬍ .01,
2
⫽ .76.
Even though pre- and post-negotiation questionnaires were ad-
ministered to different groups at different times, we performed a
post hoc exploration into the magnitude of the difference between
pre- and post-negotiation measures of trust across media. When the
data were entered into a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA)
with the media manipulation, a main effect for time was observed
for levels of reported trust, F(1, 63)
⫽ 18.44, p ⬍ .01,
2
⫽ .23,
wherein post-negotiation trust was higher than pre-negotiation
trust for all groups. The interaction of time with the manipulation,
however, was not significant, F(1, 63)
⫽ 0.04, ns, suggesting that
improvements in trust were equivalent for e-mail and face-to-face
participants. Thus, the initial negative expectations evidenced for
e-mail negotiators did not generate a downward spiral of hostility
Figure 1.
Mean interpersonal trust as a function of communication me-
dia. Open bars represent e-mail; solid bars represent face-to-face.
116
NAQUIN AND PAULSON
in the interactions, nor was the interaction enough to overcome the
initial deficiency in trust exhibited in the pre-negotiation measure.
Desired future interaction.
In Hypothesis 3A we predicted that
participants who negotiated online would be less inclined to desire
a future relationship than those who negotiated face-to-face. This
hypothesis was confirmed. Online negotiators reported a signifi-
cantly lower interest in future relations (M
⫽ 33.28, SD ⫽ 16.92)
than did persons in the face-to-face condition (M
⫽ 46.09,
SD
⫽ 16.68), F(1, 32) ⫽ 4.92, p ⫽ .03,
2
⫽ .14.
Objective outcome quality.
Outcome quality is reported in two
ways: objective and subjective. First we report an analysis of the
objective outcomes. The objective outcomes were turned in as
regular class submissions and were available for all participants
regardless of whether they had completed the pre- or post-
negotiation questionnaire. Consequently, the reported analysis of
objective outcomes includes all participants.
With respect to impasse rate, a logistic regression analysis
revealed that no difference was observed between communication
media for impasse rate, with 6.3% of dyads in the face-to-face
condition impassing (n
⫽ 32) versus 8.6% in the e-mail condition
(n
⫽ 35),
 ⫽ .34, ns.
Similarly, no differences were observed for either the creation of
joint gains or the distribution of gains. Dyads in the e-mail con-
dition (M
⫽ 13.32, SD ⫽ 2.89) captured an equivalent number of
points to those in the face-to-face condition (M
⫽ 13.38,
SD
⫽ 2.61), F(1, 65) ⫽ .01, ns. To examine the balance of gains
between individuals, we used a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (see
Sheskin, 2000). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test compares two dis-
tributions to determine whether they are equal. Higher levels of
variation in one condition relative to the other might be indicative
of a greater willingness to claim gains at the other’s expense,
whereas a smaller variation might suggest a tendency toward more
“balanced” deals. Results suggested that e-mail (M
⫽ 6.65,
SD
⫽ 1.94) and face-to-face (M ⫽ 6.69, SD ⫽ 1.66) participants
had statistically equivalent variance (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Z
⫽
.51, ns). In total, no differences were observed for joint gains or in
the distribution of gains as reflected in individual outcomes.
One final exploration into objective outcome quality revealed
that the economic indicators showed no connection to negotiator
trust levels. In a regression analysis neither pre-negotiation trust
(
 ⫽ .23, ns) nor post-negotiation trust ( ⫽ .32, ns) was predic-
tive of negotiated joint outcomes. Similarly, neither pre-
negotiation trust (
 ⫽ .36, ns) nor post-negotiation trust ( ⫽ .35,
ns) was predictive of individual objective outcome.
Subjective outcome quality.
In contrast to the objective out-
come data that were available for all participants, subjective per-
ceptual measures were limited to those who completed the post-
negotiation questionnaire (n
⫽ 34 dyads). Here, significant
differences were found in the subjective perceptions of outcomes.
Participants in the e-mail condition reported lower confidence in
their performance relative to others in their simulation role
(M
⫽ 0.37, SD ⫽ 0.17) than did those in the face-to-face condition
(M
⫽ 0.65, SD ⫽ 0.11), F(1, 32) ⫽ 33.97, p ⬍ .01,
2
⫽ .52.
Participants in the e-mail condition were also less satisfied with
their outcome (M
⫽ 2.97, SD ⫽ 0.63) than those in the face-to-face
condition (M
⫽ 3.91, SD ⫽ 0.95), F(1, 32) ⫽ 11.62, p ⬍ .01,
2
⫽
.27. Whereas online communication did not have a significant
effect on the objective economic nature of the outcome, it did have
a significant influence on how those outcomes were perceived.
