Psychoticism and Creativity: A Meta-analytic Review
Selcuk Acar and Mark A. Runco
University of Georgia
Quite a few studies have examined the association of creativity with psychoticism. The present
article reports a meta-analysis that was intended to clarify the strength of the association and to
explain variation in effects sizes reported in 32 previous studies. These 32 studies involved 6,771
participants, most of them college students. Results indicated that the effect sizes were heteroge-
neous, but the overall mean effect size was small (r
⫽ .16, k ⫽ 119, 95% CI [.12, .20]). Of most
importance was that the analyses examining 8 moderators (gender, age, the type of sample, the
particular measure of creativity, the content of the particular creativity test, the index of creativity,
the particular measure of psychoticism, and the domain of creativity) and 2 interaction terms
(Creativity Measure
⫻ Measure of Psychoticism and Creativity Measure ⫻ Content of Creativity
Test) showed that the relationship between creativity and psychoticism is large (r
⫽ .50, 95% CI
[.39, .60]) but only when psychoticism is measured by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire and
uniqueness is the index of creativity. Results are discussed with respect to their theoretical
implications.
Keywords: creativity, psychoticism, meta-analysis, EPQ, uniqueness
Creativity has long been associated with psychopathology. Ar-
istotle’s statement “No great genius has ever been without some
madness” (and the concise label mad genius) is still debated in the
literature. The relationship between creativity and psychopathol-
ogy is difficult to pinpoint, in large part because creativity and
psychopathology are both complex concepts. Also, the relationship
is probably bidirectional, with creativity sometimes influencing
psychopathology and psychopathology sometimes influencing cre-
ativity (Runco, 1991). This makes causal explanations quite chal-
lenging. It is not surprising, then, in their review of studies on the
relationship between creativity and psychopathology, that Lau-
ronen et al. (2004) found multiple links. They concluded that there
was a “fragile association between creativity and mental disorder,
but the link is not apparent for all groups of mental disorders or for
all forms of creativity” (p. 81). This echoes the conclusion of
Richards (2000 –2001) in her overview of research on this topic.
Clearly, it is necessary to be very specific in any examination of
creativity and psychopathology. With that in mind, the focus of the
present article should be explicitly stated. This article is focused on
psychoticism. It is unique in its use of meta-analytic procedures
used to explore it and its relationship with creativity.
H. J. Eysenck (1995) defined psychoticism (P) as “a disposi-
tional variable or trait predisposing people to functional psychotic
disorders of all types” (p. 203). The concept of psychoticism was
derived from psychosis, which is the common label for some
mental disorders, including schizophrenia and manic depressive
proclivities. Psychoticism might be considered “subclinical,” just
as some schizoaffective tendencies are clearly distinct from
schizophrenia (and therefore not psychopathological; Sass, 2000 –
2001; Schuldberg, 2000 –2001). This, of course, makes the possi-
ble bridge to creative performance easier to understand, given that
creative performance must be effective (Runco, 1988; Runco,
Jaeger, & Cramond, in press). Unambiguous psychopathology
would make it difficult for the person to be effective, at least in the
sense of adaptive action. To simplify, P involves some character-
istics of psychotic disorders, but high P scores do not necessarily
mean a psychotic disorder (H. J. Eysenck, 1995).
H. J. Eysenck (1995) portrayed P as a continuum that has
altruistic, socialized, empathic, conventional, and conformist
personality traits on one extreme (low psychoticism; P–); and
criminal, impulsive, hostile, aggressive, psychopathic, schizoid,
unipolar depressive, affective disorders, schizoaffective, and
schizophrenic traits on other extreme (high psychoticism; P
⫹).
P
⫹ scorers tend to have difficulty attending or with vigilance.
They may be noncooperative but are often highly original in
word association tests. This last point is also useful when
examining a bridge with creativity. MacKinnon (1962) and
Cross, Cattell, and Butcher (1967), as well as H. J. Eysenck
(1997, 2003), suggested that the study of psychoticism can
contribute much to the understanding of creativity.
One attraction of this line of work is that it bridges personality
with the cognitive underpinnings of creative thinking. As H. J.
Eysenck (2003) described it, some people have loose and very
wide associative networks, which allow divergent thinking and the
discovery of remote and highly original ideas. This is overinclusive
thinking and is, for Eysenck, rooted in personality. Open-ended
tests reveal this characteristic because they allow people with
wider associative horizons to produce novel, original, and unusual
responses. Overinclusive tendencies underlie both creativity and
This article was published Online First February 27, 2012.
Selcuk Acar and Mark A. Runco, Torrance Creativity Center, University
of Georgia, Athens.
We thank Rod Dishman and Nur Cayirdag for their contributions and
valuable comments on this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Selcuk
Acar, 350 Aderhold Hall, Torrance Creativity Center, University of Geor-
gia, Athens, GA 30602. E-mail: acarse@uga.edu
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts
© 2012 American Psychological Association
2012, Vol. 6, No. 4, 341–350
1931-3896/12/$12.00
DOI: 10.1037/a0027497
341
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
psychotic disorders. In fact, H. J. H. J. Eysenck (2003) proposed
that this is precisely why so many unambiguously creative persons
have been diagnosed with psychopathology.
H. J. Eysenck (1994) empirically tested his theory by adminis-
tering the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire—Revised ([EPQ–R]
which involves the P, Neuroticism [N], Extraversion, and Lie
scales); tests of impulsiveness (I), empathy (Em), and venture-
someness (V); Word Association Rare Response Test (WARRT);
and Barron–Welsh Art Scale (B-W). Bivariate correlations be-
tween P and creativity measures were
⫺.25 (p ⬍ .01), .27 (p ⬍
.01), and .17 (ns) for usual, unique, and rare responses from the
WAT, and .16 (ns) with the total score from the B-W. Those were
lower than coefficients reported in the earlier study by Woody and
Claridge (1977), who found correlations ranging between .32 and
.68 ( p
⫽ .001). Multidimensional scaling analysis indicated that
creativity measures (P, I, V, WAT rare, WAT unique, and B-W)
constituted a cluster different from another cluster consisting of
Em, WAT common, and N.
Kline and Cooper (1986) used scales of flexibility of closure,
spontaneous flexibility, ideational fluency, word fluency, and orig-
inality from the Comprehensive Ability Battery (Hakstian & Cat-
tell, 1976) as creativity measures to test their relationship with P
from the EPQ (H. J. Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975). Analyses were
conducted separately for men and women. The highest and the
only significant correlation was with word fluency (r
⫽ .20)
among men. All other correlations were lower and not statistically
significant.
