Gender and Relationship of Children
By
Your Name Here
For
Professor name here
Psychology 260.10
Due Date _______
Introduction
The topic of sex differences in the play preschoolers has been explored by many researchers in the past. Studies have been conducted on basic sex differences such as what toys and gender of playmates do young boys and girls prefer. The size of children's play networks, as well as if these networks change in the size during the preschool years have been explored. Also, differences in styles of play and the occurrence of positive and negative interactions have been examined. The effect that parents have on their sons and daughters, as well as preschool classrooms and teachers have been examined as possible causes of sex differences during play.
The aim of this paper is to critically review the recent literature in this field and determine whether or not sex differences occur in play. If sex differences occur, the possible reasons for this occurrence will also be examined.
Review of the Research Section
Maccoby (1990) summarized a number of studies to support her hypothesis that suggests different social situations may either heighten or suppress sex differences in behaviour.
One study was that of social interaction between pairs of young children (Jacklin & Maccoby, 1978). Pairs of 33-month old children were brought together in the same-sex or mixed-sex in a laboratory playroom, and the amount and kind of social behaviour directed more social behaviour, both positive and negative, to same sex playmates that opposite sex ones. Girls paired with boys were more likely to stand watching their partners, or withdraw towards an adult, than boys in any pairing or girls playing with girls. The point brought up in this study is that interactive behaviour is not just situationly specific, it also depends on the gender of participants.
Some of the reasons given by Maccoby (1990) for attraction to same sex partners and avoidance of other sex partners in childhood are the rough play style of boys and their orientation towards competition and dominance. Another reason is that girls find it difficult to influence boys. An example of such reasoning is supported by a study done by Poulishta (1987). Preschool aged boy-girl pairs were observed competing for an object. The children were given a chance to use a movie-viewer that could only be used by one child at a time. It seemed while pairs were alone in the playroom the boys dominated the movie-viewer. When an adult was present, however, this did not occur, The adult's presence seemed to inhibit the boy's more power assertive techniques resulting in equal access. This supports the reason why the attraction to same sex partners and avoidance of other sex partners in childhood are so strong and also why girls may also stay nearer to an adult while in a mixed pair.
Black (1989) conducted a study to distinguish between representational and social pretend play behaviours that are a function of the sex and age of the players. Black (1989), hypothesised that social skills differ by sex whereas representational skills differ by age, and the skills related to choice of play topics are related to age and sex. This study videotaped 52 preschoolers and later analyzed the videotapes to test hypotheses. Upon analysis, the hypotheses, was confirmed. Social skills were found to differ as a function of sex. Props were given to the children to use in their pretend play. It was found that older girls and younger boys play themes were more likely connected to the props than the older boys. The older boys preferred more creative topics. Another sex difference was that girls used more conversation for planning than boys did. This may have caused less misinterpretations for the play among the girls. Finally, it was found that boys were much more likely to engage in solitary play that girls.
A second study investigated the relationship between sex role flexibility and prosocial behaviour among preschool children (Doescher, & Sugawara, 1990). Prosocial behaviour are acts that help another person, such as cooperating, sharing, and helping. This study examined how the variables of preschool children's sex, age, IQ, and sex role flexibility contributed to their prosocial behaviour. It was found that sex role flexibility was positively related to boys' prosocial behaviour, but no such relationship was found among girls. This could have resulted because possibly when boys take on more flexible sex role characteristics, they are freer to express prosocial behaviour which is in contrast to the sex role stereotype of females. When the girls adopt more flexible sex role characteristics, they would not have as great an impact because girls have already developed these prosocial skills.
Benenson (1993), designed a study which examined sex differences in children's preference for a dyadic and group interaction in preschoolers. Two experiments were conducted, each using puppets. Puppets were chosen instead of a doll so that it would appeal to both females and males. In the first experiment, children between 4 and 5 years of age interacted with a female puppeteer using 1 (dyad) and 3 (group) puppets. Enjoyment of this interaction was measured by smiling and eye contact. The second experiment replicated the puppet interaction, except the content and order or presentation of the puppets was controlled. The subjects in both cases were children from a nursery school in the Boston area, who came from middle-class families. Evidence was found in both studies that females preferred dyadic interaction more than males. Some evidence was found that males preferred group interaction more than females and that males form larger play groups than females.
It appears that in the play networks of both boys and girls may undergo transformations in size after 5 years of age. Benenson (1994) conducted a study to examine this possibility. It was hypothesized that between 4 and 6 years, the size of boys' play groups increased, while the size of girls' play groups decreased. Results from the study did not confirm the hypothesis for boys, but did support the hypothesis for girls. The number of girls excluded from play groups increased significantly between the ages of 4 and 6. One possibility for these results is that girls have a preference for less stimulation and are not as active as boys. This could be self disclosure.
