17
Self-Worth
Theory
n
Self-Worth
and Motivation
n
Types
of Students
Self-Efficacy
Theory
n
Self-Efficacy
and Motivation
n
Teacher
Efficacy
M
O D U L E
Self
Theories
Outline
Learning Goals
1.
Describe
outcome expectations and efficacy expectations with respect to
student and teacher efficacy.
2.
Explain
how self-worth affects motivation and describe the motivation of
success-oriented students, overstrivers, and failure-avoiding and
failure-accepting students.
Self-Determination
Theory
n
Self-Determination
and Motivation
n
Becoming
Self-Determined
Summary
Key Concepts Case Studies: Reflect and Evaluate
Integrating
the Self Theories
n
Self
Theories Compared
n
Applications:
Enhancing Intrinsic Motivation
3.
Explain
how autonomy, competence, and relatedness can facilitate intrinsic
motivation.
4.
Define
internalization and explain how educational contexts can facilitate
internalization of behaviors.
5.
Describe
techniques teachers can use to enhance students’ intrinsic
motivation, and identify which self theory supports each technique.
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
296 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
296 10/9/08
8:55:32 AM
10/9/08
8:55:32 AM
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 297
Module
17:
Self
Theories
The
self
in self theories of motivation refers to characteristics within
individuals that cause them to be motivated. Self-efficacy,
self-worth, and self-determination theories all focus on a competence
that underlies the self and an individual’s motivation. In each
theory, intrinsic
motivation,
a tendency to engage in an activity for its own sake or out of
interest, can be achieved through feelings of competence. However,
the theories differ in several respects, as we will discuss in the
following pages. We’ll examine the self theories of motivation and
consider how each applies to students’ intrinsic motivation for
learning.
>><<
Intrinsic
motivation: See page 267 and page 279.
SELF-EFFICACY
THEORY
>><<
Albert
Bandura’s (1986, 2001) social-cognitive theory provides us with
several important concepts that are necessary for understanding
student motivation and achievement: self-efficacy, self-regulation,
and teacher efficacy. Let’s explore each of these and how they
affect students’ intrinsic motivation.
Self-Efficacy
and Motivation
Self-efficacy,
an expectation that we are capable of performing a task or succeeding
in an activity, influences our motivation for the task or activity.
To be motivated, we must have high outcome and efficacy
expectations. Outcome
expectations
are beliefs that particular actions lead to particular outcomes—in
this case, success—and efficacy
expectations
are beliefs that we have the requisite knowledge or skills to achieve
the outcome. An elementary school student might believe that learning
spelling words makes students better spellers (outcome expectation),
but to be motivated to achieve she also needs to believe that she
has the ability to memorize the assigned spelling words (efficacy
expectation). Likewise, a middle school or high school student might
believe that studying leads to performing well in school, but he also
must believe that he
has the appropriate study skills to achieve success in school
subjects. Students with high efficacy and outcome expectations are
confident about school tasks and persist when tasks are
difficult—that is, they are motivated. Students with low efficacy
and outcome expectations are easily discouraged by failure and
therefore are not motivated to learn (Bandura & Schunk, 1981;
Bouffard-Bouchard, Parent, & Larivee, 1991).
Self-efficacy
is a critical determinant of behavior in school, sports, and social
relationships (Bandura, 1977, 1997). It is domain-specific, meaning
that a student may have high self-efficacy in math
Social
cognitive theory: See page 175.
Self-efficacy.
The
Little Engine That Could
is a good example of self-efficacy, with the engine saying, “I
think I can, I think I can. . . .” as she climbs the hill.
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
297 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
297 10/23/08
3:51:05 PM
10/23/08
3:51:05 PM
304
cluster
five
motivation
Self-actualization
Cognitive
needs
knowledge
and understanding
Aesthetic
needs
beauty,
order, symmetry
Esteem
needs
competence,
approval, recognition
Belongingness
and love needs
affiliation,
acceptance, affection
Safety
needs
security,
physiological safety
Physiological
needs
food,
drink
Figure
17.2: Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs.
We
can see some similarities between self-determination theory and
Maslow’s
hierarchy of needs,
a once-popular humanistic theory. Humanistic
theories
emphasize factors intrinsic to the individual, such as needs, as
sources of motivation. Figure 17.2 describes those needs that are
components of Maslow’s theory.
In
Maslow’s (1968, 1970) theory, individuals are motivated by a need
to satisfy their full potential, called self-actualization.
In order to reach self-actualization, individuals must satisfy a
sequence of needs, as shown here. They must first satisfy deficiency
needs,
the first four levels in the hierarchy— physiological, safety,
belongingness, and esteem needs—and then move on to satisfying
growth
needs—cognitive
needs, aesthetic needs, and self-actualization. Motivation for
fulfilling deficiency needs decreases when those needs are
satisfied, while motivation for growth needs continually increases.
Both self-determination theory and Maslow’s theory focus on
internal needs as sources of intrinsic motivation. They emphasize
needs for competence (cognitive needs) and relatedness
(belongingness) as important motivational factors. Unlike
self-determination theory, which is supported by extensive research
literature, Maslow’s hierarchy has received criticism for its lack
of empirical support. Individuals do not always move in the order
suggested by the hierarchy. For example, people sometimes can be very
creative and self-actualizing even when lower-level needs such as
safety, belongingness, and esteem needs are not met.
Self-Determination
and Motivation
As
we discussed with regard to self-efficacy and self-worth theory,
feelings of competence can facilitate intrinsic motivation. Engaging
in optimally challenging tasks fulfills the need to feel competent,
encouraging intrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1992; Grolnick,
Gurland, Jacob, & DeCourcey, 2002). When cognitive tasks are
slightly above children’s skill level, they spend more time on
them, show more intrinsic motivation, and exhibit intense joy and
pride when they master the tasks (Harter, 1978; McMullin &
Steffen, 1982). Increases in feelings of academic competence from
elementary
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
304 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
304 10/29/08
9:45:08 AM
10/29/08
9:45:08 AM
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 299
Module
17:
Self
Theories
>><<
Goal
theory:
See
page 280.
efficacy
of those students in the lower groups (Schunk & Miller, 2002).