Mediational analyses.
Analyses were conducted to determine
whether interpersonal trust mediated any of the following three
variables: relational satisfaction (per Hypothesis 3B), outcome
satisfaction, and confidence in performance. Following Baron and
Kenny’s (1986) recommended methodology for mediational anal-
ysis, we regressed the following: (a) the dependent variable on the
independent variable, that is, media; (b) the purported mediating
variable, post-negotiation trust, on the independent variable; and
(c) the dependent variable on both the independent variable and the
mediating variable. These analyses revealed that for (a) partici-
pants’ confidence in their performance and (b) their outcome
satisfaction, trust was not a significant mediator (
 ⫽ .08, ns;  ⫽
.02, ns, respectively). However, in support of Hypothesis 3b, trust
was found to be the mediating mechanism for the desire to work
together again in the future. Significant results were observed for
the influence of trust on desired future interaction (
 ⫽ .35, df ⫽
33, p
⬍ .01), and they remained even when entered with the
communication media manipulation (
 ⫽ .31, df ⫽ 33, p ⬍ .05).
The effect of media was concurrently reduced to nonsignificance
(
 ⫽ .08, ns). In summary, post-negotiation trust in a negotiation
was found to be a significant mediator for desired future interac-
tion but not for perceptions of outcome satisfaction or one’s
confidence in performance.
General Discussion
The goal of the present research was to extend our understand-
ing of (a) negotiation and (b) the social–psychological conse-
quences of interacting via the Internet by empirically examining
how negotiating online influences interpersonal trust. The reported
findings demonstrate that negotiators who bargain online may
bring different expectations to the electronic bargaining table than
when they negotiate face-to-face, resulting in a host of potential
detrimental consequences on the perceptual and relational dynam-
ics in a negotiation. For example, trust in the opposing party of a
pending negotiation was significantly lower in the online context
than in the face-to-face condition. This was before any interaction
commenced and thus was necessarily rooted in expectations rather
than in processes within the negotiation. In addition, our prediction
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Between
Post-Negotiation Dependent Variables, Dyad Level
Variable
1
2
3
4
5
1. Trust
—
.50**
.39*
.65**
.14
2. Future interaction
—
.12
.39*
⫺.05
3. Outcome satisfaction
—
.29
.04
4. Perceived performance
—
⫺.08
5. Joint outcome
—
M
4.08
39.31
3.41
0.50
13.23
SD
0.51
17.78
0.92
0.20
3.01
Minimum
1
1
1
0
5
Maximum
7
100
7
1
15.10
Note.
The minimum and maximum values listed in the table are theoret-
ical rather than actual (they specify the most extreme possible outcomes,
not actual results).
* p
⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
117
ONLINE BARGAINING AND INTERPERSONAL TRUST
that the desired future interaction would be damaged in the online
context was confirmed, and this effect was mediated by trust.
Finally, although agreement rates and outcome quality measures
were parallel between online and face-to-face contexts, the levels
of confidence and outcome satisfaction were much higher in the
latter condition. These interpersonal deficiencies and perceptual
concerns suggest that persons seeking a more lasting exchange
relationship would be wise to build a base for their interactions
through some other medium (e.g., Drolet & Morris, 1995) or to
take steps to correct for initial trust problems.
These findings may appear somewhat at odds with the argu-
ments presented by Drolet and Morris (1995) in their comparisons
of telephone and face-to-face negotiators. Their results indicated a
trend toward differing expectations between media but no signif-
icant limitations therein. Instead, they argued that a lack of non-
verbal cues inhibits the development of interpersonal synchrony
necessary to build rapport, which subsequently inhibits coopera-
tion. In the online context, we recognize that the limitations of the
Internet are even more restrictive (Trevino et al., 1990) as the
nonverbal cues carried via vocal inflections and intonation (i.e.,
“vocalics”) are removed as well. Additionally, differences ob-
served in the pre-negotiation (i.e., pre-interaction) trust levels
cannot be accounted for by nonverbal behaviors. Although we do
not have data with respect to behavioral synchrony or rapport, our
findings are clearly suggestive that differences in some post-
negotiation perceptions and intentions are, at least partially, im-
pacted by differences in initial expectations.
These findings also contribute to the literature by suggesting
that the greatest problems for online negotiations may be associ-
ated with creating positive relationships and feelings rather than
with the creation of low-quality agreements. This is interesting in
that contextually based conditions (i.e., media) translated into
broad judgments of the personal characteristics of the other party.
