Late lateral energy fractions and the
envelopment question in concert halls
M. Barron *
Department of Architecture and Civil Engineering, University of Bath, Bath, Somerset BA2 7AY, UK
Received 3 August 1999; received in revised form 11 October 1999; accepted 27 June 2000
Abstract
In concert hall acoustics, spatial perception is a crucial element of the experience, yet several
questions remain unresolved. In the 1960s, there was some work on what was then called
`room impression' caused by diuse reverberation. The possible importance of early lateral
re¯ections was proposed in 1967 by Marshall [Marshall AH. A note on the importance of
room cross-section in concert halls. Journal of Sound and Vibration 1967;5:100±12] and until
recently concern for the eect of early re¯ections has overshadowed study of the spatial eects
linked to the later sound. Bradley and Soulodre [Bradley JS, Soulodre GA. The in¯uence of
late arriving energy on spatial impression. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
1995;97:2263±71; Bradley JS, Soulodre GA. Objective measures of listener envelopment.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1995;98;2590±7] have now suggested that early
re¯ections are predominantly responsible for creating a sense of source broadening [and
apparent source width (ASW)], whereas a sense of envelopment, which had on occasions been
linked to ASW, is almost solely produced by later lateral re¯ections. Bradley and Soulodre
have proposed the late lateral energy level as a measure of listener envelopment (LEV). This
paper considers some of the history of spatial perception in concert halls and reports on
measured results made in 17 halls of the late lateral energy fraction (LLF) and the late lateral
energy level (GLL). The spread of measured values of LLF turned out to be small and GLL
was found to be predominantly determined by the total acoustic absorption of halls. # 2000
Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Reverberation is perceived in two distinct ways Ð the temporal and the spatial.
The temporal aspect, which can be called `liveness', can be isolated by monophonic
recording or monaural listening. The decay of sound is responsible for creating an
Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
www.elsevier.com/locate/apacoust
0003-682X/01/$ - see front matter # 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S0003-682X(00)00055-4
* Tel.: +44-1225-826826; fax: +44-1225-826691.
E-mail address: m.barron@bath.ac.uk
audible background against which the latest note in music or latest syllable in speech
is heard. The optimum value for reverberation time is largely determined by the
temporal situation.
The spatial aspect of reverberation is more intractable. Hearing reverberation
from all directions is clearly an important part of listening to music in large auditoria.
Spatial reverberation is at its most extreme in highly reverberant spaces (particularly
large church spaces and reverberation chambers), when direct sound is relatively
weak or indeed absent. In this case the listener can be without clues as to the direction
from which the sound originates. Some aspects of spatial sound with no temporal
behaviour can be studied by using continuous noise signals.
The strength of the early relative to the reverberant sound is conventionally mea-
sured by the Clarity Index, C
80
. As its name implies, the index is usually applied to
the temporal response to reverberation. It may however also have a role to play
regarding spatial response. The Clarity Index can also be considered as an inverse
measure for perceived reverberation [4].
Some aspects of the perception of spatial reverberation were studied during the
1960s which will be reviewed below. But before several important issues of spatial
hearing of reverberation were resolved, a second spatial eect was identi®ed which
has dominated much of the subsequent discussion. Marshall in his original publica-
tion [1] was in fact less speci®c about the subjective eect that he felt was crucial to
concert hall listening than many people probably imagine [5]. The eect of early
lateral re¯ections was quickly termed `spatial impression', whereas Marshall had
referred to `spatial responsiveness', a characteristic of the space in which the music is
performed. Two measures of spatial impression due to early re¯ections have
emerged, the early lateral energy fraction (LF) [6] and cross-correlation measures [7].
The early sound is generally taken as the ®rst 80ms after the direct. Sound level is
also considered to in¯uence perceived spatial impression, though the relative
importance of the spatial and level components is as yet unresolved [8].
In 1989, Morimoto and Maekawa [9] suggested that at least two subjective spatial
eects occurred. They provided evidence that a sense of being enveloped by sound
was independent of spaciousness (spatial impression caused by early re¯ections) and
that envelopment was linked to incoherence (a low interaural cross-correlation) of
the reverberant sound. Bradley and Soulodre [2,3] conducted further subjective
experiments into spatial hearing. Their experiments were conducted in an anechoic
chamber with a simulation system using ®ve (or three) loudspeakers arranged sym-
metrically in front or to the side of subjects. They found that sound from behind had
no special in¯uence on the subjective eects being studied, and they therefore did
not include loudspeakers behind the subjects. The simulations involved direct sound
followed by four discrete re¯ections and then reverberation whose relative level from
dierent directions was varied.
