PTSD, GUILT, AND SHAME AMONG RECKLESS DRIVERS
TAMAR LOWINGER and ZAHAVA SOLOMON
School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
This study examines posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), guilt, and shame among men
convicted of having caused death through reckless driving. It also examines the contribution of
sociodemographic variables, accident-related variables, and punishment-related variables to
these outcomes. Seventy-¢ve men participated in the study, 38 who accidentally caused the death of
another in a road accident and 37 matched controls. Findings show that drivers who accidentally
caused the death of another are a high-risk group for PTSD and accident-related guilt.The ¢nd-
ings also reveal that PTSD and guilt are associated with severity of the punishment, degree of
responsibility the driver assumes for the accident, and the driver’s sense that he could have
prevented the accident. Clinical implications are discussed.
Road accidents are recognized as traumatic events (Kinzie, 1989) and
have been implicated in driving phobia, agoraphobia (Parker, 1977), anxi-
ety (e.g., Mayou, 1992; Mayou et al., 1993), depression (e.g., Blanchard
et al., 1991; Foeckler et al., 1978; Goldberg & Breznitz, 1982; Malt, 1988),
and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Bryant & Harvey, 1996;
Feinstein & Dolan, 1991; Kessler et al., 1995). Yet, almost all of the literature
on the pathogenic e¡ects of such accidents deals with persons who were
injured in the accident and tends to ignore the drivers who caused the
death of others. The few studies of drivers involved in fatal accidents con-
sist mainly of clinical reports (e.g., Kogan, 1990). The present study thus
focuses on drivers who caused the death of another person in an auto
accident that they themselves survived.
The research that bears most closely on the issue at hand consists of two
studies of policemen who were involved in shooting incidents in the line of
Received 1 March 2004; accepted 2 May 2004.
Address correspondence to Zahava Solomon, Adler Research Center, School of Social Work,Tel Aviv
University, Ramat Aviv, Tel Aviv, Israel 69978. E-mail: Solomon@post.tau.ac.il
Journal of Loss andTrauma
, 9: 327^344, 2004
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Inc.
ISSN: 1532-5024 print=1532-5032 online
DOI: 10.1080/15325020490477704
327
duty in which citizens were killed or seriously injured. One study (Stratton
et al., 1986) found that a third of the policemen su¡ered from severe post-
traumatic residuals and that another third su¡ered from subclinical PTSD.
The second study (Gersons, 1989) found that about 78% su¡ered from
PTSD, although none of the policemen who were involved in a fatal shooting
incident sought professional help on their own initiative.
However, policeo⁄cers who kill in the line of duty di¡er markedly from
drivers who unintentionally kill in a road accident. Policeo⁄cers carry
weapons, are trained for violent encounters, and shoot to hit. Drivers who
cause death have no conscious intention of killing or injuring their victim;
they often know the victim and, in some cases, were close to the victim; and
they are viewed by the law as reckless.
Manmade catastrophes have been found to be grounds for severe guilt
feelings. Guilt is de¢ned as an emotional state stemming from an individual’s
awareness that he or she violated moral, social, or ethical principles. In this
state, individuals who su¡er from guilt view their speci¢c past behaviors
negatively, regret those behaviors, and feel the need for punishment to pay for
the injury they caused (Kugler & Jones, 1992; Tangney, 1990, 1991, 1992;
Tangney et al., 1989; Wolman, 1973).
Guilt feelings are an inherent part of trauma. Persons who su¡er from
posttraumatic residue describe guilt feelings both regarding their own survi-
val when others died in the catastrophe (survivor guilt) and regarding their
conduct at the time of the traumatic event, in that they did not do enough to
prevent it and its results (Glover, 1984; Jano¡-Bulman & Wortman, 1977).
In every traumatic event in which individuals feel extreme threat, fear, and
helplessness, there is an inherent tendency to feel that, in a way, they are being
deservedly punished. The tendency to feel guilt in the wake of a traumatic
event sometimes constitutes an externalization of feelings of fear and is
anchored in feelings of powerlessness and loss of control at the time of the
catastrophe (e.g., Hendin & Haas, 1984).When one was not only exposed to a
traumatic event but was also involved in causing it, the sense of guilt can be
expected to be even more intense.
The association between guilt anchored in a traumatic event and depres-
sion has been found among male soldiers and civilian women who underwent
physical torture (Kubany et al., 1995). In both groups, the more responsibility
the victims assumed for having caused the traumatic event, the more they
viewed their behavior as wrong and unjusti¢ed, the more they believed that
their conduct violated their personal values, and the more they believed that
they could have and should have prevented the traumatic event, the higher
328
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
the level of PTSD symptoms and depression from which they su¡ered
(Kubany et al., 1995).