In particular, online negotiations yielded lower levels of interper-
sonal trust that, in turn, generated negative assessments with
respect to the suitability of the other individual for future business
dealings.
Our data also showed comparable objective economic gains
between online and face-to-face negotiations but significant dif-
ferences when it came to how those outcomes were perceived.
Participants who negotiated via e-mail had lower confidence in the
quality of their deal and lower overall satisfaction with the out-
come than did those who negotiated face-to-face. These findings
regarding outcome perceptions, however, were not directly medi-
ated by trust. As such, the influence of trust on these subjective
perceptions may instead be tied more closely to differences within
the negotiation processes or in the messaging system itself (e.g.,
Morris et al., 1999).
These findings of more negative subjective perceptions of out-
comes may also help to explain why prior research has found that
online negotiators have a more difficult time reaching an agree-
ment (Moore et al., 1999). To the extent that a negotiator feels that
he or she is receiving a low-quality offer or outcome, impasse may
be viewed as the most sensible choice regardless of absolute gain
(Neale & Bazerman, 1991). In the current study no differences
were found in impasse rates between the two experimental condi-
tions: however, we recognize that the vast majority of impasses
observed in other research have arisen amongst negotiators who
were in a distinct out-group (i.e., a competing institution; Moore et
al., 1999; Morris et al., 1999). All participants in the present study
knew that although their opponent was not in the same class
section, they were students at the same university (via the e-mail
address), perhaps yielding an initial basis for identification and
comfort. Thus, competitive choices may have been minimized for
all persons in our study. Alternatively, it is possible that the length
of time required to reach an agreement in an online setting is
greater (Kiesler & Sproull, 1992) such that the ample time offered
to our participants (3 weeks) may have provided an opportunity to
arrive at a negotiated settlement. In cases in which persons are
allotted less time, the media effect on impasse may be stronger.
Unfortunately, negotiators do not always have the luxury of
choosing their communication medium in today’s organizational
landscape. There may be times when the Internet is the only
available or feasible opportunity for communication. If this is the
case, one might make a strategic effort to overcome the reported
hazards of the e-mail context while still enjoying the benefits that
the medium provides. One avenue by which to accomplish this
(building on theory and research in trust development) is through
deterrence- or identity-based approaches. For example, one may
minimize uncertainty by attempting to establish norms governing
appropriate online behaviors or by building a common sense of
identification between parties through an exchange of personal
background (i.e., chitchat). This is an area of considerable practical
importance and warrants further research.
Further research into the boundary conditions of these reported
media effects is warranted. For example, in negotiations in which
e-mail messages are knowingly archived, some forms of deception
may be problematic, thus promoting an atmosphere of trust. Hav-
ing statements “on the record” and verifiable may make factual
misrepresentation an unattractive option. However, exaggerated
demands (i.e., bluffs) might occur more readily, as the bluffer
could more easily “keep their story straight” by reviewing their
prior personal dialogue. Researchers might draw on work on the
verbal correlates of deception (e.g., Vrij, Edward, Roberts, & Bull,
2000; Zuckerman & Driver, 1985) to more carefully explore the
opportunity for, and the use of, various forms of misrepresentation
in this context.
Another noteworthy opportunity for future research is rooted in
the limitations of the current study. We speculated that the ob-
served pre-negotiation effects on interpersonal trust may influence
negotiation processes and expectations, such as norms of informa-
tion exchange, strategy selection, or the use of specific tactics (e.g.,
deception, threats). However, our insights were limited by a lack of
interaction data, and this opens the door for future investigation.
We also hold out hope for negotiations in the online context. In
the present study, prior in-class negotiation interactions between
participants were minimized. It may be the case that if negotiations
took place between colleagues with a positive history, their exist-
ing relationship could overcome the problems associated with the
online environment. In such circumstances, the initial pre-
negotiation differences observed in this study may be minimized
or eliminated, yielding more beneficial relational and outcome
perceptions.
References
Anderson, E. D., Ansfield, M. E., & DePaulo, B. M. (1999). Love’s best
habit: Deception in the context of relationships. In P. Philippot, R.
118
NAQUIN AND PAULSON
Feldman, & E. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior:
Studies in emotion and social interaction (pp. 372– 409). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
distinction in social psychology research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 51, 1173–1182.
Barsness, Z., & Tenbrunsel, A. (1998, August). Technologically mediated
communication and negotiation: Do relationships matter? Paper pre-
sented at the annual meeting of the International Association for Conflict
Management, College Park, MD.