The effect that mothers and fathers have on their preschool children was studied by (Idle, Wood, and Desmarais, 1993). The interaction between 20 intact families was observed. Parents were first asked to complete a toy desirability scale. It was found that parents believed that neutral toys are not specific to the gender of the child while feminine toys were preferred for girls and masculine toys preferred for boys. However, this was not the case when the same parents were actively engaged in play with their child. It was observed that in general, parents spent the least amount of time with feminine toys. These results were true regardless of the gender of the parent or the child. It was found that children accepted most of the toys presented to by their parents and that their enthusiasm was equal for toys in all three categories.
Turner, Gerval, and Hinde (1993), conducted a study in both Cambridge (UK) and Budapest (Hungary). The children were interviewed to assess toy preference, awareness of stereotypes and sex-role preference. The children were also observed during free play at school. The behaviours observed included activities, playing with toys, sex of playmates, and social interactions with peers and teachers. It was found that girls liked female-typical toys, and showed more female typical behaviour than boys, and vise-versa. It was also observed that boys liked "sex-appropriate" toys more, and "sex-inappropriate" toys less than girls. Girls, however, were less stereotyped than boys in their toy and sex-role preference. In both cultures children were more frequently observed next to members of their own sex. However, the presence or adults reduce pressure to associate with one's own sex. This was shown when boys were near their teachers, the less they played exclusively with boys. When the two samples were compared, there were no significant differences in toy, sex-role, or playmate preference, but Budapest children were significantly more masculine and less feminine on the behavioral measures.
The nature of gender differences in 4-year olds was researched by Hinde, Tamplin and Barrett (1993). The results of this study showed that individual characteristics and behaviour differed in a number of ways between boys and girls.
Children prefer same-mixed playmates starting at a very young age (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1987, cited in Alexander & Hines, 1994). Explanations for this could include play styles of playmates and the gender of playmates. This was examined in the study conducted by Alexander and Hines (1994). An interview was conducted and when gender labels and play styles were presented as independent dimensions, children showed sex differences for gender labels and play styles. Boys were found to be more active, played rougher, and proffered toys such as construction and transportation toys while girls preferred dolls. When gender labels and play styles were presented as competing dimensions, boys chose female targets with stereotypical masculine play styles over male targets with feminine play styles. Preschool girls chose female targets with masculine play styles, whereas older girls chose male targets with feminine play styles.
Pellegrini and Perimutter (1989), examined the effects of age, sex and context of preschool classrooms on children's play. The subjects were children aged 3-5 years. The subjects were observed in three different play areas: art (drawing, pasting, and painting), replica (wearing dress-up clothes, playing with kitchen equipment, and playing store), and playing with blocks. It was found that children engaged in solitary play in the blocks and art areas and engaged in interactive play in the replica area. Boys were found to use the blocks area more frequently, and girls used the art areas more frequently, while both boys and girls played with the replica toys the same. Another finding was that as girls get older, their play seems to follow sex role expectations more, in that older girls' play in the blocks area (male oriented) was less advances than the younger girls' play there.
Summary and Comparison of the Research Section
All of the reviewed literature agreed in finding sex differences in preschoolers. Sex differences in play occurred in a variety of ways including the toys they preferred, the activity level, and the roughness of the play (Alexander & Hines, 1994). Generally, children prefer same-sex playmates over the opposite sex (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1987, cited in Alexander & Hines, 1994). The same finding was observed by Turner, et al., (1993). It was discovered, however, that boys preferred females with masculine play styles over boys with feminine play styles. Young girls preferred females with masculine play styles, while older girls chose male targets with feminine play styles (Alexander & Hines, 1994). This was determined by evaluating the children using an interview method.
Play styles differed between sexes even when children were pretending. Older boys were found to be more creative in pretending and didn't connect this play to props as much as girls or younger boys did (Black, 1989). Pellegrini and Perimutter (1989), found that both preschool boys and girls equally enjoyed pretending play.
Benenson (1993), found that girls prefer dyadic interaction more than boys, and further research found that the size of girls play groups decreased in size between the ages of 4 and 6 (Benenson, 1994). Black (1989) found that boys were more likely to engage in solitary play than girls, while Benenson (1993) found some evidence that males proffered group interaction more than females.
There were many sex differences when children's play with toys was observed. Pellegrini and Perimutter (1989), found that boys preferred to play with blocks, while girls played in art areas more frequently than boys. Girls prefer female-typical toys more than boys and vise versa (Turner, et al., 1993). Also, boys liked "sex-appropriate" toys more and "sex-inappropriate" toys less than girls. A possible explanation for this is that when parents are playing with their children, it was found that they spent the least amount of time with feminine toys, regardless of the gender of the parent or child (Idle, et al., 1993).
Boys were observed mixing positive and negative interactions more frequently than girls (Hinde et al., 1993). It was also discovered, that sex role flexibility was positively related to boys' prosocial behaviour (Doescher & Sugawara, 1990). Both cases agree that these findings probably resulted due to the fact that adults tend to treat girls and boys differently and that this is probably contributes to differences in gender development.