The study of guilt and posttraumatic residuals among persons who cause
death or injury has focused primarily on soldiers and policemen, where there
is an intention of and sometimes also legitimization for injuring another. The
issue has not been examined among other populations of perpetrators or in
other traumatic situations, among them motor vehicle accidents (MVAs).The
present study aims to increase our understanding and knowledge of the
subject.
Furthermore, after an accident, along with deep feelings of anger and sad-
ness and often the reproach of the relatives of the dead, the legal system is also
involved. Another feeling that has been found to be related to PTSD is the
feeling of shame. Gilbert (1998) views shame as associated with beliefs that
others look down on oneself and see one as inferior, inadequate, disgusting, or
weak in some way. Jackley (2001) examined the relationship between self-
reported symptom distress and shame in regard to the postwar adjustment of
combat veterans. The study showed a signi¢cant and positive relationship
between shame and PTSD, depression, trait anxiety, and vulnerability.
Similar ¢ndings have been reported among battered women (Street & Arias,
2001).
The literature points to considerable variance in survivors’ psychological
responses to traumatic events. At least part of this variance may be explained
by situational, social, cultural, and personality variables. Many studies have
found that the features of the traumatic event make a special contribution to
its psychological impact (e.g., Green et al., 1985). For instance, Lifton (1967)
found that survivor’s guilt was related to the extent of the physical and emo-
tional closeness to the victim. The more the survivor felt close to the person
who was killed, the higher the intensity of guilt. Jano¡-Bulman andWortman
(1977) found that the more people believed that they could have prevented the
accident, the more they blamed themselves, and the more time that had
passed since the traumatic event, the more they tended to cast the blame on
environmental factors. They also found that those who blame themselves are
divided into two groups. One group consists of people with trait self-blaming,
who tend to feel global guilt out of their belief of being incapable persons, and
the other group consists of people with behavioral self-blaming, who feel
guilty as a result of a speci¢c experience.
The reaction of society has also been found to contribute to adaptation in
the aftermath of trauma. A study of rape victims (Frieze et al., 1987) pointed
to the problematic situation such victims were caught in.Their self-esteem was
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
329
negatively a¡ected, and they had to deal with feelings of guilt and shame
resulting from social judgment. Similarly, reckless drivers are criticized and
highly judged by their society for what they have done.
We hypothesize that reckless drivers experience intense negative feelings as
a result of the accident. Thus, our aims in the present study were to (a)
examine PTSD, global guilt, event-speci¢c guilt, and shame among drivers
who accidentally caused the death of others; (b) assess changes in the intensity
of the drivers’ PTSD and sense of guilt after the accident and in the course of
the legal process; (c) examine the association between PTSD, speci¢c guilt;
and global guilt; and (d) assess the contribution of variables associated with
the accident (acquaintance with the victim, seeing the corpse, emotional
relation with the victim) and variables associated with the legal process (type
of punishment, assessment of the severity of the punishment, admission of
guilt) to PTSD, guilt, and shame.
Method
Procedure and Subjects
After receiving permission from the Adult Probation Services (APS) to carry
out the study, we identi¢ed 65 persons who had caused the death of people as a
result of reckless driving and who were sent to the APS by court order so that a
report could be obtained about them. In the second stage, we held conversa-
tions with their individual therapists, following which nine persons who were
regarded as unsuitable for the study, mainly because of psychiatric problems,
were removed. After obtaining informed consent from the drivers to partici-
pate in the study, we mailed two sets of questionnaires to all of the subjects, one
set for them and one set for the prospective controls. Each subject was asked to
complete the set meant for the drivers and to give the other set to a friend of his
own age, gender, education, and marital status who had not been in a road
accident. These friends served as control subjects. Seventy-eight sets of ques-
tionnaires were sent to the persons identi¢ed for the study group. Seventy-six
questionnaires were returned by mail, constituting a 97.4% response rate.
Seventy-¢ve men in two groups participated in the study. The study group
(N
¼ 38) consisted of drivers involved in fatal MVAs who were found guilty of
causing death through reckless driving.These subjects had been referred to the
APS by the court. Members of the control group (N
¼ 37) were matched with
the study group on age and education but had not been involved in an MVA.
330
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
About a third of the subjects (37.3%) had graduated high school; another
third (32.1%) had a post^ high school education, and the remaining third
(30.7%) had an elementary school or partial high school education. Most of
the subjects (76%) were in the 19-to 30-year age range; about half (57.3%)
were single.