Bazerman, M. H., & Brett, J. M. (1997). El-Tek negotiation case. Chicago:
Northwestern University, Dispute Resolution Research Center.
Butler, J. K., & Cantrell, R. S. (1984). A behavioral decision theory
approach to modeling dyadic trust in superiors and subordinates. Psy-
chological Reports, 55, 19 –28.
Carnevale, P. J., & Probst, T. M. (1997). Conflict on the Internet. In S.
Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the Internet (pp. 233–255). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Cummings, L. L., & Bromiley, P. (1996). The organizational trust inven-
tory (OTI): Development and validation. In R. M. Kramer & T. R. Tyler
(Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp.
302–330). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Daft, R. L. (1984). Information richness: A new approach to managerial
behavior and organizational design. Research in Organizational Behav-
ior, 6, 191–233.
Deutsch, M. (1958). Trust and suspicion. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2,
265–279.
Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Drolet, A. L., & Morris, M. W. (1995, August). Communication media and
interpersonal trust in conflicts: The role of rapport and synchrony of
nonverbal behavior. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Acad-
emy of Management, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
Duarte, D., & Snyder, N. (1999). Mastering virtual teams; Strategies,
tools, and techniques that suceed. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics,
and marriage. New York: Norton.
Fortune, A., & Brodt, S. E. (2000, August). Face to face or virtually, for
the second time around: The influence of task, past experience, and
media on trust and deception in negotiation. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Toronto, Ontario,
Canada.
Hiltz, S. R., Johnson, K., & Turoff, M. (1986). Experiments in group
decision making: Communication process and outcome in face-to-face
versus computerized conferences. Human Communication Research, 13,
225–252.
Katsh, E., & Rifkin, J. (2001). Online dispute resolution: Resolving con-
flicts in cyberspace. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Keen, P., Ballance, C., Chan, S., & Schrump. S. (2000). Electronic com-
merce relationships: Trust by design. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Kelman, H. C., & Hamilton, V. L. (1989). Crimes of obedience. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Kenny, D. A. (1995). The effect of nonindependence on significance
testing in dyadic research. Personal Relationships, 2, 67–75.
Kiesler, S. (1986, January 1). The hidden messages in computer networks.
Harvard Business Review, 64, 46 – 60.
Kiesler, S., & Sproull, L. (1992). Group decision making and communi-
cation technology. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
cesses, 52, 96 –123.
Kramer, R. M. (1994). The sinister attribution error: Paranoid cognition
and collective distrust in organizations. Motivation and Emotion, 18,
199 –230.
Kramer, R. M. (1995). Dubious battles: Heightened accountability, dys-
phoric cognition, and self-defeating bargaining behavior. In R. M.
Kramer & D. M. Messick (Eds.), Negotiation as a social process (pp.
95–120). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kramer, R. M., & Messick, D. M. (Eds.). (1995). Negotiation as a social
process. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kramer, R. M., & Tyler, T. R. (1996). Trust in organizations: Frontiers of
theory and research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Landry, E. M. (2000). Scrolling around the new organization: The potential
for conflict in the on-line environment. Negotiation Journal, 16, 133–
142.
Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1995). Trust in relationships: A model of
development and decline. In B. B. Bunker & J. Z. Rubin (Eds.), Conflict,
cooperation, and justice: Essay inspired by the work of Morton Deutsch
(pp. 133–174). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1996). Developing and maintaining trust
in work relationships. In R. M. Kramer & T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in
organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 114 –139). Thou-
sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lewicki, R. J., McAllister, D. J., & Bies, R. J. (1998). Trust and distrust:
New relationships and realities. Academy of Management Review, 23,
438 – 458.
Lewicki, R. J., & Wiethoff, C. (2000). Trust, trust development, and trust
repair. In M. Deutsch & P. Coleman (Eds.), The handbook of conflict
resolution: Theory and practice (pp. 86 –107). San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative
model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20,
709 –734.
McAllister, D. J. (1995). Affect- and cognitive-based trust as foundations
for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management
Journal, 38, 24 –59.
McKersie, R. B., & Fonstad, N. O. (1997). Teaching negotiation theory and
skills over the Internet. Negotiation Journal, 13, 363–368.
Moore, D. A., Kurtzberg, T. R., Thompson, L. L., & Morris, M. W. (1999).
Long and short routes to success in electronically mediated negotiation:
Group affiliations and good vibrations. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 77, 22– 43.