Explicit comparisons are less likely in middle school and high
school, because in many schools students are assigned to curriculum
tracks with all students within a class at the same ability level.
n
Outcome
and efficacy expectations may change over the school year. Students
may have high outcome and efficacy expectations on the first day of
school. But as the school year progresses and they receive feedback
about their performance, they may come to believe that while it is
possible for students to be successful (outcome expectation), they
personally do not have the requisite skills, abilities, or work ethic
to achieve success in that particular environment (efficacy
expectation) (Tollefson, 2000).
n
School
transitions can cause changes in self-efficacy (Schunk &
Pajares, 2002). Adolescents continually reassess their self-efficacy
in various subjects in response to the increasing emphasis on grading
and evaluation that they experience as they transition to middle
school (Schunk & Miller, 2002).
Self-efficacy
also influences self-regulation
in learners (Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Pintrich & Schunk,
2002). Students with high self-efficacy are more likely to engage in
self-regulatory processes such as goal setting, self-monitoring,
self-evaluation, and effective strategy use (Zimmerman, 2000). Many
of these processes are linked to intrinsic motivation. For example,
students with high self-efficacy tend to:
n
choose
more difficult tasks and set more challenging goals (Seijts &
Latham, 2001; Zimmerman,
Bandura,
& Martinez-Pons, 1992). Intrinsically motivated students set
moderately challenging goals that allow them to achieve mastery of
knowledge or skills.
n
respond
more positively to negative feedback and persist when faced with
failure (Pugh & Bergin,
2006;
Seijts & Latham, 2001). Intrinsically motivated students do not
fear failure; rather, they consider feedback to be useful information
for improving themselves.
n
choose
more effective strategies such as organizing information, making
connections, rereading material, making outlines, and monitoring
performance (Bouffard-Bouchard et al., 1991; Pintrich & DeGroot,
1990). This is true of students at all levels of K–12 education
(Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990). Students who are
intrinsically motivated to achieve mastery are more likely to use
effective learning strategies (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990; Pintrich
& Garcia, 1991).
Davis
High School
Read
pp. 35–47 in chemistry.
Do
math homework.
Study
for American history exam
on
Friday.
Look
up resources at library for English paper. Self-regulation.
Self-regulated students set goals and monitor their performance.
Ability
grouping: See page 373.
>><<
Self-regulation:
See page 299.
>><<
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
299 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
299 10/9/08
8:55:55 AM
10/9/08
8:55:55 AM
300
cluster
five
motivation
As
a result, students with high self-efficacy attain higher achievement
(Bandura & Locke, 2003; Valentine, DuBois, & Cooper, 2004).
Think
of a subject that you find enjoyable and one that you find
challenging. Describe your self-efficacy in each. How do your
outcome and efficacy expectations differ in each subject?
Teacher
Efficacy
Teacher
efficacy,
a belief by teachers that they have the skills necessary to teach all
students effectively, positively influences student achievement
(Tschannen-Moran, Woolfolk-Hoy, & Hoy, 1998). Teachers develop
outcome expectations (a belief that all students can learn the
material) and efficacy expectations (beliefs about their own ability
to help all children learn) (Ashton & Webb, 1986; Gibson &
Dembo, 1984).
Like
students, teachers possess different levels of efficacy. Teachers
may have low teaching efficacy for a variety of reasons. Because new
teachers tend to feel overwhelmed and sometimes unprepared to respond
to the challenges of teaching, they might believe that all teachers
can have a positive effect on the education of students (outcome
expectation) but that they
lack the skills required to teach students effectively (efficacy
expectation) (Stipek, 2002). Teachers also may have low efficacy
because they believe that:
n
a
lack of school resources hinders their ability to teach effectively,
or that the district or state requires them to teach in ways that are
not effective (Stipek, 2002);
n
a
lack of parental support for academics contributes to students’ low
achievement (Guskey & Passaro, 1994; Weinstein, Madison, &
Kuklinski, 1995); or
n
students’
low ability contributes to their poor achievement. Many teachers tend
to have an entity
view of ability—the
belief that ability is stable and uncontrollable (Oakes & Guiton,
1995; Reyna, 2000). This is in contrast to an incremental
view of ability,
in which ability is seen as improvable (unstable and controllable).
Teachers with an entity view tend to pass judgment more quickly on
the basis of initial performance and to resist changing their
judgments when they are confronted with evidence that contradicts
their initial assumptions (Butler, 2000; Plaks, Stroessner, Dweck, &
Sherman, 2001).
Teachers
who believe that low student ability, low levels of effort, and lack
of parental involvement are stable factors leading to poor academic
achievement may develop low outcome expectations for both their
students and themselves (Tollefson, 2000). This, in turn, may affect
their expectations for and interactions with students. Teachers with
low teacher efficacy tend to call on low achievers less often, give
them less praise and more busy work, and interact more with high
achievers (Ashton & Webb, 1986). Also, teachers are more likely
to give students from lower socioeconomic backgrounds and minority
students low-ability messages, such as expressing pity for students’
failures, praising students for easy tasks, or offering unsolicited
help (Graham, 1990; McLoyd, 1998). As a result, students may develop
an entity belief about their ability—believing that they have low
ability and cannot change it—and may experience lower intrinsic
motivation (Dweck, 1999; Graham & Weiner, 1993).
To
change students’ motivation, teachers might first have to change
their teaching styles (Tollefson, 2000). Teachers with higher efficacy
are more willing to try new instructional methods. They tend to use
more self-directed activities and small group discussions and to show
persistence when helping students who are having difficulty
(Tschannen-Moran et al., 1998). Teachers with high teacher efficacy
also are more open to using interactive approaches such as
cooperative learning, peer tutoring, and problem-based learning
because they believe that these types of activities enhance learning
(outcome expectation) (Tollefson, 2000). All these approaches require
students to work together to solve complex tasks or problems,
necessitating a great deal of teacher planning, organization, and
monitoring.
SELF-WORTH
THEORY
According
to self-worth theory, as proposed by Martin Covington (1998;
Covington & Beery, 1976), humans naturally strive to maintain a
sense of self-worth,
or an appraisal of one’s own value as a person. Humans are
motivated to protect their self-worth by maintaining a belief that
they are competent (Ames & Ames, 1984; Covington, 1992).