Questionnaires
The background questionnaire
tapped age, gender, education, military service,
professional occupation, family status, and religious attitudes. The PTSD
Inventory
(Solomon, 1993) is based on DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation, 1994) criteria for the diagnosis of PTSD. It consists of 17 statements,
each describing a PTSD symptom. Subjects were asked to indicate whether
they had experienced each symptom at ¢ve points of time: (a) in the previous
month, (b) before the accident, (c) from the day of the accident to the start of
the trial, (d) from the start of the trial to the day of the verdict, and (e) from
the day of the verdict to the day of ¢lling out the questionnaire.
The control group was also asked to indicate whether they had experienced
a traumatic event in the past. Those who replied that they had were asked to
indicate what it was and when they experienced it. This questionnaire has
been widely used in trauma studies and has proven psychometric qualities.
The internal consistency (Cronbach alpha) among the 17 items was .89, and
the scale was found to have high convergent validity when compared with
diagnoses based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R
(Solomon et al., 1993).
In addition to these 17 statements, two symptoms relating to guilt were
queried. A distinction was made between ‘‘survivor guilt,’’ de¢ned as a deep
sense of guilt that the person had survived while others were killed in the
accident, and ‘‘event-speci¢c guilt,’’de¢ned as guilt feelings about the person’s
behavior at the time of the accident.
Guilt and shame were assessed via Tangney et al.’s (1989) Test of Self Con-
scious A¡ect
(TOSCA).This test consists of 15 short vignettes (10 negative and 5
positive) accompanied by 5 possible responses that point to a tendency toward
shame, guilt, externalization, dissociation, and pride. With regard to each
vignette, the respondents were asked to indicate the degree to which they
would respond on a 5-point scale ranging from ‘‘not at all likely’’ (1) to ‘‘most
likely’’ (5). Previous studies (Tangney,1990,1992; Barash-Kishon,1996) found
the shame and guilt factors to have good internal consistency. In the present
study, Cronbachs alpha were .70 for shame and .73 for guilt.
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
331
The Features of the Event Questionnaire
was constructed for the present study
based on clinical knowledge gathered by therapists treating persons who have
caused the death of others in MVAs through negligence. The questionnaire
contains 32 items on the circumstances of the accident, the trial, and the
punishment. The ¢rst part gathered information about the driversfor
example, their age and occupation at the time of the accident, whether or not
they sought professional help, and whether they discussed their feelings with a
close person. The second part contained questions and items about the trial
and sentence, including whether the driver admitted his guilt immediately
after the accident, whether he had a trial where evidence was brought, how
much time elapsed between the accident and the verdict and between the
verdict and the date of answering the questionnaire, whether the driver had
his driving license revoked, whether he was sentenced to prison, and his feel-
ings about the sentence. The third part of the questionnaire contained details
about the victims (age, identity) and the subjects’ relationship with them
(degree of prior acquaintance and feelings toward them).
Attributions of responsibility for the accident were examined via four
questions derived from the Questionnaire on Speci¢c Guilt (Jano¡-Bulman, 1989).
The questions were adapted to the study population and focused on the dri-
vers’ perceptions of the causes of the accident. The drivers were asked how
much they blamed themselves, others, the surroundings, and bad luck for the
accident and death and the degree to which they believed they could have
prevented the accident and the death.
Results
Group Di¡erences
First, we examined di¡erences between the study group, consisting of the
reckless drivers, and the matched control group with regard to PTSD, guilt,
and shame.
PTSD
In the study group, 76.3% of the subjects were classi¢ed as su¡ering from
PTSD after the accident; 44.7% of them still su¡ered from PTSD up to the
time of the study. In contrast, among the control subjects, 13% were classi¢ed
as su¡ering from PTSD following an event that they termed traumatic,
and only 5.5% were classi¢ed as su¡ering from the disorder in the present.
Chi-square tests indicated that these group di¡erences were signi¢cant both
332
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
immediately after the accident, w
2
(1)
¼ 29.84, p < .001, and at the time of the
study w
2
(1)
¼ 15.33, p < .001.
To assess the severity of the disorder, we examined group di¡erences in
number of PTSD symptoms at three points in time: before the accident, after
the accident, and at the time of the study. A one-way multivariate analysis of
variance (MANOVA) showed a signi¢cant group di¡erence over the three
periods, F(3, 67)
¼ 13.23, p < .01. Means and standard deviations for number
of PTSD symptoms in the two groups are presented in Figure 1.
Before the event, no group di¡erences were found. Yet, while the control
group continued to report the same low level of symptoms in the subsequent
two periods, the study group reported a signi¢cant increase in symptoms after
the event, followed by a decline in symptoms at the time of the study. At both
latter points, the study group reported signi¢cantly more symptoms than the
controls.
A 2
3 analysis of variance (Group Time) with repeated measures showed
a signi¢cant interaction, F(2,138)
¼ 18.76, p < .001. A test of simple main e¡ects
carried out to determine the source of the interaction showed a signi¢cant
di¡erence in time only in the study group.