Bradley and Soulodre basically agreed with Morimoto and Maekawa that there
were two spatial eects which had in the past often been confused. Bradley and
Soulodre called the two eects: source broadening and listener envelopment (LEV);
this nomenclature will be used here. (The expression `spaciousness' has been used to
mean dierent things by dierent authors and will only be used here in quotations.)
186
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
Source broadening can be measured by the perceived apparent source width (ASW).
Their experiments showed that ASW is predominantly determined by the early
sound, whereas envelopment is governed by the late sound. Most earlier work had
concentrated on source broadening, referring to it as spatial impression. Just as
perceived source broadening is in¯uenced by sound level, so Bradley and Soulodre
also found sound level to be signi®cant for LEV.
Bradley and Soulodre [3] proposed as an objective measure for LEV the late lateral
sound level, LG
1
80
or more simply GLL:
LG
1
80
GLL 10 log
1
0:08
p
2
F
t:dt=
1
0
p
2
A
t:dt
; dB
1
where p
F
t
is the pressure measured at a listener position with a ®gure-of-eight
microphone with the null pointing at the source and p
A
t
is the omni-directional
response to the same source at a distance of 10 m in a free ®eld. Time t is measured
relative to the arrival time of the direct sound. The late lateral sound level is
measured at four octave frequencies (125±1000 Hz) and averaged.
Sound direction in the horizontal plane can be simply divided into frontal, lateral
left and right and from the rear. In the case of source broadening, experiments have
shown that re¯ections from the rear produce the same eect as re¯ections which
arrive from in front with the same angle to the axis through the listeners ears [6,10];
listeners cannot perceive the sound as coming from behind them.
Bradley and Soulodre consider that for LEV listeners have the same insensitivity
to whether sound arrives from in front or behind, but there is, of course, a subjective
dierence here: source broadening is heard in front of the listener whereas listener
envelopment involves feeling surrounded by sound, including the sense of receiving
sound from behind. Some researchers are unhappy with the notion that sound can
be perceived from behind when in reality no sound arrives at the listener from that
direction.
This paper ®rstly reconsiders some work on spatial perception of reverberation
from the 1960s. The remaining discussion considers measured values of the late lateral
energy fraction (LLF) and the late lateral level (GLL) and what design consequences
result if the GLL is to be optimised.
2. Room impression as studied in the 1960s
In the 1960s, German authors used the word `Raumeindruck' for the spatial
aspect of reverberation. This seems best translated as `room impression' to distin-
guish it from spatial impression generally used for the eect of early re¯ections.
Most experimental work in the 1960s involved simulations which produced diuse
reverberant sound ®elds. It is thus reasonable to equate the room impression from
these experiments with listener envelopment.
A key experience in simulations of concert hall acoustics is to vary the ratio of
direct to reverberant energy. Reichardt and Schmidt [11] quanti®ed the transition
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
187
from maximum room impression to zero perceived spatial reverberation in a paper
whose title can be translated as ``The audible steps of room impression with music''.
Subjects were presented with direct sound and reverberation from a reverberation
plate (reverberation time 2 s). Four incoherent reverberation signals were fed to four
loudspeakers arranged at 45
and 135
in azimuth relative to straight ahead of
the subject. The onset of reverberation was delayed 50 ms after the direct sound. The
reverberant separation, H (Hallabstand), was varied while the total level was kept
constant; H is the direct sound level relative to the reverberant. It is equivalent to
C
50
in this experiment.
Subjects determined just noticeable dierences (jnd) which led to a 15 point scale
of room impression (Fig. 1). However the range of room impression used in this
experiment is much larger than encountered in concert halls. A proposed preferred
range for C
80
is between 2 dB [12], which is only three steps of room impression.
Slightly larger ranges of C
80
are found in actual halls so one can conclude that only
four or ®ve steps are perceived in practice.
Schmidt [13] subsequently extended the experiment using the same apparatus but
varying both the reverberant separation H and the reverberation time. The experi-
ment showed that H was not a unique measure of room impression. An attempt by
Reichardt [14] to resolve the question of room impression for impulse responses
containing both early re¯ections and reverberation did not prove entirely successful
since two measures emerged, one more successful in reverberant ®elds and the other
in unreverberant sound ®elds. In retrospect, one can interpret Reichardt's diculties
to a lack of understanding of the role of early lateral re¯ections.
Major experimental work relating to room impression was also conducted at
GoÈttingen. Damaske [15] investigated what was required in order to simulate the
Fig. 1. Perceived room impression as a function of reverberant separation (direct relative to reverberant
level) after Reichardt and Schmidt [11]. One unit of room impression corresponds to one just noticeable
dierence.