>><<
Beliefs
about ability: See page 282.
,
Cooperative
learning: See page 373.
>><<
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
300 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
300 10/9/08
8:55:55 AM
10/9/08
8:55:55 AM
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 301
Self-Worth
and Motivation
Because
schools value and reward competencies (being able, smart,
successful), students’ perceptions of ability contribute to their
self-worth (Covington, 1998). Proving their ability, therefore,
becomes a primary focus of students’ learning. This leads students
to be motivated to avoid a negative consequence—looking less
competent than their peers (Covington & Müeller,
2001).
Consequently,
students become extrinsically
motivated—that
is, motivated by external factors, and their intrinsic motivation to
learn becomes compromised.
Students
may be both intrinsically and extrinsically motivated but appear
extrinsically motivated because of external pressures to prove their
self-worth. When college students (like you) were asked why they
would do extra, unassigned work for a course, they most often
reported that it would satisfy their curiosity and interest
(intrinsic motivation). In practice, however, college students didn’t
take time to pursue topics that interested them because it would take
time away from studying for exams (extrinsic motivation) (Covington &
Müeller, 2001).
As
students progress from elementary school through high school, they
experience greater emphases on competition and performance
evaluation, and their self-worth increasingly depends on their
ability to achieve competitively (Gottfried, Fleming, &
Gottfried, 2001; Harari & Covington, 1981). Extrinsic rewards for
learning, such as good grades and high performance on standardized
tests, are symbols of success that maintain self-worth. However,
because success is defined by comparing one’s performance to that
of others, the self-worth of low-achieving students may be threatened
when they face standards that are too high for them to have success
(Stipek, 2002). For example, the No
Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act of 2001,
which mandates set proficiency goals in reading and mathematics and
to report assessment of progress toward meeting those goals, requires
documentation of proficiency levels of students grouped on various
criteria: socioeconomic status, ethnicity, disability, and limited
English proficiency. The academic challenges faced by students with
disabilities and students with limited English proficiency put them
at risk for low self-worth. Students from lower socioeconomic
backgrounds and minority students, who traditionally have performed
poorly on standardized tests, also may experience low self-worth (Kim
& Sunderman, 2005). A focus on extrinsic factors such as grades
and test scores therefore may decrease students’ intrinsic
motivation (Lepper, Corpus, & Iyengar, 2005; Lepper, Sethi,
Dialdin, & Drake, 1997).
Types
of Students
According
to self-worth theory, the distinction between “approaching success”
and “avoiding failure” is central to understanding students’
motivation (Covington, 1992; Covington & Beery, 1976). This
distinction allows us to understand the motivation of different types
of students, as shown in Figure 16.1, based on how much each student
is driven to approach success and to avoid failure.
Success-oriented
students
are intrinsically motivated. Because they value ability as a tool to
achieve mastery on personally meaningful goals, they define success
in terms of becoming the best they can be, regardless of the
achievements of others. The other three types of students define
success (and their resulting self-worth) as doing better than others,
so they are motivated to avoid failure or to avoid looking as if they
have low ability (Covington & Müeller, 2001).
Like
success-oriented students, overstrivers
are driven by high hopes for success, but unlike success-oriented
students they have an excessive fear of failure (Beery, 1975).
Therefore, they are
Module
17:
Self
Theories
Self-worth.
Schools often reward students for their competencies, leading
students to develop a sense of self-worth based on beliefs about
their own abilities.
No
Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act of 2001: See page 541.
>><<
>><<
The
concept of success-oriented students is similar to mastery-oriented
students: See page 280. The concept of over-strivers is similar to
performance-approach goals: See page 280.
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
301 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
301 10/9/08
8:55:56 AM
10/9/08
8:55:56 AM
302
cluster
five
motivation
motivated
to prove their ability by performing better than others. To do this,
they use several strategies to ensure their success (Covington, 1984;
Stipek, 2002):
1.
Attempting
only very easy tasks. This
guarantees success with little learning.
2.
Having
low aspirations.
A student might announce that he is not prepared for a test and hopes
simply to pass. Doing better than passing (success) with minimal
effort implies that the student has high ability.
3.
Rehearsing
responses.
An elementary school student might rehearse a section of text
that she expects to read aloud to minimize any reading errors.
Likewise, a student in a high school foreign language or math class
might practice the answer to a question before being called upon.
4.
Excessive
attention to detail.
Overstrivers doubt their ac tual abilities and attribute success to
extreme effort, such as being overprepared or showing excessive
attention to detail (Covington, 1984; Covington & Beery, 1976).
An elementary school student might ask the teacher if he is on the
right track with a math worksheet after every few problems, or a
middle or high school student might ask the teacher for clarification
or feedback several times while working on an individual project.
5.
Cheating.
Students
might cheat as an extreme measure to ensure success because they
believe that asking for help indicates low ability (Butler, 1998).
Overstrivers
are motivated by a sense of pride stemming from their success and by
the temporary relief of not failing (of avoiding negative
consequences), creating a continual cycle of having to prove
themselves (Covington & Müeller, 2001).
Failure-avoiding
students
also are highly motivated to avoid failure, but unlike overstrivers
they do not have high expectations for success. Failure-avoiding
students are motivated to temporarily avoid a negative outcome—the
anxiety of being identified as incompetent—and learn to
internalize feelings of relief rather than pride (Covington &
Müeller, 2001). To avoid looking incompetent, they use several
self-handicapping strategies that do not lead to any real learning
(Covington, 1984; Covington & Beery, 1976):
1.
Minimizing participation (not volunteering answers or being absent on
the day of a test).
2.
Making excuses (for missing or incomplete homework; “forgetting”
a presentation at home).
3.
Procrastination (studying or starting a term paper the night before
an exam or due date).
4.
Setting unattainable goals or selecting very difficult tasks.
5.
Not trying or making others think you didn’t try.
6.
Avoiding class participation to prevent being called on (sitting at
the back of the room out of the teacher’s view, note taking with
head down, or pretending to pay attention with a pensive look).