Barron (1993) also suspected a relationship between creativity
and psychoticism. In fact, he found that creative individuals had
high scores on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
Schizophrenia scale, compared with the general population (Bar-
ron, 1963/1990). Evidence regarding high ego strength in creative
individuals, along with a negative correlation between ego strength
and schizophrenia in the general population, convinced him to
argue that weird thoughts become defensibly original because ego
strength allows the individual to sort out inconvenient thoughts.
That is what he called controlled weirdness. Fodor (1995) reported
more recent evidence supporting a similar view. He found that
people with psychosis proneness and high ego strength were more
creative than those with psychosis proneness with low ego
strength, high ego strength without psychosis proneness, and low
ego strength without psychosis proneness. This finding held across
various measures of creativity.
Other findings raised some doubts about the relation of P to
creativity. Csikszentmihalyi (1993) argued that psychoticism does
not say much about creativity and the relationship between the two
is weak: “How significant is it, for instance, that less than 3% of
the variance in a divergent-thinking test filled out by university
students is in common with the variance in their scores on psy-
choticism?” (p. 189). On the other hand, Martindale (1993) ac-
cepted H. J. Eysenck’s (1993) theory because he felt that the
relationship between creativity and psychoticism explains swings
of physiological arousal in creative people.
Mixed results about the relationship between creativity and
psychoticism suggest that it would be beneficial to look more
closely. A meta-analysis of studies reporting the relationship be-
tween creativity and psychoticism could help to resolve the con-
troversy. Although psychoticism represents a broader conceptual-
ization than the P scale of H. J. Eysenck (1995), there were several
reasons to focus the meta-analysis on the P scale. First, the inclu-
sion of schizotypy would have complicated analyses because of
additional heterogeneity that hinders meaningful interpretation.
Besides, studies have shown that schizotypal scales involve quite
a bit of neuroticism (for a review, see H. J. Eysenck, 1993), which
could be the reason for loading on different factors than P in factor
analytic studies (e.g., White, Joseph, & Neil, 1995). In addition,
there are enough studies (more than 30) specifically examining the
relationship between the P scale and creativity measures. Prece-
dents for meta-analyses with 30 – 40 studies are easy to find (e.g.,
Bennett & Gibbons, 2000; Kim, 2008). Taken together, these
reasons supported the decision to conduct a meta-analysis that
focused entirely on P and creativity.
A meta-analysis on creativity and psychoticism could also ex-
amine specific factors to determine whether they lead to the
aforementioned mixed results. One such factor reflects the variety
of definitions and measures of creativity used in previous studies.
Large effect sizes would be expected from research using word
association and divergent thinking tests, at least if H. J. Eysenck
(1993, 2003) was correct in his suggestion that high psychoticism
is related to higher performance in overinclusive thinking. Word
association and divergent thinking tasks are open-ended and as
such allow this sort of associational processes (Glazer, 2009).
The nature of the test content, as verbal or figural, may be
relevant. Guilford (1968) reported that verbal (symbolic and se-
mantic) divergent thinking was related to higher IQ than figural
tests. Runco (1986) found that verbal and figural stimuli produce
qualitatively and quantitatively different outcomes. He raised the
possibility that each elicits a unique ideational and associative
process. Runco (1993) also noted the difference between ideations
generated from verbal and figural tasks in that the former tend to
be more “rote and preconceived” and the latter more “effortful and
spontaneous” (Runco, 1986, p. 351). Also, verbal measures could
be more subject to experiential bias (Runco & Acar, 2010). At-
tempts to enhance creativity, as measured by tests of divergent
thinking, at least sometimes lead to improvements in verbal indices
but not figural (Kauffman & Rich, 2010). Richardson (1986) used
these kinds of findings to postulate a two-factor theory of creativ-
ity. One factor is verbal, the other figural.
H. J. Eysenck and Eysenck (1976) examined the specific scores
from tests of divergent thinking and concluded that psychoticism is
more related to originality than fluency and flexibility. This fits
with later findings that reported positive correlation between psy-
choticism and uniqueness scores in word association tests (H. J.
Eysenck, 1993).
Domain differences in creative performance (Baer, 1998;
Plucker, 1998; Runco, 1987) may also be relevant to the
creativity–psychoticism relationship. Sass (2000 –2001), Ludwig
(1992), and Post (1994) all found that incidences of psychopathol-
ogy varied according to domains. Post, for example, found that
psychopathologies are more common among creative artists than
among creative scientists. For this reason, the domains of science,
writing, and arts, and relationships with psychoticism were exam-
ined in the meta-analysis.
The relationship between creativity and psychoticism may differ
in males and females, given that male gender is positively corre-
lated with P (H. J. Eysenck, 1993; H. J. Eysenck & Eysenck,
1976). Certainly, differences could also be attributed to social
factors that influence creative efforts and achievements (Csik-
342
ACAR AND RUNCO
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
szentmihalyi, 1993; Runco, Cramond, & Pagnani, 2010). With this
in mind, the meta-analyses, reported below, took into account sex,
as well as age.
The relationship of psychopathology and creativity may be most
obvious in studies with eminent individuals (Andreasen & Powers,
1974, 1975; Ludwig, 1994). Perhaps the presence of psychoticism
itself has an impact on the salience that attracts fame and earns
eminence. A related possibility is that fame leads to the collection
of detailed information (e.g., biographies and autobiographies),
which in turn allows easier interpretation of problems or psycho-
pathology. Consider as one example Isaac Newton. He was ru-
mored to have autism, but this rumor only started after autism was
defined (in the 1940s) and recognized as a legitimate diagnostic
category for psychologists and psychiatrists. Once autism was
defined, biographers could look back at Newton’s idiosyncrasies,
which were readily available. But they were only available because
Newton was famous and a great deal had been written about him.
Ludwig’s (1995) study of 1,000 eminent people indicated that
increased eminence is related to higher incidences of psychopa-
thology. Richards (1981) found higher levels of psychopathology
among eminent creators than among laypeople. Gotz and Gotz
(1979b) compared highly and less successful artists and found that
there was higher P in the former group but no difference in
originality. Bachtold (1980) studied 18 eminently creative artists
and scientists and concluded that the relationship between creativ-
ity and psychoticism is more related to assertiveness and success
rather than the creative process. This variation as a function of
eminence could apply specifically to psychoticism. Level of
achievement was therefore tested in the following meta-analysis.