188
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
eect of a diuse reverberant ®eld, without the hundreds of interfering re¯ections
which exist in real rooms. Most of his experiments were undertaken with continuous
noise signals. Fig. 2 illustrates the results of two experiments using (a) two and (b)
four incoherent noise signals; subjects were required to indicate the areas in the
hemispherical space above them from which they heard sound.
One can conclude from Fig. 2 that, except for sound arriving only in the median
plane [last diagram in Fig. 2(a)], sound is perceived as arriving from the region
between the various directions of the incident sound. The directional acuity of lis-
teners is seen not to be large but, for a sense of subjective diuseness, sound must
arrive from four principal directions. Unfortunately no result is presented for sound
from the front, back and two sides.
The importance of Damaske's work at the time is recorded by Kuttru ([16]
p. 197): ``For a long time it was common belief among acousticians that spacious-
ness (i.e. a sense of subjective diuseness) was a direct function of the uniformity of
the directional distribution in the sound ®eld: the higher the diusion, the higher the
degree of spaciousness. This belief originated from the fact that many old and highly
renowned concert halls are decorated with statuettes, pillars, coered ceilings and
other projections which supposedly re¯ect the sound rather in a diuse manner than
specularly. It was the introduction of synthetic sound ®elds as a research tool which
led to the insight that the uniformity of the stationary directional distribution is not
a primary cause of spaciousness''.
In retrospect, one might question generalising from Damaske's result, which used
noise signals, to the case of music in concert halls. But the design solution with all
surfaces highly diusing (as in the Beethovenhalle, Bonn of 1959 [12]) has only
rarely been used since.
Neither of the studies from the 1960s is conclusive regarding room impression
(envelopment) but, reviewing this work in 1970, one would have probably concluded
that spatial reverberation depended on sound arriving from all four key directions
and that the magnitude of perceived room impression was determined by the ratio of
early to reverberant sound.
3. Measurement procedures in British concert halls
Objective measurements have been made in 17 unoccupied British halls which are
used for concert performance (Table 1). Detailed accounts of the individual halls
are to be found in [12]. The survey used an omni-directional loudspeaker at a central
position close to the front of the stage and a microphone of variable directivity
placed at ear height in audience locations. On average, 11 microphone positions
were used per hall. The source was placed 3 m from the stage front. The survey used
single cycle sine pulses as signals with the impulse responses being recorded on
analogue tape. The microphone used was an AKG C414EB; careful calibration of
the relative sensitivities of the ®gure-of-eight and omni-directional characteristics is
needed with this sort of microphone. The expressions for the early lateral energy
fraction (LF) and late lateral energy fraction (LLF) are:
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
189
Fig. 2. Perceived areas in the hemisphere above subjects listening to incoherent noise signals in an
anechoic chamber after Damaske [15]. Arrows indicate the positions of loudspeakers with (a) two loud-
speakers and (b) four loudspeakers.
190
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
LF
0:08
0:005
p
2
F
t:dt=
0:08
0
p
2
O
t:dt LLF
T
0:08
p
2
F
t:dt=
T
0:08
p
2
O
t:dt;
2
where p
F
t
is the pressure measured at the listener position with the ®gure-of-eight
characteristic with the null pointing at the source and p
O
t
is the pressure measured
with the omni-directional characteristic. Time t 0 is the arrival time of the direct
sound; the limiting time, T, for integration for the LLF was taken as 0.4 rever-
beration time to ensure sucient accuracy without contamination by background
noise. The responses were analysed by computer to produce results in the four
octaves between 125 and 1000 Hz.
To calculate the late lateral level (GLL), a measurement is also required of the
total relative sound level (G). The total sound level is measured with the same
loudspeaker fed with calibrated octave noise signals, while the sound level in the
same seat locations was measured with sound level meters. Further details about the
measurement procedure are given in [17].