Boys
tend to use more self-handicapping strategies than do girls (Midgley
& Urdan, 1995; Urdan, Midgley, & Anderman, 1998).
To
the failure-avoiding student, self-handicapping strategies are a
useful way to attribute failure to causes other than low ability,
leading to less shame (Stipek, 2002). Putting in a lot of effort to
succeed on a task that others mastered with less effort implies low
ability, and failing after putting in effort would be a public
admission of low ability (Covington & Omelich, 1979). Failure
without effort, though, does not reflect negatively on ability
(Covington & Beery, 1976).
However,
lack of effort can become a “double-edged sword” (Covington &
Omelich, 1979). Because teachers value effort, students who purposely
do not try risk teacher disapproval and punishment (Urdan et al.,
1998; Weiner, 1994). Teachers may require elementary school students
to complete work during recess or as a homework assignment, and in
the upper grades they may give detentions
Approach
High
Overstrivers
Success-oriented
students
High
Avoidance
Low
Avoidance
Failure-avoiding
students
Failure-accepting
students
Low
Approach
Figure
17.1: Four Types of Students.
Students have different types of motivation, according to self-worth
theory.
From
M. V. Covington and K. J. Mueller (2001), “Intrinsic versus
Extrinsic Motivation: An approach/avoidance reformulation,”
Education
Psychology Review,
Vol. 13, No. 2, pp. 157–176. Copyright © 2001. Reprinted by
permission of Springer.
>><<
Performance-avoidance
goals: See page 280.
,
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
302 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
302 10/9/08
8:55:59 AM
10/9/08
8:55:59 AM
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 303
Learned
Helplessness.
Students
with learned helplessness accept failure and are not motivated to
try.
Learned
helplessness: See page 290.
Module
17:
Self
Theories
or
failing grades. Therefore, the student is stuck between two competing
alternatives: being punished for not trying, or trying and risking a
demonstration of low ability.
Unlike
the other three types of students, failure-accepting
students
neither approach success nor avoid failure. Rather, in response to
repeated failures to perform up to their expectations, they accept
failure and give up the struggle to demonstrate their ability and
maintain their self-worth (Covington & Omelich, 1985). Similar to
students with learned
helplessness,
who are not motivated to learn because they believe that past
failures are due to causes they do not control, failure-accepting
students (Covington, 1984):
n
take
little credit for success and believe that success is determined by
external, uncontrollable factors;
n
blame
themselves (i.e., their low ability) for failure; and
n
view
a new failure as confirmation of their belief that they lack
ability.
Therefore,
these students are the most difficult to motivate because positive
reinforcement for successes does not work, and convincing them that
they could succeed in the future is difficult (Ames, 1990; Covington
& Omelich, 1985).
Failure-avoiding
and failure-accepting students, whose sense of ability is threatened,
may attempt to maintain positive self-worth by discounting the
importance of school success (Harter, Whitesell, & Junkin, 1998).
Some adolescents use this strategy as a last resort. They shift their
attention to developing competencies in nonacademic areas such as
sports, music, art, or delinquent behavior (Stipek, 2002).
Which
type of student do you consider yourself? Has your motivational
orientation changed throughout your schooling? If so, how has it
changed?
SELF-DETERMINATION
THEORY
According
to self-determination theory, humans possess universal, innate needs
for autonomy, competence, and relatedness (deCharms, 1976; Ryan &
Deci, 2000a). We need to feel autonomy,
or
self-determination.
That is, we perceive our behavior to be internally controlled or
self-regulated, leading us to have choices in our actions rather than
being controlled or pressured (deCharms, 1968; Deci & Ryan,
1985). We also have a need
for
competence,
that is, an innate desire to explore and attempt mastery of skills
(White, 1959). To feel safe enough to explore our environment,
though, we also need to feel relatedness,
or a sense of being securely connected to others (Ryan, Deci, &
Grolnick, 1995).
>><<
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
303 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
303 10/9/08
8:55:59 AM
10/9/08
8:55:59 AM
304
cluster
five
motivation
Self-actualization
Cognitive
needs
knowledge
and understanding
Aesthetic
needs
beauty,
order, symmetry
Esteem
needs
competence,
approval, recognition
Belongingness
and love needs
affiliation,
acceptance, affection
Safety
needs
security,
physiological safety
Physiological
needs
food,
drink
Figure
17.2: Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs.
We
can see some similarities between self-determination theory and
Maslow’s
hierarchy of needs,
a once-popular humanistic theory. Humanistic
theories
emphasize factors intrinsic to the individual, such as needs, as
sources of motivation. Figure 17.2 describes those needs that are
components of Maslow’s theory.
In
Maslow’s (1968, 1970) theory, individuals are motivated by a need
to satisfy their full potential, called self-actualization.
In order to reach self-actualization, individuals must satisfy a
sequence of needs, as shown here. They must first satisfy deficiency
needs,
the first four levels in the hierarchy— physiological, safety,
belongingness, and esteem needs—and then move on to satisfying
growth
needs—cognitive
needs, aesthetic needs, and self-actualization. Motivation for
fulfilling deficiency needs decreases when those needs are
satisfied, while motivation for growth needs continually increases.
Both self-determination theory and Maslow’s theory focus on
internal needs as sources of intrinsic motivation. They emphasize
needs for competence (cognitive needs) and relatedness
(belongingness) as important motivational factors. Unlike
self-determination theory, which is supported by extensive research
literature, Maslow’s hierarchy has received criticism for its lack
of empirical support. Individuals do not always move in the order
suggested by the hierarchy. For example, people sometimes can be very
creative and self-actualizing even when lower-level needs such as
safety, belongingness, and esteem needs are not met.