Last but not least, different versions of the P scale could lead to
variation. The P scale (H. J. Eysenck & Eysenck, 1976) was
designed to measure psychoticism as a personality trait that is
believed to exist in varying degrees in psychotic as well as non-
psychotic groups. Factor analytic methods were employed in its
construction and revision. Its main use is in nonpsychotic groups;
thus, it is focused on personality traits rather than symptoms. Still,
it has been used a criterion against psychiatric diagnoses. All 25
items are worded as “yes” or “no” questions (e.g., “Would being
in debt worry you?”; “Do most things taste the same to you?”; “Do
you try not to be rude to people?”). After the release of the P scale
as a component of the EPQ, a new version (S. B. G. Eysenck,
Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985) was developed to improve the psycho-
metric quality of the scale. There were three problems with the
initial form: low reliability, low range of scoring, and highly
skewed distribution of scores. After addition and removal of sev-
eral items, the revised P consisted of 32 items. The correlation of
the new and old P was .88 and .81 for male and female groups,
respectively. When compared with the old form, the revised P had
slightly higher reliability (.78 and .76 against .74 to .68 for males
and females, respectively), a remarkably larger range of scores,
and moderately improved skewness. The measure of psychoticism
was included in this study because of those significant differences
between the two forms.
In sum, based on the theoretical and prior empirical data from
previous investigations, we addressed the following questions:
1.
How strong is the relationship between creativity and
psychoticism?
2.
Are there domain-based differences in the relationship
between creativity and psychoticism?
3.
Can the strength of relationship between creativity and
psychoticism be explained by the type of creativity tests
used in the studies? More specifically, do divergent
thinking and word association tests yield higher effect
sizes than the other tests?
4.
Does the figural or verbal content of the creativity tests
make a difference in terms of the relationship between
creativity and psychoticism?
5.
To what degree do different indices of creativity explain
the variability in the effect sizes? More specifically, does
the number of unique responses yield higher effect sizes
than the others?
6.
Does the particular measure of psychoticism explain the
variability of effect sizes?
7.
Do eminence, age, and gender explain the strength of the
relationship between creativity and psychoticism?
Method
Literature Search
Studies published in 1975 through 2010 were located through an
extensive literature search in English. The year 1975 was chosen as
the starting year because use of the P scale became possible after
the publication of the EPQ (H. J. Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975). The
literature search was mainly conducted through keywords, which
consisted of creativity and psychoticism. Those keywords were
entered to electronic databases including Google Scholar, Psycho-
logical Abstracts Index, Science Direct and Web of Science, and
Educational Resources Information Center search engines. Sec-
ond, the reference lists and bibliographies of the articles found
through electronic search were reviewed.
The decisions for inclusion and exclusion of the studies were
made based on the research questions listed above. Figure 1
summarizes this process. The following criteria were followed for
the studies to be included in or excluded from analyses:
Figure 1.
Flow chart for selection of studies.
343
PSYCHOTICISM AND CREATIVITY
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
1.
Publication: Only empirical articles that were published
in peer-reviewed journals were included. We did not
include dissertations, conference presentations, or other
research reports because they usually do not go through
a strict review process as do the journal articles.
2.
Sufficient statistics reported: Studies that did not report
basic statistics such as Pearson r, means, standard devi-
ations, t values, F values, or p values were not included
because they did not allow calculation of an effect size.
3.
Control group: Comparative studies reporting psychoti-
cism or creativity scores without any score of an appro-
priate control group were not included because those
scores were not comparable.
4.
Language: Only studies published in English were in-
cluded.
5.
Reanalysis: Studies that used the same data only once
were included. The preference was made on the basis of
completeness of the reporting.
Study Characteristics and Calculation of Effect Sizes
Thirty-two empirical studies representing 6,771 participants
allowed the calculation of 119 effect sizes. Most of the studies
yielded multiple effect sizes (the number of effect sizes per
article ranged from one to 14, with a mean of 3.75 and median
of 3). Most of the studies included samples of college students
in undergraduate and graduate programs. Given the age distri-
bution, we set a cutoff age at 30 years. Most studies used mixed
groups and reported the results together, but some studies
reported the results specific to gender, and some studies included
only a particular gender. Other studies were mixed but the sample was
dominated by a gender (above 75%). Such mixed studies were col-
lapsed to the dominant gender in the sample. To test our hypotheses
regarding different indices of creativity (which yielded multiple effect
sizes), we did not collapse multiple effect sizes from the same study.
Pearson r was selected as the effect size index because of the
nature of the research questions. Studies had different designs
but were mostly correlational and most of the original effect
sizes (n
⫽ 103) were reported as Pearson rs in bivariate corre-
lation tables. Other studies were comparative (P in high creative
vs. low creative; P in creative vs. control; creativity in high vs.
low psychoticism). Those studies reported means and standard
deviations that allowed calculating of F or t values. Some
provided their own t values. Each of these was converted to r
using the formula r
⫽ sqrt[t
2
/(t
2
⫹ df)]. When standard devia-
tions were not reported (i.e., Aguilar-Alonso, 1996), we calcu-
lated effect sizes from the exact p values, as suggested by
Rosenthal (1994).
After the effect sizes were retrieved and calculated, a grad-
uate student in educational psychology examined the data set to
ensure accuracy of the individual effect sizes and the codes for
the moderators. There was agreement of 97% across effect sizes
and codes. What few discrepancies existed were resolved by
logic.
Statistical Analysis
Because Pearson rs were not normally distributed, we trans-
formed them to Fisher’s Zr for the analyses. Effect size values
reported in the results were back-transformed to Pearson r. To give
more weight to studies with larger sample sizes, we weighted
individual correlation coefficients by the inverse variance weight
of (N – 3) associated with Zr values. A macro (SPSS 13.0) was
used to calculate the aggregated mean r effect size, the confidence
interval (95% CI), and the sampling error variance on the basis of
a random effects model. Random effects were employed to explain
both study-level sampling error and random sources of variability
(Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 119). In this model, effects sizes were
weighted by the inverse of variance of each size. This yielded a
new estimated effect size value with the addition of a random
effects component (Hedges & Olkin, 1985).