4. Mean values and distributions of lateral energy fractions
Table 2 lists the mean values and standard deviations for the 189 early and late
lateral energy fractions measured in the 17 halls. The early lateral energy fraction
Table 1
Basic details of the 17 British concert spaces
Hall
Label Year of
completion
Seating
capacity
a
Volume
(m
3
)
Mean
width
(m)
Plan
form
Royal Festival Hall, London
F
1951
2645+256 21 950
32
Parallel-sided
Royal Albert Hall, London
A
1871
4670+419 86 650
47
Queen Elizabeth Hall, London
Q
1967
1106
9600
23
Parallel-sided
Barbican Concert Hall, London
R
1982
2026
17 750
34
Wigmore Hall, London
G
1901
544
2900
13
Parallel-sided
Fair®eld Hall, Croydon
C
1962
1539+250 15 400
26
Wessex Hall, Poole
P
1978
1473+120 12 430
30
Parallel-sided
Colston Hall, Bristol
B
1951
1940+182 13 450
22
Parallel-sided
St. David's Hall, Cardi
D
1982
1687+270 22 000
34
Assembly Hall, Watford
W
1940
1586
11 600
22
Music School Auditorium,
Cambridge
S
1977
496
4100
20
Parallel-sided
Royal Concert Hall, Nottingham N
1982
2315+196 17 510
26
Free Trade Hall, Manchester
M
1951
2529
15 400
22
Philharmonic Hall, Liverpool
L
1939
1767+184 13 560
27
Parallel-sided
Usher Hall, Edinburgh
E
1914
2217+333 16 000
29
Conference Centre, Wembley
Y
1976
2511
24 000
50
Fan-shape
Butterworth Hall,
Warwick University
K
1981
1152+177 12 100
30
Parallel-sided
a
The second number refers to choir seating.
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
191
results are discussed in more detail elsewhere [8]. One observes that the mean early
fraction is signi®cantly lower than the late. A major reason for this is surely that the early
sound contains the direct sound which by de®nition is not lateral. The mean value of the
late lateral energy fraction at 0.31 is very close to the theoretical value for a diuse sound
®eld of 0.33. The scatter of measured values is small for the late lateral energy fraction.
Fig. 3 shows the distributions of both the early and late lateral energy fractions.
Tests on the statistical normality of each set of data using the
2
test show the early
lateral energy fraction to be ``almost certainly not normal'' with a con®dence limit of
over 0.1%. On the other hand the
2
test on the late lateral energy fraction shows
that the data is ``highly normal'' (signi®cantly less than the P=10% value).
Fig. 4 shows the means and standard deviations of measurements of the early and
late lateral energy fractions as a function of frequency. Though the mean of the early
Table 2
Means and standard deviations of measured early and late lateral energy fractions
a
Early lateral
energy fraction
Late lateral
energy fraction
Diuse
Mean
0.19
0.31
0.33
Standard deviation
0.085
0.047
±
Distribution
Not normal
Highly normal
±
a
Data set of 189 values.
Fig. 3. Distributions of the early and late lateral energy fractions as measured at 189 receiver positions.
192
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
fraction is constant with frequency, the late fraction has lower values at the lower
frequencies. Since the measurement systems for both fractions were identical, and
indeed the two fractions were derived from the same impulses responses, these lower
values of the late fraction at low frequencies would seem to be either a feature of
sound ®elds or a consequence of the measuring process. The second looks unlikely
since consideration of the dierences between the character of early and late sound
does not obviously lead to this conclusion. (This could be checked by rotating the
®gure-of-eight microphone through 90 degrees and measuring the late frontal energy
fraction.)
Nor is there an obvious reason why the directional character of the late sound
®eld should vary with frequency. For instance, a major in¯uence on low frequency
sound is attenuation at grazing incidence, also known as the seat dip eect, but if the
late sound ®eld is diuse, we would expect attenuation at grazing incidence to eect
both frontal and lateral sound and thus have a neutral eect on the late lateral
energy fraction. It is just possible that the low values of late fraction at low fre-
quencies indicate less diuse sound ®elds at these frequencies.
The other feature to note in Fig. 4 is that the scatter of measured values of both
the early and late lateral fractions is smallest at mid-frequencies (500/1000 Hz) and
largest at 125 Hz.
Apart from unresolved details regarding frequency, the conclusion regarding the
late lateral energy fraction is that it is randomly distributed with a small degree of
scatter around a mean, which is close to the value expected in a diuse sound ®eld.
Fig. 4. Octave means 1 standard deviation for the measured early and late lateral energy fractions.
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
193
5. Late lateral energy fractions averaged by hall
When late lateral energy fractions are averaged by hall, the hall means only vary
between 0.25 and 0.34, a range of 0.09 compared with a range of 0.20 for the early
lateral energy fraction [8].
The hall mean values have been compared to various hall parameters by regression
analysis. The following were considered but found to be unrelated to the mean late
lateral energy fraction (LLF): auditorium volume, seat capacity and mid-frequency
reverberation time. The only parameter found to correlate with mean LLF was hall
width, with a correlation coecient of r ÿ0:55 signi®cant at the 5% level. (The
corresponding coecient for the early lateral energy fraction is ÿ0.59, signi®cance
level <2%.) Mean LLF is plotted in Fig. 5 against hall width.