Discussion
According to recent literature, many sec differences occur in play in preschoolers. Sex differences occur in many different aspects of play. For example, the size of the groups that children play in differs with a function of sex (Benenson, 1993). This study found that girls prefer dyadic interaction, but fails to find out why this was the case. One possible reason for this is that because males generally have a grater activity level, this dyadic interaction is less interesting for them than for females. Also, the puppeteers in this study were female. Future research should be conducted to determine the effects of the puppeteer. More studies should also be conducted to determine if this sex difference is genetic, or learned. Possibly this could be done by conducting more cross-cultural studies involving cultures different from our own.
Preschoolers play with toys also contains sex differences (Turner, et al., 1993). These sex differences seem to originate form the child learning stereotypes from society. Children simply learn to like certain "sex-appropriate" toys, because they are exposed to them and encouraged to play with them more often. According to one study (Idle, et al., 1993), children accepted most toys presented to them by their parents regardless if they were "sex-appropriate" or not.
Preschoolers also show sex differences in their behaviour during play. Black (1989), found that boys preferred more creative topics than girls pretend play and girls used more conversation for planning than boys. One possible explanation for this is if males found pretend play to be easy, they did not converse with each other to understand what was going on. Future studies examine this more closely to try to determine why these sex differences occur.
In a cross-cultural study, it was found that children preferred to play with members of their own sex. (Turner, et al., 1993). The presence of adults may reduce this pressure to associate with own's own sex. This may occur because the adults may pressure the child to associate with the opposite sex, to the child has learned that adults find it acceptable to interact with members of the same sex. It seems that the largest factor why children prefer members of the same sex is because their behaviour is similar. Alexander and Hines (1994), discovered that boys chose female targets with stereotypical masculine play styles over male targets with feminine play styles, and girls chose male targets with feminine play styles. This probably occurs because if one child's play style is similar to another's, that child will be more interested and will want to interact with that child much greater than if their play styles differ.
It seems that in many cases sex differences in play in preschoolers are a result of learned behaviours. Our society seems to play a large role in determining gender differences because children are encourages to act according to male of female stereotypes. More studies, especially cross-cultural ones should be conducted to try to determine why these sex differences occur, because as of now, no single theory can account completely for childrens's sex differences in play.
REFRENCES
Alexander, G.M., & Hines, M. (1994). Gender labels and play styles: Their relative contribution to children's selection of playmates. Child Development, 65, 869-879.
Benebson, J.F. (1994). Ages four to six years: Changes in the structure of play networks of girls and boys. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 479-487.
Benenson, J.F. (1993). Greater preference among females than males for dyadic interaction in earily childhood. Child Development, 64. 544-555.
Black, B. (1989). Interactive Pretense: Social and symbolic skills in preschool play groups. Meril-Palmer Quarterly, 35,
379-394.
Doescher, S.M., & Sugawara, A.I. (1990). Sex role flexability and
prosocial behavior among preschool children. Sex Roles, 22, 111-123.
Hinde, R.A., Tamplin, A.,& Barrett, J. (1993). Gender differences in the correlates of preschoolers' behavior. Sex Roles, 28,
607-622
Jacklin, C.N., & Maccoby, E.E (1977). Social behavior at 33 months in same sex and mixed sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557-569.
Maccoby, E.E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45, 513-520.
Idle,.T.,Wood, E. Desmarals, S. (1993). Gender role socialization in toy play situations: Mothers and Fathers with their sons
and daughters. Sex Roles, 28, 679-691.
Pellegrini, A.D. Perlmutter, J.C. (1989). Classroom contextual
effects on children's play. Developmental Psychology, 25, 289-296.
Turner, P.J.,Gerval, J. Hinde, R.A. (1993). Gender-typing in young children: Preference, behavior and cultural differences. British Journal of Develepmental Psychology,
11, 323-342.
Wyszukiwarka
Podobne podstrony:
Brothers and Sisters of Children with Disabilities Medicine Jessica Kingsley PublishersThe Roles of Gender and Coping Styles in the Relationship Between Child Abuse and the SCL 90 R SubscThe Relations of Gender and Personality Traits on Different CreativitiesGender and Racial Ethnic Differences in the Affirmative Action Attitudes of U S College(1)Locke and the Rights of Childrenthe relation of hindu and?ltic culture K5CV4NTQUKSGGYWMQWHQMRLUYCHBWDE3ZIKEJUQIserbyt, Charlotte All Children left Behind How Federal Education Reform dramatically alters the PuA Propagandist of Extermination, Johann von Leers and the Anti Semitic Formation of Children in NaziBreast and other cancers in 1445 blood relatives of 75 Nordic patients with ataxia telangiectasiaThe Relation Of Hindu And Celtic CultureThe Relationship of ACE to Adult Medical Disease, Psychiatric Disorders, and Sexual Behavior ImplicHite Evolutionary prossesses and paths of relationally embedded netowork ties in emerging enterpreneChamberlain, GENDER AND THE METAPHORICS OF TRANSLATIONDifferences between the gut microflora of children with autistic spectrum disorders and that of healBritish Patent 13,563 Improvements in, and relating to, the Transmission of Electrical EnergyStatus of children’s blood lead levels in Pakistan Implications for research and policyBritish Patent 14,579 Improvements in and relating to the Transmission of Electrical Energy