The reliable interpretation of mean effect sizes is possible if
the data set is homogeneous. Heterogeneity was therefore tested
in the random effects model. The Q
T
value—representing the
sum of squares of each effect size that is related to the mean
weighted effect size—indicates heterogeneity if it is significant
at the p
⫽ .05 level. Sampling error accounting for less than
75% of the observed variance was used here as the indicator of
heterogeneity (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Hunter, Schmidt, &
Jackson, 1982). Fail-safe N sample size (Rosenberg, 2005) was
also calculated. It indicates the estimated number of studies
with zero effect size that needs to be added to alter the results.
Moderators
Variability in the effect sizes that is not explained by sampling
error can be attributed to particular features of the study or to
moderators. For this reason, we prepared an iterative coding
scheme. There were eight moderators: domain, the measure of
creativity, the type (or content) of the creativity test, the index of
the creativity measure, the scale for or measure of psychoticism,
the sample characteristics, gender, and age. The definitions of the
moderators are provided in Table 1.
Analyses of Moderators
Each of eight moderators was coded with contrasts weights at
the .05 level (see Table 2). Two interaction terms (Measure of
Creativity
⫻ Index of Creativity Test and the Content of the
Creativity Test
⫻ the Index of Creativity Test) were entered to
weighted least squares regression analyses to determine inde-
pendent effects of each variable. For this analysis, we used a
macro (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, pp. 216 –220) that runs within
SPSS. Maximum likelihood estimation was specified in the
mixed effects multiple linear regression analysis after adjusting
for nonindependence of the multiple effect sizes from a single
study. Tests of the regression model and contingent residual
value were requested. Last, number of effects (k), mean effect
sizes (r), 95% CIs, and p values for the planned contrasts were
provided by using another macro, from Lipsey and Wilson
(2001, pp. 209 –212; see Table 2).
Results
The first analysis examined the distribution of the effect sizes
that were based on Pearson r values (see Figure 2). The distri-
344
ACAR AND RUNCO
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
Table 1
Definitions and Scope of Moderators
Moderator
Definition/test
Gender
Male
Samples with male groups above 75%
Female
Samples with female groups above 75%
Mixed
Samples with both males and females
Age (years)
Above 30
Samples with age above 30
Below 30
Samples with age below 30
Eminence
Eminent
Studies with eminent or highly successful individuals in a field of creativity
Noneminent
Studies with normal individuals
Measure of creativity
Divergent thinking (DT)
DT (Wallach & Kogan, 1965); Torrance Test of Creative Thinking, (Torrance,
1974, 2008); Inventiveness subscale of the Berlin Intelligence Structure Test
(Ja¨ger, Su¨ß, & Beauducel, 1997); House–Tree–Person Projective Drawing
Technique (H-T-P;Buck, 1992); Word Fluency (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1972);
Letter Fluency (Lezak, 1995), Thurstone Written Fluency Test, (Thurstone &
Thurstone, 1962); Alternate Uses (Guilford, Christensen, Merrifield, &
Wilson, 1978); and Utility Test (Wilson, Merrifield, & Guildford, 1969)
Word association tests
Word Association Rare Response Test (Kent-Rosanoff, 1910; Palermo &
Jenkins, 1964); Word Halo Test (Armstrong & McConaghy, 1977); AMT
(Merten & Fischer, 1999)
Barron–Welsh (B-W)
B-W (Barron & Welsh, 1987; Welsh & Barron, 1963); Polygons (Vanderplas
and Garvin, 1959); Welsh Figure Preference Test (Welsh, 1949); and
origence test (Rawlings & Georgiou, 2004)
Creative personality
Adjective Checklist or Creative Personality Scale (ACL/CPS; Gough, 1979)
Other
Remote Associates Test (Mednick, 1962); Bridge-the-Associative-Gap
(Gianotti, Mohr, Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger, 2001); conceptual
expansion (T. B. Ward, 1994); constraint of examples (Smith, Ward, &
Schumacher, 1993); creative imagery (Finke, 1990); How Do You Think
(Davis & Subkoviak, 1975); insight problems (Karimi, Windmann,
Gu¨ntu¨rku¨n, & Abraham, 2007); number of publications (Rushton, 1990); and
performance based (writing a story or poem; Martindale & Dailey, 1996)
Measure of psychoticism
P scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ)
H. J. Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975
P scale of the Eysenck Personality
Questionnaire—Revised (EPQ–R)
S. B. G.Eysenck et al., 1985; H. J. Eysenck & Eysenck, 1991
Symptom Checklist–90 (SCL-90)
Derogatis, Lipman, & Covi, 1973
Other
66-item anonymous survey (Rushton, 1990)
Content of creativity test
Verbal
All tests with verbal content, including DT, WAT, CPS/ACL, and other tests
Figural
All tests with figural content, including DT, B-W, and other tests
Both
Composite of both verbal and figural scores
Numerical
Items related to numbers in the Inventiveness scale of the Berlin Intelligence
Structure Test
None
Studies using no measures or number of publications as a measure of creativity
Indices of creativity
Fluency
Number of total responses or total score
Flexibility
Number of categories
Infrequency
Number of infrequent ideas including unique scores
Uniqueness
Number unique scores only
Practicality
Practical value of ideas
Other
Studies reporting no particular index of creativity
Domains
Arts
Participants were artists or measurement of creativity relied on artistic skills
(H-T-P, Buck, 1992; B-W, Welsh & Barron, 1963; Polygons, Vanderplas &
Garvin, 1959); Welsh Figure Preference Test (Barron & Welsh, 1987;
Welsh, 1949); and origence test (Rawlings & Georgiou, 2004)
Science
Measurement was scientific based (number of publications)
Writing
Participants were artists or measurement of creativity relied on writing skills
(Creative Writing—Poetry Measure; Joy, 2008)
General
General population or usual measures of creativity
Note.
DT
⫽ divergent thinking; WAT ⫽ Word Association Test; AMT ⫽ multiple-choice association test.
345
PSYCHOTICISM AND CREATIVITY
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
bution of effects was slightly skewed positively (g1
⫽ 228) and
leptokurtic (g2
⫽ 1.37). The fail-safe sample size was (N⫹)
8,452. The mean effect size (r) was .16 (k
⫽ 119, 95% CI [.12,
.20], p
⬍ .00001). Because of its heterogeneity, QT(118) ⫽
625.67, the mean effect size was not representative of the
studies included. The more meaningful analyses, therefore, are
probably those focused on the moderators (below).