One observes in Fig. 5 that the narrowest hall (G, Wigmore Hall, London) is one
of three halls with the highest measured LLF, whereas the widest (Y, Wembley
Conference Centre) has the lowest LLF. The Wembley hall is semicircular in plan,
which is an extreme form of fan-shape; one notes that it sits below the regression line
in Fig. 5. On the other hand, the Royal Albert Hall, London (A) behaves well for its
width with regard to LLF. In spite of its well-known faults [12], the Royal Albert
Hall does have a particularly good dynamic response with extremely smooth tran-
sitions during crescendos. A good dynamic response is thought to be a characteristic
of diuse sound ®elds and the result here regarding the mean hall LLF provides
objective evidence to corroborate this subjective observation.
Another outlier in Fig. 5 deserves comment: the Assembly Hall, Watford (W) has a
low mean LLF value for its width. This rectangular-plan hall has six large windows
along each side wall which are covered with curtains; this surely in¯uences lateral
re¯ections and therefore lateral fractions.
Fig. 5. Hall mean late lateral energy fractions as a function of hall width. Hall labels according to second
column of Table 1.
194
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
6. Late lateral energy fractions within halls
The standard deviation (S.D.) for LLF measurements within halls ranges from
0.02 to 0.06, with a mean S.D. of 0.04. Again this is smaller than the within hall
mean standard deviation of 0.06 for the early lateral fraction.
An interesting observation can be made on the halls with the lowest standard
deviations for the LLF; there are four halls with a standard deviation of 0.03 or
0.02. Two of these halls are the smallest in volume and seating capacity of the 17
measured: Cambridge Music School and the Wigmore Hall, London. The other two
halls with low scatter of LLF are St. David's Hall, Cardi and the Butterworth Hall,
Warwick University. Both these halls have designs which are likely to promote a
diuse sound ®eld [12]. In the case of St. David's Hall, the audience seating is
arranged in ``vineyard terraces'' and the ceiling is highly diusing; both character-
istics are good for diuse late sound. The Warwick University hall also has a highly
diusing ceiling.
Within each hall, the behaviour of the late lateral energy fraction has been com-
pared with two distances: the source-receiver distance and the lateral distance, the
distance of the measurement position from the axis of symmetry. In three (out of the
17 halls) there is a positive correlation (at the 10%) level between source-receiver
distance and LLF, whereas there is one hall with a negative correlation at the 10%
level. Three other halls exhibit positive correlations of LLF with lateral distance.
None of this behaviour is very consistent nor is there any correlation between LLF
values and either source-receiver distance or lateral distance for the whole data set.
7. Comparison between measured early and late lateral energy fractions
Fig. 6 is a scatter diagram for the 189 measured values of the early and late lateral
energy fractions. The correlation coecient is 0.45. Though this correlation is statis-
tically signi®cant (at the 0.1% level), there is probably a considerable random element.
We would not expect a good correlation since the early and late sound have dierent
characters. The late sound consists of many interfering re¯ections with an approxi-
mately diuse directional distribution. The early sound is dominated by the direct
sound and discrete early re¯ections; the relative level of the direct sound is a function of
source±receiver distance and the early re¯ections are determined by the hall geometry.
In the light of the above, it comes as a surprise to observe the relationship between
hall mean early and late energy fractions (Fig. 7). With a correlation coecient of
0.80 signi®cant at the 0.1% level, one concludes that the hall average LLF is
strongly determined by the average early lateral fraction. The regression equation is:
Mean late lateral fraction=0.37 * Mean early lateral fraction+0.24
In view of the relationship between hall mean lateral fractions, the correlation
between both hall mean fractions and hall width mentioned in Section 5 is expected.
Regarding the relationship in Fig. 7, the surprise arises due to the dierent direc-
tional distributions of early and late sound incident on room surfaces. Late re¯ections
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
195
arriving at listeners from the side have been incident on the side wall surfaces from
all directions. The early sound is generally dominated by the ®rst-order re¯ections,
which have originated from the source only. Thus, a small plane surface which
provides ®rst-order early lateral re¯ections will only provide late lateral re¯ections
Fig. 6. Late versus early lateral energy fractions in 17 concert spaces.
Fig. 7. Mean hall late versus mean hall early lateral energy fractions. Hall labels according to second
column of Table 1.
196
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
for sound which originates from the stage area; the surface does not produce late
lateral re¯ections for sound originating from elsewhere.