Self-Determination
and Motivation
As
we discussed with regard to self-efficacy and self-worth theory,
feelings of competence can facilitate intrinsic motivation. Engaging
in optimally challenging tasks fulfills the need to feel competent,
encouraging intrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1992; Grolnick,
Gurland, Jacob, & DeCourcey, 2002). When cognitive tasks are
slightly above children’s skill level, they spend more time on
them, show more intrinsic motivation, and exhibit intense joy and
pride when they master the tasks (Harter, 1978; McMullin &
Steffen, 1982). Increases in feelings of academic competence from
elementary
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
304 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
304 10/9/08
8:56:03 AM
10/9/08
8:56:03 AM
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 305
school
to middle school result in increased intrinsic motivation for
schoolwork, while lowered feelings of competence over the years
decrease intrinsic motivation (Harter, 1992; Harter, Whitesell, &
Kowalski, 1992). High school students whose perceptions of competence
increased over the semester found the subject they were learning more
interesting at the end of the semester than at the beginning
(MacIver, Stipek, & Daniels, 1991).
Feelings
of competence enhance intrinsic motivation only when they are
supported by autonomy (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Earning an A on an
exam (feeling competent) will lead you to be intrinsically motivated
if you believe that your actions—studying—were internally
regulated, or autonomous. If you study hard because your parents
expect you to do well in school or because you want to impress the
teacher, your studying behavior is not internally regulated, leading
you to be more extrinsically motivated. Highly autonomous students
are more engaged in school, achieve higher academic performance, and
stay in school until graduation (Grolnick et al., 2002; Hardre &
Reeve, 2003).
Intrinsic
motivation also is more likely to flourish when students feel
relatedness (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Students who feel secure in
their environment and connected to others are more likely to seek out
mastery experiences, promoting a sense of competence. They may
develop intrinsic motivation for academic tasks and activities if
these are modeled or valued by others with whom they feel or want to
feel attached (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). For example, students may
become intrinsically motivated to learn if they have “bonded”
with a teacher who shows them the value of learning. Students who
believed their teachers to be cold and uncaring had lower intrinsic
motivation (Ryan & Grolnick, 1986). Relatedness may positively
affect the motivation of girls more than that of boys. Girls report
closer relationships with teachers, and teachers consider their
relationships with girls to be closer than their relationships with
boys (Howes, Phillipsen, & Peisner-Feinberg, 2000; Valeski &
Stipek, 2001).
Becoming
Self-Determined
Like
self-efficacy, self-determination is specific to a particular
activity or subject (Grolnick et al., 2002). Individuals can develop
self-determination for behaviors such as schoolwork, chores, or
attending religious functions (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997). An
individual develops self-determination through a developmental
process called internalization,
moving from less self-determined (more extrinsically motivated) to
more self-determined behavior (Grolnick et al., 1997). As Figure 17.3
shows, individuals may show different types of motivation that vary
in their degree of autonomy depending on how successful they are at
internalizing external regulations of behavior (Deci & Ryan,
1985; Grolnick et al., 1997).
1.
Amotivation
is a lack of motivation. Individuals simply “go through the
motions” and are not willing to act. This may result from:
n
not
valuing an activity (Ryan, 1995),
n
not
feeling competent to do the activity (Bandura, 1986), or
n
not
expecting the activity to yield a desired outcome (Seligman,
1975).
2.
External
regulation
is the least autonomous form of extrinsic motivation. Externally
regulated individuals perform behaviors in response to external
contingencies such as rewards, praise, punishments,
Non
self-determined Self-determined Behavior
Motivation
Regulatory
styles
Module
17:
Self
Theories
>><<
Extrinsic
motivation: See page 267.
Teacher-student
relationships: See page 334.
>><<
Extrinsic
motivation
Intrinsic
motivation
Amotivation
Nonregulation
Integrated
regulation
Figure
17.3: A Taxonomy of Human Motivation. The
degree of autonomy we have affects our level of motivation from non
self-determined to self-determined.
Adapted
from R. M. Ryan, & E. L. Deci (2000). “Self-determination
theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social
development, and well-being.” American
Psychologist,
55(1): 68–78, p. 72, January 2000. Copyright © 2000 American
Psychological Association. Used with permission.
Intrinsic
regulation
External
regulation
Introjected
regulation
Identified
regulation
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
305 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
305 10/9/08
8:56:03 AM
10/9/08
8:56:03 AM
306
cluster
five
motivation
and
deadlines. An elementary school student who studies to get money for
As and Bs on her report card exhibits external regulation of
behavior, as does a high school student who completes homework
assignments to avoid detentions.
3.
Introjected
regulation
is a form of extrinsic motivation in which individuals engage in an
activity in order to comply with external pressure. Because
individuals have partially internalized the behavior and have not
taken ownership of it, they perform the behavior to avoid guilt or
anxiety or to achieve a sense of pride (Grolnick et al., 1997; Ryan &
Deci, 2000a). A middle school student who studies before going to
baseball practice (because he would feel guilty if he put sports
ahead of schoolwork) is showing introjected regulation, as is a high
school student who feels intense pressure to ace an exam in order to
prove her self-worth (because she has not yet accepted studying as
part of her internal values).
4.
Identification
is a slightly internalized form of regulation that approximates
intrinsic motivation. Individuals identify with the value of an
activity, have accepted regulation of the activity as their own, and
more willingly engage in the activity because they see its personal
relevance. An elementary school student who says “I do my
schoolwork because learning new things makes you smarter” exhibits
identified regulation, as does a high school student who chooses to
learn a foreign language because of its importance for career goals.
5.
Integration
occurs
when individuals have fully accepted extrinsic regulations by
integrating them with other aspects of their values and identity
(Ryan, 1995). A high school student might study regularly because it
has become a part of his identity as a student.
Amotivation
and external and introjected regulation are considered controlled
(pressured or coerced), while intrinsic motivation and
well-internalized forms of extrinsic motivation (identified and
integrated regulation) are considered autonomous, or self-determined
(Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci, 2006).
What
level of self-determination do you have? How have your parents or
teachers influenced your self-determination?
Home
or school contexts that fulfill individuals’ needs for competence,
autonomy, and related-ness—called autonomy-supportive contexts—can
facilitate internalization and encourage intrinsic motivation
(Grolnick et al., 1997; Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Autonomy-supportive
parents spend time with their children, know about their daily life,
and provide them with opportunities to explore and master their
environment (Grolnick et al., 1997). As a result, their children tend
to be mastery-oriented and to have increased self-esteem, connection
to school, and academic achievement (Eccles, Early, Frasier,
Belansky, & McCarthy, 1996; Grolnick et al., 1997). Teachers with
a strong sense of autonomy in their teaching tend to be more
autonomy-supportive (Roth, Assor, Kanat-Maymon, & Kaplan, 2007).
