Stages of Language development- Małgorzata Szulc-Kurpaska
Biological foundations of language
Eric Lenneberg 1967
The Biological Foundations of Language
The behaviour emerges before it is necessary.
(The law of anticipatory maturation)
Its appearance is not the result of a conscious decision.
Its emergence is not triggered by external events (though the surrounding environment must be sufficiently `rich' for it to develop adequately).
There is likely to be a `critical period' for the acquisition of behaviour.
Direct teaching and practice have relatively little effect.
Child: Want other one spoon daddy.
Father: You mean, you want the other spoon.
Child: Yes, I want other one spoon, please daddy.
Father: Can you say `the other spoon'?
Child: Other...one...spoon.
Father: Say `other'.
Child: Other
Father: `spoon'
Child: spoon.
Father: `other spoon'.
Child: other...spoon. Now give me other one spoon?
There is a regular sequence of `milestones' as the behaviour develops, and these can usually be correlated with age and other aspects of development.
Stages of language development
The incremental nature of first language development
The length of the utterances increases step by step
The grammatical complexity gradually increases
Caretaker speech
Motherese, Fatherese, Parentese
Two functions:
To communicate with the child
To direct child's behaviour
Features of caretaker speech:
higher pitch
explicit intonation
careful articulation
slow pace
using gestures and facial expressions
simplified vocabulary
diminutives, 'baby talk', onomatopoeic words
short utterances
simple sentences
grammatical correctness
topic referring to the 'here and now'
general questions and imperatives
attention getters
repetitions
paraphrases
comprehension checks
expansions
redundancy
Stages of language development
|
Birth 6 weeks 6 months 8 months 1 year 18 months 2 years 2 and ¼ years 5 years 10 years |
Babbling drift
a child's babbling gradually moves in the direction of the sounds he hears around him
One-word utterances
Four theories
the child has overgeneralised the word `ba'; that is the child has learned the name `ba' for `bath' and has wrongly assumed that it can apply to anything which contains liquid
Russian psychologist Lev Vygotski (1893-1934) suggested that when children over-generalise they do so in a quite confusing way - they appear to focus attention on one aspect of an object at a time
according to David McNeill, the child is uttering holophrases - single words that stand for whole sentences
Lois Bloom put forward another hypothesis - the meaning of a one-word utterance varies according to the age of the child
Two-word utterances
Roger Brown
Adam, Eve and Sarah
|
Eve |
Adam |
Two words |
20 months |
26 months |
Three words |
22 months |
36 months |
Four words |
28 months |
42 months |
Telegraphic speech (18-28 months)
One explanation (Martin Braine)
Pivot grammar
a small number of words such as allgone, this, more, no which occurred frequently, never alone and in a fixed position- pivots, because the utterance appears to pivot round them;
many more words which occurred less frequently but in any position and sometimes alone (often nouns), e.g. open class words, since an `open' class is a set of words which can be added to indefinitely;
Relations
Mummy push
Agent action
Eat dinner
Action and object
Mummy pigtail
Agent and object
Play garden
Action location
Cookie plate
Entity and location
Mummy scarf
Possessor possession
Green car
Attributive and entity
That butterfly
Demonstrative and entity
Operations
This (is a) truck
Nomination
More milk
Recurrence
Allgone milk
Non-existence
Grammatical morphemes
In the acquisition of grammatical morphemes:
-ing progressive and -s plural are early acquired
past irregular is acquired earlier than past regular
copula `be' is acquired earlier than auxiliary `be'
uncontractible `be' is acquired earlier than contractible `be'
3rd person present tense is late acquired
Why is the plural -s early early acquired and the third person singular present tense -s late acquired?
Because plural `s is more frequent than third person singular present tense.
Overgeneralisation - the use of a grammatical form in all contexts even if it does not apply, e.g. childrens, wented, goed.
A grammatical morpheme is acquired when it is used in 90% of obligatory occasions over w two week period
Obligatory occasion instances where an adult native speaker would use a particular grammatical morpheme
Bilingual Syntax Measure an instrument for collecting data on the acquisition of grammatical morphemes (it contains a series of pictures to which questions are asked)
Question formation
Intonation questions?
After the second birthday, a child places the wh-word in front of the rest of the sentence
What Mummy doing? (FRONTING)
A second stage 3-4 months later the child adds an auxiliary verb such as can or will to the main verb
Where you will go? (FRONTING)
Before the age of 3, the child realises that the subject noun must change places with the auxiliary
Where will you go? (INVERSION)
Negation formation
`Put no, or not in front of the whole sentence'
No play that (EXTERNAL NEGATION)
`Put the negative after the first noun phrase and before the rest of the sentence' (At this stage can't and don't seemed to be treated as alternatives to no)
He no bite you (INTERNAL NEGATION I)
The negative was placed in the third slot in the sentence, after the noun and auxiliary and copula and before the rest of the sentence.
Paul can't have one (INTERNAL NEGATION II)