In two situations, one can argue that hall design features would promote both
early and late lateral re¯ections. If the side wall surfaces are highly diusing, then
the angle of incidence on these surfaces is irrelevant and we could expect the strength
of both early and late lateral sound to be similarly in¯uenced by the location of these
surfaces. Likewise, a large side wall surface will provide lateral re¯ections to listeners
both for sound originating at the source and sound from other directions ``visible''
from the side wall surface. Only a few of the 17 halls measured have particularly
diusing side walls but many have large side wall surfaces.
It can be concluded that design for good early lateral re¯ections is on average
good for late lateral re¯ections. However, the dierences between the mean values of
the late lateral fraction for halls are small.
8. Determinants of the late lateral level
As discussed in Section 1, Bradley and Soulodre [3] have proposed the late lateral
level (GLL) as a measure for perceived listener envelopment (LEV). In practice, the
late lateral level is likely to be calculated from measured values of three quantities: the
total relative sound level (G), the early-to-late sound index (C
80
) and the late lateral
energy fraction (LLF). The calculation uses the logarithmic version of the late lateral
energy fraction, which can be called the late lateral index [LLI=10. log(LLF)].
Late lateral level; GLL Late level 10: log LLF
G ÿ 10: log 1 10
C
80
=10
ÿ
10: log LLF
3
In other words, the late lateral level is the sum of a level term (the late level G
L
)
and a directional term based on the late lateral energy fraction. It is of interest to
establish the relative importance of level and late re¯ection directivity for the late
lateral level. With knowledge of their relative importance, it may be possible to
establish the design implications to achieve a high late lateral level.
As part of the original acoustic survey of British auditoria, both C
80
and G were
measured [17]. Octave results for each measure were combined into two frequency
bands: a bass frequency (125 and 250 Hz octaves) and mid-frequency band (500,
1000 and 2000 Hz). From these the bass and mid-frequency late levels have been
calculated. These two levels have been averaged by taking antilogs, averaging and
taking logarithms to give a mean late level. The late lateral energy fraction is mea-
sured over the frequency range 125±1000 Hz. The minor discrepancy in frequency
range for the late level is very unlikely to be of signi®cance. The measurements in
halls were made in unoccupied auditoria and no corrections for occupancy have
been made; the validity of the arguments below is not likely to be aected by this
either. Levels are quoted relative to the level of the direct sound at 10 m from the
omni-directional source.
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
197
Table 3 lists the basic statistics of the measured components in Eq. (3). Bradley
and Soulodre [3] quote a measured range from ÿ14.4 to +0.8 dB for GLL, which is
in good agreement with the range in British halls. One notices in Table 3 that the
standard deviation for the late lateral index is much smaller than that for the late
level. The ratio of these two standard deviations can provide us with the information
we are seeking: the relative importance of the late level, G
L
, and directivity, LLI,
towards the late lateral level, GLL. However one condition needs to be ful®lled for
this ratio to be used, it is necessary that the G
L
and LLI are independent of one
another. The very low correlation coecient between them of 0.073 suggests that
they are independent. A better check involves normalising G
L
, LLI and GLL by
subtracting the relevant mean value in each case. The sums of squares for each
normalised quantity is calculated, as is the sum of the cross-products between G
L
and LLI. The sum of squares for GLL is 2157 and the sum of cross-products is 30,
which is considered small enough to indicate independence between G
L
and LLI.
The relative importance of G
L
compared with LLI for the late lateral level is thus
3.27/0.68=4.8. In other words, the level G
L
contributes to 83% of the variation of
the late lateral level and the late lateral index only contributes towards 17% of the
variation. Inspection of the correlation coecients in ®nal row of Table 3 provides
qualitative support for this conclusion. The late lateral level is thus in practice
principally determined by the level of the late sound. Though the late lateral energy
fraction was found to be related to hall width and the mean hall early lateral energy
fraction, these variations have only a small in¯uence on the proposed measure, the
late lateral level.
The discussion now turns to the late level and what are the main determinants of
the late level. Two theoretical values are considered for comparison with the mea-
sured late levels: the traditional re¯ected sound level and the late sound level
according to revised theory [12]. For both theoretical values, linear regressions have
been conducted with measured values. The results of the regression analysis are
presented in Table 4.
Details of revised theory are given in the Appendix. The comparison of measured
with prediction according to revised theory is shown in Fig. 8. Agreement is good
with a regression line with a slope of unity. The reasons for the level dierence of
0.9 dB have already been discussed in detail; they include such things as the eects
Table 3
Measured means, standard deviations and range of values for the components in Eq. (3)
a
Late level
(G
L
)
Late lateral
index (LLI)
Late lateral
level (GLL)
Mean (dB)
0.3
ÿ5.2
ÿ4.8
Standard deviation (dB)
3.27
0.68
3.39
Range (dB)
ÿ8.4 to +8.2
ÿ7.2 toÿ3.4
ÿ14.1 to +3.4
Correlation coecient for regression with GLL
0.98
0.27
(1.00)
a
Data set of 189 values.