They empathize with students’ perspectives; allow students to make
choices, take initiative, and solve their own problems; and use few
external pressures to motivate students (Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, &
Leone, 1994). As a result, their students show several benefits of
autonomous motivation:
n
deep,
meaningful learning (Vansteenkiste, Lens, Dewitte, De Witte, &
Deci, 2004);
n
greater
creativity (Koestner, Ryan, Bernieri, & Holt, 1984);
n
higher
achievement (Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2005); and
n
enhanced
well-being (Levesque, Zuehike, Stanek, & Ryan, 2004).
Autonomy-supportive
learning contexts may be especially important during adolescence,
when students are experiencing important changes such as going
through puberty, establishing their independence and identity, and
transitioning to middle and high school. Self-determination is
critical during times of change. It facilitates problem solving and
flexible strategies in new situations and helps promote healthy
sociocognitive development in early adolescence (Eccles, Midgley, et
al., 1993; Grolnick et al., 2002). Ironically, schools seem to become
more controlling just as students’ autonomy needs begin to increase
(Eccles,
Midgley,
et al., 1993; Midgley & Feldlaufer, 1987). Middle school students
face more rules and discipline, have fewer opportunities to make
decisions, and experience harsher grading practices (Anderman &
Maehr, 1994). The structure of middle schools and high
schools—where students have multiple
Autonomy.
Autonomy-supportive parents allow their children to master their
environment. The father shown here looks on to help when needed as
his daughter attempts the puzzle, but does not complete the puzzle
for her.
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
306 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
306 10/9/08
8:56:04 AM
10/9/08
8:56:04 AM
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 307
To
protect our perception of competence
To
feel autonomous, competent, and related
TA
B L E 1 7.1 Self
Theories Compared
Module
17:
Self
Theories
Self-efficacy
Self-worth Self-determination
Description
Expectations for success on a particular task
Overall
evaluation of our worth as individuals
Feeling
that we have choice in our actions Core needs To believe we have the
knowledge or skills to succeed on a task
Domain-specific
Yes No Yes Focus Perceived competence A need to develop competence
teachers,
switch classes, and often are grouped by ability—also may
discourage connectedness (Juvonen, 2007).
INTEGRATING
THE SELF THEORIES
Self
Theories Compared
Table
17.1 describes some of the similarities and differences among the
self theories. Self-efficacy, self-worth, and self-determination are
characteristics that define human beings. Competence is an important
component of motivation in all three theories. At the core of
self-efficacy is whether individuals believe they have the knowledge
or skills to succeed on a task. Underlying our self-worth is a basic
need to protect our perception of competence. A need to feel
competent is at the core of our motivational striving for
self-determination.
However,
the three self theories also differ in some respects. Self-efficacy
and self-worth theories both focus on perceived
competence, on whether individuals think
they have ability, whereas self-determination theory emphasizes the
need
for competence, the need to develop mastery of knowledge and skills.
Self-efficacy is domain-specific, referring to our expectations
about accomplishing a specific goal. Likewise, self-determination is
domain-specific in that our feelings of autonomy can vary depending
on the situation. However, our needs for autonomy, competence, and
relatedness, like our need to protect self-worth, are general human
characteristics.
Despite
differences among the theories, they provide similar suggestions for
enhancing students’ intrinsic motivation. We’ll explore these
next.
Applications:
Enhancing Intrinsic Motivation
Students
with high self-efficacy, positive self-worth, and self-determination
are more likely to be intrinsically motivated than are students with
lower levels of these traits. According to the self theories,
teachers can enhance students’ intrinsic motivation by following
these guidelines.
Capitalize
on interest and relevance.
When teachers point out the relevance of new material, students are
more likely to become self-determined in their learning (Assor,
Kaplan, & Roth, 2002; Deci et al., 1994). This is especially true
when students have low initial interest. Students also are more
likely to value what they are learning and to enjoy it more when they
are studying something of personal interest. In one study, students
valued learning more when it involved a topic of interest to them,
even when they experienced failure (Covington & Müeller, 2001).
Students are more likely to become engaged with assignments that
yield tangible but intrinsically oriented rewards, such as sharing
the results of their work with others or explaining to someone why
what they learned is important (Covington & Müeller, 2001).
Provide
realistic choices among tasks. Teachers
can enhance students’ autonomy by giving all students realistic
choices, as when elementary school students choose which book they
want to read or when middle and high school students select their own
topics for research projects. Giving all
students control over the process or the product of a task fosters a
belief in personal control, promotes self-determination, and enhances
intrinsic motivation (Deci, Vallerand, Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991;
Lepper &
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
307 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
307 10/9/08
8:56:08 AM
10/9/08
8:56:08 AM
308
cluster
five
motivation
Hodell,
1989). Choices among students must be perceived as equal and should
be guided by students’ interests (Ryan, Connell, & Deci, 1985).
Teach
and model skills necessary for success.
Rather than expect that students will acquire learning strategies on
their own, teachers need to explicitly teach strategies such as study
skills, mnemonic techniques, and math algorithms. Students who learn
strategies improve their self-efficacy as well as their academic
skills (Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons,
1990). Teacher modeling of cognitive strategies can also promote
higher self-efficacy and achievement compared to independent
learning where students read and answer questions without guidance
(Schunk, 1981).
Focus
on mastery.
When students complete tasks that are moderately difficult—just
slightly beyond their capabilities—they are more likely to prefer
the tasks and be motivated to master them (Harter, 1974). Emphasizing
mastery encourages students to be success-oriented rather than
failure-avoiding. Covington (1992) describes a mastery approach
called the “grade-choice arrangement,” in which students can earn
any grade they choose by accruing credits (so many for an A, fewer
for a B, etc.), but the higher the grade they choose to aim for, the
more they must accomplish and the better they must perform. Students
compete not against one another but for a standard of performance.