The overall multiple regression model explained a significant
amount of the variance of the effect sizes, Q
R(11)
⫽ 89.36, p ⬍
.0001, R
2
⫽ .46; Q
E(98)
⫽ 104.59, p ⫽ .31. This model indicated
that the type of sample (
 ⫽ .22, z ⫽ 2.77, p ⫽ .006), the measure
of psychoticism (
 ⫽ .29, z ⫽ 3.35, p ⫽ .001), and the interaction
of Measure of Psychoticism
⫻ Index of Creativity Test ( ⫽ .58,
z
⫽ 4.48, p ⬍ .001) were independently related to effect size.
Planned comparisons indicated that studies consisting of eminent or
highly successful individuals reported smaller effect sizes (r
⫽ ⫺.04,
95% CI [
⫺.32, .24]) than those with only unexceptional participants
(r
⫽ .17, 95% CI [.13, .21]). In addition, studies measuring psychoti-
cism with the EPQ had a larger mean effect size (r
⫽ .20, 95% CI
[.15, .26]) than those using the EPQ–R (r
⫽ 11, 95% CI [.08, .15]) or
the Symptom Checklist–90 ([SCL-90] r
⫽ ⫺.44, 95% CI [⫺.55,
⫺.31]). Cell comparisons of the interaction of Measure of Psychoti-
cism
⫻ Index of Creativity Test indicated that the largest effect sizes
were reported in studies that used the EPQ as the measure of psy-
choticism and uniqueness as the index of creativity measure (r
⫽ .50.
95% CI [.39, .60]; see Figure 3). The other three cells each had mean
effect sizes slightly above .10.
All effect sizes for each level of the moderators are provided in
the Table 2 along with the number of effect sizes (k), the mean r
effect size values, the 95% CIs, and the p values for planned
comparisons. The descriptive results of the overall regression
model are also presented in the Table 3.
Table 2
Number of Studies and Categories and Univariate Analyses of Moderators
Moderator
Contrast weight
k
Mean r
95% CI
p
Gender
Male
⫺1
16
.13
[.05, .20]
.002
Female
.5
21
.17
[.11, .22]
⬍.001
Mixed
.5
82
.17
[.11, .22]
⬍.001
Age (years)
Below 30
1
93
.18
[.13, .22]
⬍.001
Above 30
⫺1
14
.05
[
⫺.07, .16]
.46
Not reported
0
12
.15
[.07, .23]
.001
Eminence
Eminent
⫺1
5
⫺.04
[
⫺.32, .24]
.78
Noneminent
1
114
.17
[.13, .21]
⬍.001
Measure of creativity
DT
1/2
70
.18
[.12, .24]
⬍.001
WAT
1/2
10
.22
[.14, .31]
⬍.001
B-W
⫺1/3
11
.11
[.03, .18]
.006
CPS
⫺1/3
4
.03
[
⫺.02, .08]
.27
Other measures
⫺1/3
20
.12
[.02, .21]
.02
Not used
—
4
.18
[.10, .25]
⬍.001
Type of creativity test
Verbal
0
71
.16
[.11, .22]
⬍.001
Figural
⫺1
37
.15
[.08, .22]
.001
Numerical
—
1
Both
—
2
.22
[.00, .43]
.05
None
1
8
.18
[.09, .27]
.001
Indices of creativity
Fluency
⫺1/4
34
.13
[.06, .21]
.001
Flexibility
⫺1/4
7
.00
[
⫺.11, .11]
.99
Infrequency
⫺1/4
17
.16
[.10, .22]
⬍.001
Uniqueness
1
18
.40
[.27, .51]
⬍.001
Practicality
—
1
Other
⫺1/4
42
.11
[.06, .16]
.001
Domains
Arts
⫺1/2
17
.14
[.08, .19]
⬍.001
Science
1
2
.36
[.19, .51]
.001
Writing
⫺1/2
10
.17
[.03, .30]
.02
General
0
90
.16
[.11, .21]
⬍.001
Measure of P
EPQ
1
67
.20
[.15, .26]
⬍.001
EPQ–R
0
49
.11
[.08, .15]
⬍.001
SCL-90
⫺1
2
⫺.44
[
⫺.55,⫺.31]
⬍.001
Other
—
Note.
DT
⫽ divergent thinking; WAT ⫽ Word Association Rare Response Test; B-W ⫽ Barron–Welsh
Art Scale; CPS
⫽ Creative Personality Scale; P ⫽ psychoticism; EPQ ⫽ Eysenck Personality Question-
naire; EPQ–R
⫽ Eysenck Personality Questionnaire—Revised; SCL-90 ⫽ Symptom Checklist–90.
346
ACAR AND RUNCO
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
Discussion
The primary research question concerned the strength of the
relationship between creativity and psychoticism (Question 1 in
the introduction). This meta-analysis indicated that the effect size
can be as large as .50 (which is a large effect size by Cohen’s,
1988, oft-cited standards) when the EPQ is the measure of psy-
choticism and uniqueness is the index of creativity, but it ap-
proaches .00 with certain samples, measures, or indices of creativ-
ity. In most of cells in the table summarizing the univariate
analyses (see Table 2), the mean effect sizes are between .10 and
.20, which are regarded as a small effect (Cohen, 1988). Therefore,
the relationship between creativity and psychoticism is strong only
when the EPQ and uniqueness are involved. Relating this back to
the mad genius hypothesis, it appears that creativity and psycho-
pathology may have only an occasional and very specific relation-
ship rather than a broad and general one. As Simonton (2005)
suggested, the idea of a mad genius is probably often exaggerated.
The highest correlations with unique scores are consistent with
H. J. Eysenck’s (1993) expectations. Uniqueness is one way of
defining originality, and Eysenck argued that psychoticism is more
related to originality than fluency. It is important to note that
uniqueness and originality are not synonymous with creativity.
Creative ideas or products are original and sometimes unique, but
not all original ideas are really creative (H. J. Eysenck, 1993;
Runco, 2004; Runco et al., in press).
The variation among the psychoticism measures is intriguing.
The P scale of the earlier form, the EPQ, had a higher correlation
with the creativity measures than the revised form, the EPQ–R.