198
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
of balcony overhangs [18], absorption in the stage area [17], a fan-shaped hall [12]
and attenuation at grazing incidence in the bass [19].
Revised theory was developed in response to the observed reduction in sound level
in concert halls as one moves away from the source. The theory matches measured
behaviour well on average. The expression for the theoretical late sound according
to revised theory contains three terms:
Theoretical G
L
10: log
31200:T
V
ÿ
482
T
ÿ
0174:r
T
4
where T is the reverberation time, V the auditorium volume and r the source±receiver
distance. The ®rst term is the traditional one for the re¯ected sound level (assuming
Table 4
Results of linear regression between measured and theoretical late levels
a
Slope
Dierence of
means (dB)
Correlation
coecient, r
Standard error of
estimate (dB)
Correlation of measured with:
Late sound according to revised theory
0.99
ÿ0.9
0.89
1.5
Traditional re¯ected sound level
1.04
ÿ5.8
0.82
1.9
a
Data set of 189 values.
Fig. 8. Measured late sound level relative to the theoretical late sound level according to Eq. (4), includ-
ing line of best ®t.
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
199
Sabine's reverberation time equation is valid). The second term relates to it being the
late sound arriving more than 80 ms after the direct sound. The third term takes
account of the arrival time of the direct sound after it is emitted from the source; this
term provides the variation in level with position. The theoretical late level accord-
ing to Eq. (4) is used in Fig. 8.
One question of interest in concert halls is whether the sense of envelopment varies
with distance from the source. The variation in late level with distance in a typical hall is
2.6 dB (30 m range, RT=2 s). If the late lateral level is the relevant measure, then we
expect the sense of envelopment to reduce as we move away from the source; the 2.6 dB
range should be compared with the total measured range for the late lateral level of
17.5 dB. On this basis, the variation with distance may not be subjectively signi®cant.
Eq. (4) contains three variables on the right hand side. Can we usefully reduce the
correlation of measured late levels to a single objective measure? Table 4 also
includes the correlation between the measured late level and the traditional expression
for the re¯ected sound [the ®rst term in Eq. (4)]. The correlation between the mea-
sured value and traditional theory is still good with a standard error of 1.9 as
opposed to 1.5 dB for revised theory.
The conclusion therefore regarding the major in¯uence on the late lateral level has
to be that it is the ratio of reverberation time to auditorium volume, in other words
the total acoustic absorption. We would thus expect the sense of envelopment to be
high in small halls and low in large halls.
Sound level for the listener is also determined by the performance and ¯uctuates during
a piece of music. For comparison between halls it is appropriate to compare halls when
used with similar musical forces. We can expect on the basis of the late lateral level to
experience the greatest sense of envelopment when a large orchestra plays in a small hall.
9. Conclusions
The proposal by Bradley and Soulodre on spatial hearing in rooms is a major
advance in our understanding of auditorium acoustics. They have clari®ed the dis-
tinction between the spatial eects created by early and late re¯ections. Their work
led them to propose a speci®c measure, the late lateral sound level (GLL), as relating
to listener envelopment (LEV).
The aim of this paper was to determine what were the design implications of the
late lateral level and to consider whether there was any evidence that the late lateral
level might not be the ®nal answer to the envelopment question.
The late lateral level can be considered as the sum of two components: a directional
component based on the late lateral energy fraction (LLF) and a level component.
Analysis of measured values collected from 17 British music spaces showed that the
LLF does not vary much between or within halls. The late lateral level was dis-
covered to be substantially determined by the late level. And the late level itself was
found to be principally linked to the total acoustic absorption in halls. This leads
one to the conclusion that listener envelopment should be high in small halls and
low in large ones. Subjective studies at real concerts would be welcome to establish
200
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
whether this simple conclusion is valid, and also to ®nd out whether there are sig-
ni®cant changes in perceived LEV within halls. It is possible that the late lateral level
is biased too far towards the level component. Is the design criterion for good
envelopment just to optimise sound level?