Students working under this approach learned more and were more
motivated than students in a typical competitive environment
(Covington, 1998; Covington & Omelich, 1984b). Teachers should
not allow students to select a grade option that allows them to
minimize effort, protect self-worth, or avoid failure (Ryan et al.,
1985). For example, allowing students to choose a C grade option when
you know they are capable of B or A work reinforces their attempt to
minimize effort and avoid failure rather than encouraging them to
strive for mastery.
Help
students set appropriate goals. Teachers
can break down tasks and assignments into smaller components; provide
short-term, moderately difficult goals; and offer strategies for
making progress toward goals. Mastering small components of tasks
teaches students to accept credit for their successes (Covington,
1984). Also, when students learn to set short-term, realistic goals
and learn ways to make progress toward goals, they (Schunk &
Miller, 2002; Tollefson, 2000):
n
learn
that effort as well as ability contributes to success,
n
are
more willing to put in effort,
n
improve
academic skills, and
n
develop
positive self-efficacy and self-worth.
Remember
that assigning easy tasks or helping students complete an assignment
they could not have done independently will not necessarily enhance
efficacy expectations, because students will not attribute their
success to their own ability or effort.
Provide
appropriate feedback. When
teachers give students feedback indicating that their success was due
to increased effort, students feel greater self-efficacy and higher
motivation (Schunk, 1987; Schunk & Miller, 2002). Be aware,
however, that telling students to work harder following poor
performance may lower their self-efficacy, especially if they
believe they already are trying as hard as they can (Ames, 1990;
Tollefson, 2000). Whenever possible, teachers should use
informational feedback rather than controlling feedback.
Informational feedback provides information about students’
competence and enhances their intrinsic motivation (Deci, Koestner, &
Ryan, 1999a; Grolnick et al., 1997). For example, stating “The
argument in your paper is clear and compelling” conveys what the
student has done well. In contrast, giving positive feedback in a
controlling manner undermines intrinsic motivation (Kast &
Connor, 1988; Ryan, Mims, & Koestner, 1983). For example,
students perceive words like should
and ought
as controlling even when teachers intend the feedback to be positive
and motivating, as in “Excellent, you should
study that hard all the time.”
Limit
the use of external constraints
in
teaching.
Some educational practices—such as close monitoring, the use of
threats and directives, and the imposition of goals and deadlines—can
be perceived as controlling and lead to diminished intrinsic
motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000b; Stipek, 2002). However, the way
such tools are introduced, expressed, or administered makes a
difference. For example, goals and deadlines are a necessary part of
instruction, but the more students see them as a valued component of
the learning process and the more autonomy they have in learning, the
more likely goals and deadlines will support intrinsic motivation.
Foster
relatedness in the classroom.
Show students that you care about them as individuals.
As
discussed earlier, students who believe that their teachers care
about them have higher intrinsic motiva-
>><<
Grading
practices: See page 473.
Mastery
goals: See page 280.
>><<
>><<
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
308 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
308 10/9/08
8:56:09 AM
10/9/08
8:56:09 AM
Controlling
educational practices: See page 269.
module
seventeen
Self
Theories 309
>><<
tion.
Also, show trust in your students. For example, an elementary school
student might be trusted to bring the lunch count to the main office,
and high school students might be asked to abide by an “honor
system” when the teacher leaves the classroom during an exam. Use
strategies to build a sense of community in the classroom. Examples
include highlighting group achievements, increasing opportunities for
students to interact with one another during the school day, and
engaging students in relationship-building activities (Burden, 2003).
Feelings of relatedness promote internalization as well as
integration of extrinsic values (Deci et al., 1994).
Module
17:
Self
Theories
Establishing
positive relationships in the classroom: See page 334.
Building
Community in the Classroom.
Allowing opportunities for interaction increases students’
motivation.
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
309 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
309 10/9/08
8:56:09 AM
10/9/08
8:56:09 AM
310
key
concepts
Summary
Describe
outcome expectations and efficacy expectations with respect to
student and teacher efficacy. A
student may believe that studying leads to good grades
(outcome expectation) and that he
has adequate study skills to obtain good grades (efficacy
expectation). Teachers also have outcome expectations, about the
ability of all students to learn, and efficacy expectations, beliefs
about their own teaching effectiveness.
Explain
how self-worth affects motivation, and describe the motivation of
success-oriented students, overstrivers, and failure-avoiding and
failure-accepting students. Because
our sense of competence contributes to our overall feeling of
self-worth, we are motivated to protect our self-worth by maintaining
a positive feeling of competence. Success-oriented students, who
are intrinsically motivated, value learning as an opportunity to
improve their ability and are not discouraged by failure.
Overstrivers have high hopes for success but fear failure, so
they use strategies to ensure that they will perform better than
other students. Failure-avoiding students use many self-handicapping
strategies to avoid situations that lead to failure or to avoid
looking incompetent. Failure-accepting students neither approach
success nor avoid failure because they have learned to accept
failure.
Explain
how autonomy, competence, and relatedness can facilitate intrinsic
motivation. Individuals
are more likely to be intrinsically motivated to perform activities
over which they feel they have autonomy. Autonomy-supportive contexts
lead to many benefits, including increased autonomy, perceived
competence, and in -trinsic motivation. Feelings of competence are
associated with increased intrinsic motivation for school-work.
Optimally challenging tasks enable students to feel competent,
increase students’ sense of pride, and
stimulate
intrinsic motivation. Students also are more likely to be
intrinsically motivated to engage in school activities if teachers
have a connectedness with their students.
Define
internalization and explain how educational contexts can facilitate
internalization of behaviors. Within
the context of motivation, internalization is a developmental process
in which individuals move from less self-determined (more
extrinsically motivated) to more self-determined behavior.
Educational contexts can facilitate internalization and encourage
students’ intrinsic motivation if they allow for the satisfaction
of autonomy, competence, and relatedness needs.
Describe
techniques teachers can use to enhance students’ intrinsic
motivation, and identify which self theory supports each
technique. Teachers
can encourage intrinsic motivation with these techniques: (1)
capitalizing on interest and relevance, (2) providing realistic
choices of tasks, (3) teaching skills necessary for success, (4)
focusing on mastery, (5) helping students set appropriate goals, (6)
providing appropriate feedback, (7) limiting external constraints
in teaching, and (8) fostering relatedness. Pointing out the
relevance of new material and providing students with choices among
tasks may make students more self-determined. Teaching students the
skills they need to achieve success will increase their self-efficacy.