The revised P scale was intended to improve the psychometric
shortcomings of the EPQ, one of which was a skewed distribution
(S. B. G. Eysenck et al., 1985). The revised form included 13 new
items and excluded six items from the old form. Although the new
scale was more reliable and less skewed, it was still nonnormally
distributed. H. J. Eysenck (1992) mentioned the possibility that
skewness could be “inherent in the ‘true’ distribution of P” (p. 757)
rather than a psychometric deficiency. Given his theory linking
psychoticism with creativity, the earlier form is more defensible
than the revised form, at least with the assumption that P has a
naturally skewed distribution.
The specific measure of psychoticism used in the original stud-
ies (which we examined to answer Question 6) had an independent
effect (
 ⫽ .29, p ⫽ .001) in addition to being part of the
interaction effect. The interaction term compared the EPQ with the
EPQ–R, but the independent effect involved all three measures,
including the SCL-90. The SCL-90 was originally developed for
the “measurement of psychopathology in psychiatric and medical
outpatients” (Derogatis, Rickels, & Rock, 1976, p. 280). The items
in the Psychoticim subscale of SCL-90 include some symptoms of
schizophrenia and describe a withdrawn, isolated, even schizoid
Figure 2.
Stem-and-leaf plot of the r values.
Figure 3.
Effect size r values in four interaction groups.
347
PSYCHOTICISM AND CREATIVITY
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
lifestyle. As expected, it had a strong negative effect size (r
⫽
⫺.44) because it is a diagnostic tool rather than a personality-based
instrument, such as the P scale. In spite of the few effect sizes
available, this finding is potentially important in that it suggests
that psychoticism becomes counterproductive when it reaches the
level of mental illness. Several empirical studies have supported
this argument (Kinney et al., 2000 –2001; Schuldberg, 1990,
2000 –2001). This is entirely consistent with H. J. Eysenck’s
(1997) theory that psychoticism and psychosis represent one con-
tinuum and that only the former is subclinical and supports over-
inclusive thinking in a manner that is beneficial to creative per-
formances. It is also interesting to speculate about a parallel
between this finding and the inverted-U relationships that seems to
characterize the benefits of various kinds of asynchrony for cre-
ativity (Acar, 2011).
The mean effect sizes did not show differences between males
and females, nor between older and younger participants. There-
fore, they did not have independent effects. But there was an
independent effect for the type of sample (eminent or noneminent;
 ⫽ .22, p ⫽ .01). The mean effect size values were ⫺.04 for the
eminent group and .17 for the noneminent. This could be explained
by the possibility that noneminent people can express their “so-
cially unacceptable” behaviors listed in the P scale more comfort-
ably than eminent people.
None of other moderators had independent effects. It is inter-
esting that, among the creativity measures, the Creative Personal-
ity Scale had the lowest correlation with psychoticism, even
though both P and the Creative Personality Scale are personality-
based assessments. It might be useful to follow up on this using
another personality-based measure of creativity. Indeed, there
were limitations to this study. Some levels in the comparison group
had only a few effect sizes, and some confidence intervals were
quite large. Future studies could compute stronger analyses as they
provide more effect sizes for those specific levels of moderators.
For now, it appears that the theory of psychoticism is a moderately
useful one for studies of creativity.
The focus of the present study was on the relationship between
creativity and psychoticism. There certainly are conceptual and
theoretical issues involving these concepts outside of the meta-
analysis. For example, validity of P as a measure of psychoticism,
the relationship between schizotypy and P, and the role of other
personality traits in creativity are some of the issues that can be
examined in future studies.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
meta-analysis.
*Abraham, A., Windmann, S., Daum, I., & Gu¨ntu¨rku¨n, O. (2005. Concep-
tual expansion and creative imagery as a function of psychoticism.
Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 520 –534. doi:10.1016/j.con-
cog.2004.12.003
Acar, S. (2011). Asynchronicity. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of creativity (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 72–77). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-375038-9.00015-7
*Aguilar-Alonso, A. (1996). Personality and creativity. Personality and
Individual Differences, 21, 959 –969.
Andreasen, N. J. C., & Powers, P. S. (1974). Overinclusive thinking in
mania and schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 125, 452– 456.
Andreasen, N. J. C., & Powers, P. S. (1975). Creativity and psychosis: An
examination of conceptual style. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32,
70 –73.
Armstrong, M. S., & McConaghy, N. (1977). Allusive thinking, the word
halo and verbosity. Psychological Medicine, 7, 439 – 445.
Bachtold, L. M. (1980). Psychoticism and creativity. Journal of Creative
Behavior, 14, 242–248.
Baer, J. (1998). The case for domain specificity in creativity. Creativity
Research Journal, 11, 173–177.
Barron, F. (1990). Creativity and psychological health. Buffalo, NY:
Creative Education Foundation. (Original work published 1963)
Barron, F. (1993). Controllable oddness as a resource in creativity. Psy-
chological Inquiry, 4, 182–l84.
Barron, F., & Welsh, G. W. (1987). Barron–Welsh Art Scale. Menlo Park,
CA: Mind Garden.
Bennett, D. S., & Gibbons, T. A. (2000). Efficacy of child cognitive–
behavioral interventions for antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Child
and Family Behavior Therapy, 22, 1–15.
*Booker, B. B., Fearn, M., & Francis, L. J. (2002). The personality profile
of artists. Irish Journal of Psychology, 22, 277–281.
Buck, J. N. (1992). House–Tree–Person Projective Drawing Technique:
Manual and interpretive guide. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psycholog-
ical Services.
*Burch, G. St. J., Hemsley, D. R., Corr, P., & Pavelis, C. (2006). Person-
ality, creativity and latent inhibition. European Journal of Personality,
20, 107–122.
*Chavez-Eakle, R. A., Lara, M. C., & Cruz-Fuentes, C. (2006). Personal-
ity: A possible bridge between creativity and psychopathology? Creativ-
ity Research Journal, 18, 27–38.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
(2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
*Cox, A. J., & Leon, J. L. (1999). Negative schizotypal traits in the relation
of creativity to psychopathology. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 25–
36.
Cross, P. G., Cattell, R. B., & Butcher, H. J. (1967). The personality pattern
of creative artists. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 292–
299.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). Does overinclusiveness equal creativity?
Psychological Inquiry, 4, 188 –189.
Davis, G. A., & Subkoviak, M. J. (1975). Multidimensional analysis of a
personality-based test of creative potential. Journal of Educational Mea-
surement, 12, 37– 43.