Shame and Guilt
A MANOVA carried out to compare event-speci¢c guilt in the two
groups after the event and at the time of the study showed a signi¢cant group
FIGURE 1 The means of the number of PTSD symptoms in the two groups during three
periods of time.
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
333
di¡erence over the two times, F(2, 66)
¼ 22.02, p < .001. As expected, the
reckless drivers reported greater event-speci¢c guilt than the controls both
immediately after the event (drivers: M
¼ 2.27, SD ¼ 0.77; controls: M ¼1.19,
SD
¼ 0.54) and at the time of the study (drivers: M ¼1.73, SD ¼ 0.77; con-
trols: (M
¼1.09, SD ¼ 0.39).
Moreover, as with PTSD symptoms, the means showed a decline in guilt
feelings between the period immediately following the event and the time of
the study. An analysis of variance with repeated measures showed a sig-
ni¢cant di¡erence in event-speci¢c guilt feelings after the event and at the
time of the study, F(1, 67)
¼ 10.48, p < .01. Moreover, a signi¢cant interaction
was found between group and time, F(1, 67)
¼ 10.48, p < .01.The di¡erence in
the two groups’ current guilt was smaller than that at the time of the event.
A one-way multivariate analysis of covariance carried out to examine
group di¡erences in shame and global sense of guilt yielded no signi¢cant
di¡erences.That is, the drivers who caused death did not feel greater shame or
a stronger global sense of guilt than the controls.
In summary, examination of the di¡erences between the two groups clearly
showed that reckless drivers are a high risk group for PTSD and that their
symptoms may persist for a long period of time.The ¢ndings also clearly show
that this group is characterized by a relatively large number of PTSD symp-
toms and a high level of event-speci¢c guilt feelings.
Changes in Drivers’PTSD and Guilt Feelings OverT|me
We also examined changes in PTSD and guilt over time in the drivers’
group. The following points in time were measured: between the accident
and the start of the trial, between the start of the trial and the verdict,
between the verdict and the completion of the questionnaires, and in the
previous month.
PTSD
A total of 76.3% of the drivers were classi¢ed as having PTSD between the
accident and the start of the trial; 71.1% during the trial and before senten-
cing; 55.3%, after sentencing; and 44.7%, in the month prior to their ¢lling
out the questionnaire.
To examine changes in PTSD over time, we also calculated the average
number of PTSD symptoms in each period. Findings showed that, before the
accident, the drivers had very few PTSD symptoms. Following the accident,
the number of symptoms they reported rose sharply and then gradually
334
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
declined to a moderate level at the time of the study. An analysis of variance
with repeated measures showed signi¢cant di¡erences between the periods,
F
(4, 140)
¼ 59.51, p < .001.T tests for dependent samples carried out between
each pair of adjacent periods revealed signi¢cant di¡erences in the intensity of
PTSD in all of the periods within an overall pattern of decline. Sign-test
analyses (Sigel, 1956) performed to examine the di¡erences in the number of
persons classi¢ed with PTSD in the four periods revealed signi¢cant di¡er-
ences between the period covering the time between the accident and the start
of the trial and that covering the time between the start of the trial and sen-
tencing, as well as between sentencing and the time of the study. However, no
signi¢cant di¡erence was found between the two earlier periods and between
the two later periods.
Guilt
Similar time comparisons were carried out on the drivers’ guilt feelings.
The changes in guilt corresponded with those found for PTSD symptoms.
That is, the greatest guilt was reported in the period between the accident and
the sentencing. After that, the guilt feelings declined, reaching their lowest
level at the time of the study. Here too an analysis of variance with repeated
measures showed signi¢cant di¡erences in the four time periods, F(3,
102)
¼ 12.64, p < .001. Paired-comparison t tests showed a signi¢cant di¡er-
ence between all of the periods except for the ¢rst two (between the accident
and the start of the trial and between the start of the trial and the sentencing).
Features of the Accident, PSTD, and Guilt
The drivers were asked about their acquaintance with the victim, whether
they saw the corpse after the accident, and their feelings toward the victim. Of
the drivers, 44.27% knew the victim before his or her death. In 16.7% of these
cases, the victim was a family member; in the remaining 83.3%, the victim
was a friend or acquaintance. Fourteen percent of the drivers saw the corpse,
and most of the drivers (72%) reported positive feelings toward the victim.
Chi-square tests showed no signi¢cant association between any of these
variables and either PTSD or guilt feelings.
Sense of Responsibility for the Accident, PTSD, and Guilt
The respondents were asked how much they blamed themselves for having
caused the death, how much they believed that they could have prevented the
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
335
accident, and how much responsibility they believed that luck, circumstances,
some other factor, and they themselves bore for the accident.