When teachers focus on mastery and help students set moderately
challenging, short-term goals, students become success-oriented and
develop positive self-efficacy and self-worth, increasing their
intrinsic motivation. Feedback that is informational and focuses on
effort also increases self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation.
Limiting external constraints and fostering relatedness in the
classroom also will enhance self-determination.
Key
Concepts
amotivation
autonomy deficiency needs efficacy expectations entity view of
ability external regulation extrinsically motivated failure-accepting
students failure-avoiding students growth needs
humanistic
theories identification incremental view of ability integration
internalization intrinsic motivation introjected regulation Maslow’s
hierarchy of needs need for competence outcome expectations
overstrivers
relatedness self-actualization self-determination self-efficacy
self-regulation self-worth success-oriented students teacher efficacy
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
310 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
310 10/9/08
8:56:11 AM
10/9/08
8:56:11 AM
case
studies: reflect and evaluate
311
Case
Studies: Refl
ect and Evaluate
Early
Childhood: “The
Worksheets”
These
questions refer to the case study on page 258.
1.
According to self-efficacy theory, what is Melissa’s efficacy
expectation for completing her schoolwork? How would you characterize
Claire’s self-efficacy? Martin’s?
2.
Explain why asking a peer to show Melissa how to complete the math
sheet might improve her self-efficacy.
3.
How can Elizabeth improve the self-efficacy of students in her
class?
4.
Based on self-worth theory, which student—Melissa, Martin, or
Claire—would be most difficult to motivate? Why?
Which
student would be easiest to motivate? Why?
5.
Based on the case study, speculate on the degree of Elizabeth’s
teaching efficacy.
6.
Assume that Elizabeth seeks advice from a more experienced teacher
about how to enhance her students’ feelings of autonomy and
competence. Think of specific suggestions that her colleague would
provide, and explain how each would enhance students’ motivation.
Elementary
School: “Writer’s
Block”
These
questions refer to the case study on page 260.
1.
Contrast Carter’s and Shanti’s self-efficacy for writing. What
failure-avoiding tactics does Carter use during free writing? 2.
Using your response to the previous question, explain why you would
expect to see a failure-avoiding motivational pattern in Carter and
not in Shanti, according to the research on gender differences
discussed in the module.
3.
What specific things can Yuiko do to increase Carter’s
self-efficacy for free writing? Would your suggestions for
increasing self-efficacy change if the student you were considering
were Mason? Why or why not?
4.
According to self-worth theory, how can Yuiko encourage James and
Carter to be more intrinsically motivated for writing activities?
5.
Howareself-regulation
and internalization
similar? Which students in Yuiko’s class are most self-regulated?
How can Yuiko encourage all her students to be self-regulated in
writing? How will this affect their self-determination?
6.
What changes can Yuiko make to increase her students’ autonomy? How
will this affect their motivation?
7.
Based on the information in the case, speculate on the degree of
relatedness in Yuiko’s classroom. Now think outside the writing
activity. How can Yuiko foster relatedness in her classroom, and how
will this affect students’ self-determination and motivation?
Middle
School: “The
Math Review”
These
questions refer to the case study on page 262.
1.
How would you describe Sam’s self-efficacy for completing the math
problems?
2.
Speculate on whether Jack has high teaching efficacy. Use details in
the case to support your answer.
3.
What can Jack do to promote positive self-efficacy in his students?
4.
Discuss the effects a state mastery test might have on students’
perception of competence, self-worth, and intrinsic motivation.
5.
According to self-worth theory, which student—Aaron, Sam, or
Rachel—would be most difficult to motivate? Why?
Which
student would be easiest to motivate? Why?
6.
The eighth graders feel external pressures due to the need to perform
on the state test. Provide Jack with suggestions for creating a
classroom that promotes student autonomy to improve their motivation.
How can Jack promote feelings of competence and relatedness in his
classroom in general?
,
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
311 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
311 10/9/08
8:56:15 AM
10/9/08
8:56:15 AM
312
case
studies: reflect and evaluate
,
High
School: “Exam
Grades”
These
questions refer to the case study on page 264.
1.
How would you describe Chelsea’s self-efficacy? Compare this to
Nicholas’s self-efficacy. Based on research evidence regarding
gender differences, how typical is this motivational pattern?
2.
Based on the comments of Reggie, Tamika, and Carla, describe the
self-efficacy of students in the general science class. Assuming
that general science is a class for students with lower achievement
than students in AP physics, explain how this practice of ability
grouping (assigning students to different levels of classes) might
affect students’ self-efficacy. What are the outcome expectations
in science for students in the general science class?
3.
Is Curtis’s reassuring of Chelsea that she will do better next time
likely to improve her self-efficacy? Why or why not?
4.
According to self-worth theory, which student—Nicholas, Chelsea, or
Reggie—would be most difficult to motivate? Why? Which student
would be easiest to motivate? Why?
5.
Curtis realizes that the general science class and the AP physics
class have different motivational needs. Help Curtis create a
motivational plan for each class for increasing students’
self-efficacy, enhancing their self-worth, and facilitating their
self-determination. Provide specific examples that are consistent
with each theory. How do the motivational plans differ for the
general science class and the AP physics class?
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
312 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
312 10/9/08
8:56:18 AM
10/9/08
8:56:18 AM
boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
313 boh7850x_CL5Mod17.p296-313.indd
313 10/9/08
8:56:20 AM
10/9/08
8:56:20 AM
Wyszukiwarka
Podobne podstrony:
EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL5Mod16EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL5Mod15EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL7Mod25EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL7Mod23EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL6EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL8Mod28EdPsych Modules word boh7850x creEdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL2Mod08EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL INTROEdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL6Mod21EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL1Mod02EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL4EdPsych Modules word boh7850x refEdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL2EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL7Mod24EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL6Mod18EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL2Mod06EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL1Mod05EdPsych Modules word boh7850x CL8więcej podobnych podstron