Morris, M. W., Nadler, J., Kurtzberg, T. R., & Thompson, L. (1999,
August). Schmooze or lose: The effects of enriching e-mail interaction to
foster rapport in a negotiation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of
the Academy of Management, Chicago, IL.
Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. (1991). Cognition and rationality in
negotiation. New York: Free Press.
Oliver, R. M., Balakrishnan, P. V., & Barry, B. (1994). Outcome satisfac-
tion in negotiation: A test of expectancy disconfirmation. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 252–275.
Overholt, A. (2001). Intel’s got (too much) mail. Fast Company, 44,
56 – 65.
Overly, M. R. (1999). E-policy. New York: Amacom.
Purdy, J. M., Nye, P., & Balakrishnan, P. V. (2000). The impact of
communication media on negotiation outcomes. International Journal of
Conflict Management, 11, 162–187.
Rangaswamy, A., & Shell, G. R. (1997). Using computers to realize joint
gains in negotiation: Toward an “electronic bargaining table.” Manage-
ment Science, 43, 1147–1163.
Rempel, J. K., & Holmes, J. G. (1986). How do I trust thee? Psychology
Today, 20, 28 –34.
Ring, P. S., & Van de Ven, A. (1992). Structuring cooperative relationships
between organizations. Strategic Management Journal, 13, 483– 498.
Rosenoer, J., Armstrong, D., & Gates, J. R. (1999). Click with trust. New
York: Free Press.
Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so
119
ONLINE BARGAINING AND INTERPERSONAL TRUST
different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Man-
agement Review, 23, 393– 404.
Shapiro, D. L., Sheppard, B. H., & Cheraskin, L. (1992). Business on a
handshake. Negotiation Journal, 8, 365–377.
Sheskin, D. J. (2000). Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statis-
tical procedures (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1991). Connections: New ways of working in the
networked organization. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the
social psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1981). The social identity theory of intergroup
behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of inter-
group relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Thompson, L. (2001). The mind and heart of the negotiator (2nd ed.).
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Trevino, L. K., Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1990). Understanding
managers’ media choices: A symbolic interactionist perspective. In J.
Fulk & C. Steinfield (Eds.), Organizations and communication technol-
ogy (pp. 71–94). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Tyler, T. R., & Kramer, R. M. (1996). Whither trust? In R. M. Kramer &
T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and
research (pp. 1–15). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Valley, K. L., Moag, J., & Bazerman, M. H. (1998). A matter of trust:
Effects of communication on the efficiency and distribution of out-
comes. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 34, 211–238.
Vrij, A., Edward, K., Roberts, K. P., & Bull, R. (2000). Detecting deceit via
analysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behav-
ior, 24, 239 –263.
Walther, J. B. (1995). Relational aspects of computer-mediated communi-
cation: Experimental and longitudinal observations. Organization Sci-
ence, 6, 186 –203.
Walther, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992). Relational communication in
computer-mediated interaction. Human Communication Research, 19,
50 – 88.
Watzlavick, P., Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. D. (1967). Pragmatics of human
communication: A study of interactional patterns, pathologies, and
paradoxes. New York: Norton.
Williamson, O. E. (1993). Calculativeness, trust, and economic organiza-
tion. Journal of Law and Economics, 36, 453– 486.
Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. E. (1985). Telling lies: Verbal and nonverbal
correlates of deception. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Mul-
tichannel integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 129 –148). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Received October 4, 2001
Revision received April 22, 2002
Accepted April 22, 2002
䡲
Appendix
Post-Negotiation Trust Questionnaire
Please circle the number to the right of each statement that most clearly describes your opinion of members of
the other party.
Strongly
disagree
Disagree
Slightly
disagree
Neither agree
nor disagree
Slightly
agree
Agree
Strongly
agree
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
1
I think the other party told the truth in the
negotiation.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
2
I think that the other party met its
negotiated obligations.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
3
In my opinion, the other party is reliable.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
4
I think that the other party succeeds by
stepping on other people.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
5
I feel that the other party tries to get the
upper hand.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
6
I think that the other party took advantage
of my problems.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
7
I feel that the other party negotiated with
me honestly.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
I feel that the other party will keep its word.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
9
I think the other party has not misled me.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
10
I feel that the other party tries to get out of
its commitments.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
11
I feel that the other party negotiated joint
expectations fairly.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
12
I feel that the other party takes advantage of
people who are vulnerable.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Note.
Items 4, 5, 6, 10, and 12 were reverse scored.
120
NAQUIN AND PAULSON