The majority (54.4%) of the drivers viewed themselves as responsible for
the accident; 18.2% attributed the responsibility to the circumstances; 11.7%
attributed it to others; and 15.7% blamed luck. The degree of reported self-
blame was relatively high (M
¼ 3.42, SD ¼1.32) on a 5-point scale. Their
assessment that they could have prevented the accident was moderate
(M
¼ 2.76, SD ¼1.28).
Pearson correlations were carried out to examine the associations between
the responsibility variables, PTSD, and guilt.The coe⁄cients are presented in
Table 1.
As can be seen, signi¢cant positive correlations were found between self-
blame (the driver’s belief that he could have prevented the accident and his
attribution of the cause of the accident to himself ) and (a) PTSD symptoms
after the accident and (b) event-related guilt after the accident and at the time
of the study. Similarly, negative correlations were found between attribution
of the cause of the accident to other people and PTSD symptoms and event-
speci¢c guilt feelings. The more the drivers saw themselves as responsible for
the accident, the more PTSD symptoms they reported, and the more severe
their event-speci¢c guilt. On the other hand, no signi¢cant correlations were
found between these variables and intensity of PTSD at the time of the study.
A positive correlation was found between the driver’s belief that he could have
prevented the accident and his feelings of shame, and a negative correlation
was found between attribution of the accident to luck and feelings of global
guilt. Drivers who attributed the accident to luck felt less guilt.
The drivers were asked to assess the reasons for the accident. Their answers
were grouped into two categories: internal reasons (e.g., my careless driving)
and external reasons (e.g., the car malfunctioned, weather conditions). Chi-
square tests carried out to determine whether there was any association
between causal attribution for the accident and PTSD after the accident and
at the time of the study showed signi¢cant di¡erences between drivers who
attributed the accident to external factors and those who attributed it to
internal ones in regard to PTSD at the time of the study, w
2
ð1Þ ¼ 3:80, p < .05,
and PTSD after the accident, w
2
ð1Þ ¼ 4:38, p < .05. Figure 2 presents the
¢ndings with regard to PTSD after the accident.
As can be seen, the PTSD drivers were more inclined to attribute the acci-
dent to internal causes than to external ones. At the time of the study, although
the number of PTSD drivers declined, those who attributed the accident to
external factors still tended to report fewer PTSD symptoms than those who
336
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
TA
B
L
E
1
P
ears
o
n
C
o
rr
el
a
tions
Be
tw
een
R
esp
o
n
si
b
il
ity
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
a
nd
P
T
S
D
a
n
d
G
ui
lt
B
la
ming
the
self
for
ca
using
d
ea
th
Bel
ie
f
tha
t
the
a
cci
d
en
t
could
h
av
e
been
p
rev
en
te
d
L
u
ck
Ex
ternal
ca
use
s
Ot
h
er
pe
o
p
le
S
elf
P
T
S
D
sy
m
p
to
ms
at
ti
m
e
of
st
u
d
y
.2
2
.2
0
7
.28*
.0
3
7
.0
1
.1
7
P
T
S
D
sy
m
p
to
ms
aft
er
the
a
cci
d
en
t
.42*
*
.28*
7
.0
1
7
.0
1
7
.4
7
*
.3
2
*
S
peci
fi
c
g
u
il
t
at
ti
m
e
of
st
u
d
y
.4
8
*
*
.5
1*
*
7
.2
4
7
.0
8
7
.2
6
.3
7
*
S
peci
fi
c
g
u
il
t
aft
er
the
a
cci
d
en
t
.4
7*
*
.42*
*
7
.1
3
7
.1
1
7
.3
5
*
.3
8
*
S
h
a
m
e
.29
*
.29
*
7
.1
7
.1
0
7
.0
9
.1
1
Gl
o
b
al
gu
il
t
.0
9
.1
4
7
.28*
.2
2
7
.0
2
.07
*
p
<
.0
5;
*
*
p
<
.0
1.
337
attributed it to internal factors. In other words, the more the driver saw him-
self as responsible for the accident and the more he believed that he could have
prevented it, the more likely he was to su¡er from PTSD.
LegalVariables, PTSD, and Guilt
The subjects who accidentally killed persons were asked a number of questions
about the legal process, for example, whether they admitted their guilt and
the severity of their punishment (e.g., prison, community service, suspended
sentence). About 60% of the drivers had admitted their guilt to the police.
Most (62.5%) received community service. About a ¢fth (21.9%) received
prison sentences, and only 15.6% received suspended sentences. Close to half
(45.5%) felt that their punishment ¢t their crime. Over a third (36.4%) felt
that it was too severe. Only 18.2% felt that it was too lenient.