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., & Covi, L. (1973). SCL-90: An outpatient
psychiatric rating scale—Preliminary report. Psychopharmacology Bul-
letin, 9, 13–28.
Derogatis, L. R., Rickels, K., & Rock, A. F. (1979). The SCL-90 and the
MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report scale. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280 –289.
Eysenck, H. J. (1992). The definition and measurement of psychoticism.
Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 757–785.
Table 3
Standardized Regression Coefficients for the Moderators
Effect moderator

p
Gender
.09
.24
Age
⫺.10
.24
Type of sample
.22
.01
Content of creativity test
.12
.14
Measure of creativity
.01
.95
Index of creativity measure
⫺.03
.85
Measure of psychoticism
.29
.00
Domain of creativity
⫺.04
.65
Index of Creativity Measure
⫻ Measure of Psychoticism
.58
.00
Index of Creativity Measure
⫻ Content of Creativity Test
.06
.52
348
ACAR AND RUNCO
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions for a theory.
Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147–178.
*Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Creativity and personality: Word association,
origence, and psychoticism. Creativity Research Journal, 7, 209 –216.
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. New York,
NY: Cambridge University Press.
Eysenck, H. J. (1997). Creativity and personality. In M. A. Runco (Ed.),
The creativity research handbook (pp. 41– 66). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton
Press.
Eysenck, H. (2003). Creativity, personality, and the convergent– divergent
continuum. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Critical creative processes (pp.
95–114). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck
Personality Questionnaire (adult and junior). London, England: Hodder
& Stoughton.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1976). Psychoticism as a dimension
of personality. London, England: Hodder & Stoughton.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1991). Manual of the Eysenck
Personality Scales (EPS Adult). London, England: Hodder & Stoughton.
*Eysenck, H. J., & Furnham, A. (1993). Personality and the Barron–Welsh
Art Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 837– 838.
Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version
of the Psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6,
21–29.
Finke, R. A. (1990). Creative imagery: Discoveries and inventions in
visualization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fodor, E. M. (1995). Subclinical manifestations of psychosis-proneness,
ego-strength, and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 18,
635– 642.
Gianotti, L. R., Mohr, C., Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann, D., & Brugger, P.
(2001). Associative processing and paranormal belief. Psychiatry and
Clinical Neurosciences, 55, 595– 603.
Glazer, E. (2009). Rephrasing the madness and creativity debate: What is
the nature of the creative construct? Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 46, 755–764.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1972). The assessment of aphasia and
related disorders. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
*Gotz, K. O., & Gotz, K. (1979a). Personality characteristics of profes-
sional artists. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 327–334.
Gotz, K. O., & Gotz, K. (1979b). Personality characteristics of successful
artists. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 919 –924.
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the Adjective Check
List. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1398 –1405.
Guilford, J. P. (1968). Intelligence, creativity and their educational impli-
cations. San Diego, CA: Robert Knapp.
Guilford, J., Christensen, P., Merrifield, P., & Wilson, R. (1978). Alternate
Uses (Form B, Form C). Orange, CA: Sheridan Psychological Services.
Hakstian, A. R., & Cattell, R. B. (1976). Manual for the Comprehensive
Ability Battery (CAB). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and
Ability Testing.
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis.
New York, NY: Academic Press.
*Hu, C., & Gong, Y. (1990). Personality differences between writers and
mathematicians on the EPQ. Personality and Individual Differences, 11,
637– 638.
Hunter, J. E., Schmidt, F. L., & Jackson, G. B. (1982). Meta-analysis:
Cumulating research findings across studies. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Ja¨ger, A. O., Su¨
, H. M., & Beauducel, A. (1997). Berliner
Intelligenzstruktur-Test. BIS-Test, Form 4 [Berlin Test of Intelligence
Structure, BIS-Test, Form 4]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
*Joy, S. (2008). Personality and creativity in art and writing: Innovation,
motivation, psychoticism, and (mal)adjustment. Creativity Research
Journal, 20, 262–277.
*Karimi, Z., Windmann, S., Gu¨ntu¨rku¨n, O., & Abraham, A. (2007). Insight
problem solving in individuals with high versus low schizotypy. Journal
of Research in Personality, 41, 473– 480.
Kauffman, J. D., & Rich, S. A. (2010, November). Torrance tests,
science, and future problem solving: A great mix. Paper presented at
57
th
Annual Convention of the National Association for Gifted Chil-
dren, Atlanta, GA.
Kent, G. H., & Rosanoff, A. J. (1910). A study of association in insanity.
Baltimore, MD: Lord Baltimore Press.
*Kidner, D. (1976). Creativity and socialization as predictors of abnormal-
ity. Psychological Reports, 39, 966.
Kim, K. H. (2008). Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achieve-
ment to both IQ and divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Creative
Behavior, 42, 106 –130.
Kinney, D. K., Richards, R., Lowing, P. A., LeBlanc, D., Zimbalist, M. E.,
& Harlan, P. (2000 –2001). Creativity in offspring of schizophrenics and
controls. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 17–26.
*Kline, P., & Cooper, C. (1986). Psychoticism and creativity. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 147, 183–188.
Lauronen, E., Veijola, J., Isohanni, I., Jones, P. B., Nieminen, P., &
Isohanni, M. (2004). Links between creativity and mental disorder.
Psychiatry, 67, 81–98.
Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford, England:
Oxford University Press.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ludwig, A. M. (1992). Creative achievement and psychopathology: Com-
parison among professions. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 46,
330 –356.
Ludwig, A. (1994). Mental illness and creative activity in female writers.
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1650 –1656.
Ludwig, A. M. (1995). The price of greatness: Resolving the creativity and
madness controversy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
MacKinnon, D. W. (1962). The personality correlates of creativity: A study
of American architects. In G. S. Nielsen (Ed.), Proceedings of the 14th
International Congress of Applied Psychology, Copenhagen, 1961 (Vol.
2, pp. 11–39). Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.
Martindale, C. (1993). Psychoticism, degeneration, and creativity. Psycho-
logical Inquiry, 4, 209 –211.
*Martindale, C. (2007). Creativity, primordial cognition, and personality.
Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1777–1785.
*Martindale, C., & Dailey, A. (1996). Creativity, primary process cogni-
tion, and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 409 –
414.
*McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to
experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258 –
1265.
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process.
Psychological Review, 69, 220 –232.
*Merten, T., & Fischer, I. (1999). Creativity, personality and word asso-
ciation responses: Associative behaviour in 40 supposedly creative per-
sons. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 933–942.