Objective measures involving energy are either absolute or relative. Sound level is
an absolute measure, as is the proposed late lateral level; other components of the
sound excluded from the late lateral level are assumed not to in¯uence the subjective
eect. A relative measure involves the balance between one component and another,
as in the Clarity Index, C
80
. A relative measure takes into account the masking of
one sound component by another. Using a very simple impulse response, Reichardt
and Schmidt found that room impression, which is surely equivalent to envelopment,
was determined by the balance between direct and reverberant sound. (They used a
relative measure but in fact, because total level was kept constant in their experi-
ment, an absolute measure of reverberant sound level would have worked as well!)
Is listener envelopment really immune to the relative level of early sound, for
instance? Bradley and Soulodre [2,3] provide evidence that variations in C
80
have
some in¯uence on perceived LEV, but reject it as a major in¯uence.
The ®nal question concerns the role of sound from behind. To this author, the
presence or absence of sound from behind can be perceived in concert halls. The
proposal that sound from behind is irrelevant to the sense of feeling surrounded by
sound is surprising.
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to the managements of the various halls for allowing access to make
measurements in their halls. The measurement work was undertaken while the
author was at the Martin Centre for Architectural and Urban Studies, Cambridge,
with the assistance of Lee-Jong Lee. The measurement programme was supported
by the Science and Engineering Research Council.
Appendix. Revised theoretical level for late sound
Energies are expressed relative to the direct sound at 10 m from the omni-directional
source. The expression for the direct sound is the traditional one with the direct
energy=100/r
2
, where r is the distance from the source. According to revised theory,
the late sound energy, l, arriving later than 80ms after the direct sound is [17]:
l 31200T=V
e
ÿ1:11=T
:e
ÿ0:04r=T
:
where V is the auditorium volume, T the reverberation time. Hence the late level is
Theoretical G
L
10: log
31200:T
V
ÿ
482
T
ÿ
0174:r
T
dB:
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202
201
References
[1] Marshall AH. A note on the importance of room cross-section in concert halls. Journal of Sound
and Vibration 1967;5:100±12.
[2] Bradley JS, Soulodre GA. The in¯uence of late arriving energy on spatial impression. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 1995;97:2263±71.
[3] Bradley JS, Soulodre GA. Objective measures of listener envelopment. Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 1995;98:2590±7.
[4] Barron M. The Gulbenkian Great Hall, Lisbon, II: an acoustic study of a concert hall with variable
stage. Journal of Sound and Vibration 1978;59:481±502.
[5] Marshall AH, Barron M. Spatial responsiveness in concert halls and the origins of spatial impres-
sion. Applied Acoustics, 2000;62(2):91±108.
[6] Barron M, Marshall AH. Spatial impression due to early lateral re¯ections in concert halls: the
derivation of a physical measure. Journal of Sound and Vibration 1981;77:211±32.
[7] Keet W de V. The in¯uence of early lateral re¯ections on the spatial impression. Proc 6th Interna-
tional Congress on Acoustics, Tokyo, 1968, paper E-2-4.
[8] Barron M. Measured early energy fractions in concert halls and opera houses. Journal of Sound and
Vibration, 2000;232:79±100.
[9] Morimoto M, Maekawa Z. Auditory spaciousness and envelopment. Proc 13th International Con-
gress on Acoustics, Belgrade, 1989;2:215±8.
[10] Morimoto M, Iida K, Sakagami K. The role of re¯ections from behind the listener in spatial
impression. Applied Acoustics, 2000;62(2):109±24.
[11] Reichardt W, Schmidt W. Die hoÈrbaren Stufen des Raumeindruckes bei Musik. Acustica
1966;17:175±8.
[12] Barron M. Auditorium acoustics and architectural design. London: E & FN Spon, 1993.
[13] Schmidt W. Zusammenhang zwischen Hallabstand und Nachhallzeit fuÈr den Raumeindruck (Hal-
ligkeit und RaÈumlichkeit bei Musik). Hochfrequenz und Electroakustik 1968;77:37±42.
[14] Reichardt W. Der Impuls-Schalltest und seine raumakustische Beurteilung. In: Proc 6th Interna-
tional Congress on Acoustics, Tokyo, 1968, paper GP-2-2 p. GP11±20.
[15] Damaske P. Subjektive Untersuchungen von Schallfeldern. Acustica 1967;19:199±213.
[16] Kuttru H. Room acoustics. 3rd ed. London: Elsevier Applied Science, 1991.
[17] Barron M, Lee L-J. Energy relations in concert auditoriums, I. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America 1988;84:618±28.
[18] Barron M. Balcony overhangs in concert auditoria. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
1995;98:2580±9.
[19] Barron M. Bass sound in concert auditoria. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
1995;97:1088±98.
202
M. Barron / Applied Acoustics 62 (2001) 185±202