Chi-square tests revealed no signi¢cant association between these variables
and PTSD rates, either after the accident or at the time of the study. None-
theless, signi¢cant associations were found between the length of time for
which the person’s driving license was revoked and both his level of symptoms
after the accident (r
¼.40, p < .01) and his event-speci¢c guilt after the acci-
dent (r
¼.44, p < .01). Persons whose driving license was revoked for longer
periods reported higher levels of symptoms.
To examine the association between the legal process and feelings of guilt, t
tests were carried out on admission of guilt, and one-way analyses of variance
were carried out on perceived severity of the punishment. These analyses
showed no di¡erences with regard to admission of guilt and the judge’s
FIGURE 2 The distribution of drivers with and without PTSD after the accident according
to attribution of accident.
338
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
assessment of the punishment. But several signi¢cant di¡erences were found
in regard to subjects’assessment of the severity of the punishment.
Those who regarded their punishment as too lenient seemed to di¡er from
the two other groups on all of the measures. They reported more guilt and
more PTSD in the present, though the di¡erence did not reach signi¢cance.
The di¡erence was signi¢cant only for shame (punishment too severe:
M
¼ 2.69, SD ¼ 0.48; punishment appropriate, M ¼ 2.79, SD ¼ 0.64; punish-
ment too easy: M
¼ 3.82, SD ¼ 0.42). A Sche¡e paired-comparison analysis
showed that the di¡erence was between those who believed that their pun-
ishment was too lenient and the other two groups. It thus seems that persons
who believed that their punishment was too lenient tended to feel more guilt
and shame and to report more PTSD symptoms than those who believed that
their punishment was appropriate or too severe.
Discussion
Consistent with the ¢rst hypothesis, we found that men who caused the death
of others through reckless driving su¡ered from high rates of PTSD and event-
speci¢c guilt. While this study showed that around 45% of reckless drivers
continue to su¡er from PTSD, two recent prospective studies showed that
32% of road accident victims in Israel had PTSD 6 (Sayag, 2000) and 12
months after the accident (Koren et al., 1999). These ¢ndings suggest that
road accidents can cause psychological damage in both the victims and the
reckless drivers and that the latter are at even more risk than the former.
Most of our knowledge of the pathogenic e¡ects in perpetrators comes from
clinical observations of soldiers with PTSD (Haley, 1978; Hendin & Hass,
1984; Lifton, 1973). Both the careless driver and the soldier cause the death of
others, though it is important to remember that the circumstances and
meanings in the two cases are very di¡erent. Wars and road accidents di¡er
from one another both in the legitimacy they provide for killing and in the
emotional readiness of the perpetrators to cause and be exposed to death.
Causing death without malicious intent can explain the high rates of event-
speci¢c guilt found among the study group. Drivers who caused death and
remain alive become a target for feelings of anger, aggression, accusations,
and helplessness from all social circles: the families of the victims, society, and
the legal system.Thus, they feel a lack of legitimacy to externalize and express
their distress openly. Thus, as in other traumatized populations under similar
constraints, these feelings are displaced into strong feelings of guilt. The
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
339
drivers reported two sources for their guilt feelings. The ¢rst related to killing,
and thus crossing the boundaries of moral and social laws and values. The
second was that they themselves survived while others, sometimes their
friends and family, died because of their actions. The combination of causing
death and surviving intensi¢ed their speci¢c feelings of guilt.
On the other hand, global guilt and shame were not characteristic feelings of
reckless drivers. Subjects in the two groups did not di¡er from one another on
these measures. These ¢ndings are similar to a study of concentration camp
survivors, who did not di¡er in the level of guilt they experienced in their daily
lives from controls who had not been exposed to the Holocaust (Lobel &
Yahia, 1985). That is, our ¢ndings do not support generalizing from event-
speci¢c guilt feelings to general guilt feelings. This points to the need to distin-
guish between guilt that is speci¢c to and focused on a particular event and
guilt that is global and represents a personality tendency (Tangney,1990,1991).
With regard to changes over time, the highest rates of both PTSD and
speci¢c guilt feelings were reported in the period between the accident and
the sentencing, and gradually declined with time. The period between the
accident and the end of the trial was characterized by intense psychological
distress as the drivers were exposed to stimuli resembling or symbolizing the
traumatic accident. Furthermore, during the legal process the drivers had to
deal with both the court’s accusations and their own personal guilt. Kubany
et al. (1995) found that the intensity of guilt that individuals feel after a trau-
matic event depends on the degree to which they view the results of the event
as negative, see themselves as responsible for the negative results of the event,
see their act as unjusti¢ed, and believe that they could have prevented the
event had they acted di¡erently. In a way, the legal system acts as the ‘‘other
voice’’ that continues to remind the driver that he could or should have acted
di¡erently.