Palermo, D. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). Word association norms: Grade
school through college. Minneapolis. MN: University of Minnesota
Press.
Plucker, J. A. (1998). Beware of simple conclusions: The case for the
content generality of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 179 –
182.
Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology. A study of 291 world-
famous men. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 22–34.
Rawlings, D., & Georgiou, G. (2004). Relating the components of figure
preference to the components of hypomania. Creativity Research Jour-
nal, 16, 49 –57.
*Rawlings, D., & Toogood, A. (1997). Using a “taboo response” measure
349
PSYCHOTICISM AND CREATIVITY
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.
to examine the relationship between divergent thinking and psychoti-
cism. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 61– 68.
*Rawlings, D., Twomey, F., Burns, E., & Morris, S. (1998). Personality,
creativity and aesthetic preference: Comparing psychoticism, sensation
seeking, schizotypy and openness to experience. Empirical Studies of the
Arts, 16, 153–178.
*Reuter, M., Panksepp, J., Schnabel, N., Kellerhoff, N., Kempel, P., &
Hennig, J. (2005). Personality and biological markers of creativity.
European Journal of Personality, 19, 83–95.
Richards, R. L. (1981). Relationships between creativity and psycho-
pathology: An evolution and interpretation of the evidence. Genetic
Psychological Monographs, 103, 261–324.
Richards, R. (2000 –2001). Creativity and the schizophrenia spectrum:
More and more interesting. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 111–131.
Richardson, A. G. (1986). Two factors of creativity. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 63, 379 –384.
Rosenberg, M. S. (2005). The file-drawer problem revisited: A general
weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis.
Evolution, 59, 464 – 468.
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Parametric measures of effect size. In H. Cooper &
L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 231–244).
New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
*Rossmann, E., & Fink, A. (2010). Do creative people use shorter asso-
ciative pathways? Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 891– 895.
Runco, M. A. (1986). Flexibility and originality in children’s divergent
thinking. Journal of Psychology, 120, 345–352.
Runco, M. A. (1987). The generality of creative performance in gifted and
nongifted children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 31, 121–125.
Runco, M. A. (1988). Creativity research: Originality, utility, and integra-
tion. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 1–17.
Runco, M. A. (1991). Divergent thinking. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Runco, M. A. (1993). Creativity, causality, and the separation of person-
ality and cognition. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 221–225.
Runco, M. A. (2004). Everyone has creative potential. In R. J. Sternberg,
E. L. Grigorenko, & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Creativity: From potential to
realization (pp. 21–30). Washington, DC: American Psychological As-
sociation.
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2010). Do tests of divergent thinking have an
experiential bias? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4,
144 –148.
Runco, M. A., Cramond, B. L., & Pagnani, A. (2010). Gender differences
in creativity. In J. C. Chrisler & D. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of
gender research in psychology (pp. 343–357). New York, NY: Springer.
Runco, M. A., Jaeger, G., & Cramond, B. (in press). The standard defini-
tion of creativity. Creativity Research Journal.
*Rushton, J. P. (1990). Creativity, intelligence, and psychoticism. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 11, 1291–1298.
*Rust, J., Golombok, S., & Abram, M. (1989). Creativity and schizotypal
thinking. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 225–227.
Sass, L. A. (2000 –2001). Schizophrenia, modernism, and the “creative
imagination”: On creativity and psychopathology. Creativity Research
Journal, 13, 55–74.
Schuldberg, D. (1990). Schizotypal and hypomanic traits, creativity, and
psychological health. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 219 –231.
*Schuldberg, D. (2000 –2001). Six subclinical spectrum traits in normal
creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 5–16.
*Schuldberg, D. (2005). Eysenck Personality Questionnaire scales and
paper-and-pencil tests related to creativity. Psychological Reports, 97,
180 –182.
Simonton, D. K. (2005). Are genius and madness related? Contemporary
answers to an ancient question. Psychiatric Times, 22, 21–23.
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Schumacher, J. S. (1993). Constraining
effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory & Cognition,
21, 837– 845.
*Stavridou, A., & Furnham, A. (1996). The relationship between psychoti-
cism, trait creativity and the attentional mechanism of cognitive inhibi-
tion. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 143–153.
Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, T. G. (1962). Primary mental abilities.
Chicago, IL: Science Research Associates.
Torrance, E. P. (1974). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms—
Technical manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
Torrance, E. P. (2008). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms—
Technical manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
*Upmanyu, V. V., & Kaur, K. (1986). Diagnostic utility of word associ-
ation emotional indicators. Psychological Studies, 32, 71–78.
*Upmanyu, V. V., & Upmanyu, S. (1988). Utility of word association
emotional indicators for predicting pathological characteristics. Person-
ality Study and Group Behaviour, 8, 13–22.
Vanderplas, J., & Garvin, E. (1959). The association value of random
shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 147–154.
Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children:
A study of the creativity–intelligence distinction. New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
*Ward, P. B., McConaghy, N., & Catts, S. V. (1991). Word association and
measures of psychosis proneness in university students. Personality and
Individual Differences, 12, 473– 480.
Ward, T. B. (1994). Structured imagination: The role of conceptual struc-
ture in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 1– 40.
Welsh, G. S. (1949). Welsh Figure Preference Test. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press.
Welsh, G. S., & Barron, F. (1963). Barron–Welsh Art Scale. Palo Alto, CA:
Mind Garden.
White, J., Joseph, S., & Neil, A. (1995). Religiosity, psychoticism and
schizotypal traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 847– 851.
Wilson, R. C., Merrifield, P. R., & Guilford, J. P. (1969). Scoring guide for
the Utility Test. Form A. Beverly Hills, CA: Sheridan Psychological
Services.
*Woody, E., & Claridge, G. (1977). Psychoticism and thinking. British
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16, 241–248.
*Wuthrich, V., & Bates, T. C. (2001). Schizotypy and latent inhibition:
Non-linear linkage between psychometric and cognitive markers. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 30, 783–798.
Received July 18, 2011
Revision received November 29, 2011
Accepted January 3, 2012
䡲
350
ACAR AND RUNCO
This
document
is
copyrighted
by
the
American
Psychological
Association
or
one
of
its
allied
publishers.
This
article
is
intended
solely
for
the
personal
use
of
the
individual
user
and
is
not
to
be
disseminated
broadly.