Our ¢ndings are similar to those of Kubany et al. (1995), who found asso-
ciations between the intensity of event-speci¢c guilt feelings and the rate of
PTSD symptoms among both American soldiers who had fought inVietnam
and physically abused women. The subjects in the present study di¡ered from
these groups in terms of age, ethnic origin, culture, gender, and, of course,
type of traumatic event. Nonetheless, our ¢ndings reinforce Kubany et al.’s
(1995) contention that it is important to deepen and broaden the systematic
study of di¡erent types of guilt and of their implications for PTSD.
Contrary to expectation, our ¢ndings showed no signi¢cant correlation
between the features of the accident and the legal process (e.g., acquaintance
with the victim, seeing the corpse after the accident, feelings toward the
340
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
victim, type and severity of punishment) and PTSD or guilt feelings. Our
¢ndings are inconsistent with a previous study of soldiers who were involved in
atrocities committed against civilians and prisoners. They were found to be at
higher risk for PTSD and had stronger guilt feelings than soldiers who fought
an enemy army from a distance and without close personal contact (Hendin &
Haas, 1984). The ‘‘ceiling e¡ect’’ phenomenon could serve as an explanation
for our ¢ndings. The ceiling e¡ect occurs when a variable is rated very highly
across all study groups, and this lack of variability prevents one from obtain-
ing signi¢cant results. As we have seen, causing death give rise to very high
negative feelings among the reckless drivers, and so other associated variables
such as acquaintance with the victim did not enhance PTSD rates and guilt as
we had expected (e.g., Fatzinger, 1997; Parente, 2001).
Drivers who perceived their punishment as too easy reported more feelings
of guilt, shame, and PTSD symptoms at the time of the study than drivers
who regarded their punishment as appropriate or too severe. It seems that
severe punishments made the reckless drivers feel as though they were paying
for what they had done or getting what they deserved thus promoting a sense
of relief. However, more than half of the drivers in this study assumed
responsibility for causing the accident, while the other half divided the
responsibility in roughly equal proportions between the surrounding circum-
stances, luck, and other people.
In the last two decades, substantial e¡orts have been made to study the
psychological e¡ects of traumatic events. In light of the proliferation of stu-
dies, the limited attention paid to perpetrators is salient. The high rates of
psychological distress found among the perpetrators in this study and in other
studies (e.g., Haley, 1978) raise the question of why these individuals receive
limited professional attention.
Haley (1978) pointed out the di⁄culties that therapists experience in
treating Vietnam veterans who had been involved in atrocities, suggesting
that these encounters challenge their values and give rise to feelings of vul-
nerability. Similar observations were documented in a study of o¡spring of
Nazi perpetrators conducted by Bar-On (1989; Bar-On & Charny, 1988).
Bar-On claims that both perpetrators and professionals use denial when they
are engaged in a dialogue. He suggested that there is a ‘‘double arrow’’
between perpetrators of death and clinicians and researchers: It is not only
that perpetrators ¢nd it di⁄cult to share their moral hesitations; professionals
also prefer not to ask and not to listen. The ‘‘double arrow’’ allows both the
perpetrator and the mental health professional to maintain their silence
(Bar-On, 1989; Bar-On & Charny, 1988).
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
341
Drivers who injure others evoke complex reactions. On the one hand, it is
impossible to ignore the terrible consequences of their carelessness; on the
other hand, the act occurred accidentally. It would seem that all drivers could
¢nd themselves in a similar situation, and no one is immune to being involved
in a fatal accident. In meeting a perpetrator of the Holocaust, for example,
people may think that they are ‘‘di¡erent’’ and would not have behaved like
the Nazis. But there is no such‘‘immunity’’ for reckless drivers.
Some claim that relating to perpetrators as victims is immoral. For exam-
ple, Alan Young (1995) objects to viewing a¥icted Vietnam veterans as vic-
tims. In his opinion, the victim label legitimizes the war and promotes
mobilization to help. He sees these soldiers as war criminals who committed
terrible acts. A possible result of such a claim is the delegitimization of
various traumatized populations (Witzum et al., 1996;Young, 1995) including
perpetrators such as reckless drivers.
Although the ¢ndings of this study are limited because it involved a small
sample and a retrospective design, our results point to the need to draw clin-
ical and research attention to populations at risk and in distress who have not
received appropriate professional attention as a result of social and legal
processes.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Barash, R. K. (1996). Factors associated with two facets of altruism inVietnamWar veterans
with post-traumatic stress disorder. Dissertation Abstracts International, 56(11-B), 6453.
Bar-On, D. (1989). Legacy of silence: Encounters with children of theThird Reich. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Bar-On, D. & Charny, I. W. (1988). Children of Holocaust perpetrators: How do they form
their own ‘‘moral self ’’? IsraelJournal of Psychology, 1, 29^38.
Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., & Taylor, A. E. (1991). The psychophysiology of motor
vehicle accident related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biofeedback & Self-Regulation, 16,
449^458.
Bryant, R. A. & Harvey, A. G. (1996). Initial posttraumatic stress response following motor
vehicle accidents. Journal ofTraumatic Stress, 9, 223^234.
Fatzinger, T. A. (1997). E¡ects of therapist self-disclosure on client perceptions of the ther-
apeutic alliance and session impact. Dissertation Abstracts International, 58(6-B), 3313.
Feinstein, A. & Dolan, R. (1991). Predictors of post-traumatic stress disorder following phy-
sical trauma: An examination of the stressor criterion. Psychological Medicine, 21, 85^91.
Foeckler, M. M., Garrard, F. H.,Williams, C. C.,Thomas, A. M., & Jones,T. J. (1978).Vehicle
drivers and fatal accidents. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 8, 174^182.
342
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon
Frieze, I. H., Hymer, S., & Greenberg, M. S. (1987). Describing the crime victim: Psycholo-
gical reactions to victimization. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 18, 299^315.
Gersons, B. P. R. (1989). Patterns of PTSD among police o⁄cers following shooting incidents:
A two-dimentional model and treatment implications. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2, 247^
259.
Gilbert, P. (1998). Shame and humiliation in the treatment of complex cases. In N. Tarrier
et al. (Eds.), Treating complex cases: The cognitive behavioural therapy approach (pp. 241^271).
Chichester, England: Wiley.
Glover, H. (1984). Survival guilt and the Vietnam veteran. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
orders
, 172, 393^397.
Goldberg, L. & Breznitz, S. (Eds.). (1982). Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects.,
NewYork: Free Press.
Green, B. L., Wilson, J. P., & Lindy, J. D. (1985). Conceptualizing PTSD: A psychological
framework. In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Trauma and its wake (pp. 53^69). New York:
Brunner=Mazel.
Haley, S. A. (1978). Treatment implications of post-combat stress response syndromes for
mental health professionals. In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Stress disorders among Vietnam veterans:
Theory, research and treatment
(pp. 254^267). NewYork: Brunner=Mazel.
Hendin, H. & Haas, A. P. (1984).Wounds of war:The psychological aftermath of combat inVietnam.
NewYork: Basic Books.
Jackley, P. A. (2001). Shame-based identity and chronic post-traumatic stress disorder in
help-seeking combat veterans. Dissertation Abstracts International, 61(9B), 4986.
Jano¡-Bulman, R. (1989). Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic events: Applications
of scheme construct. Social Cognition, 7, 113^136.
Jano¡-Bulman, R. & Wortman, C. B. (1977). Attributions of blame and coping in the ‘‘real
world’’: Severe accident victims react to their lot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
35
, 351^363.
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., & Hughes, M. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder
in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1048^1060.
Kinzie, D. J. (1989). Post-traumatic stress disorder. In J. I. Kaplan & D. J. Sadock (Eds.).,
Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry
. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins Co.
Kogan, I. (1990). A journey to pain. InternationalJournal of Psychological Analysis, 71, 629^639.
Koren, D., Arnon, I., & Klein, E. (1999). Acute stress response and post-traumatic stress
disorder in tra⁄c accident victims: A one-year perspective, follow up study. American
Journal of Psychiatry
, 156, 367^373.
Kubany, E. S., Abueg, F. R., Owens, J. A., Brennan, J. M., Kaplan, A. S., & Watson, S. B.
(1995). Initial examination of a multidimensional model of trauma-related guilt: Appli-
cations to combat veterans and battered women. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment
, 17, 353^376.
Kugler, K. & Jones,W. H. (1992). On conceptualizing and assessing guilt. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology
, 62, 318^327.
Lifton, R. J. (1967). Death in life:The survivors of Hiroshima. NewYork: Random House.
Lifton, R. J. (1973). Home from the war:Vietnam veterans. Neither victims nor executioners. NewYork:
Simon & Schuster.
Lobel, T. E. & Yahia, M. (1985). Guilt feelings and locus of control of concentration camp
survivors. InternationalJournal of Social Psychiatry, 153, 810^818.
PTSDAmong Reckless Drivers
343
Malt, U. (1988). The long-term psychiatric consequences of accidental injury: A longitudinal
study of 107 adults. BritishJournal of Psychiatry, 153, 810^818.
Mayou, R. (1992). Psychiatric aspects of road tra⁄c accidents. International Review of Psy-
chiatry
, 4, 45^54.
Mayou, R. A., Bryant, B., & Duthie, R. (1993). Psychiatric consequences of road tra⁄c
accidents. British MedicalJournal, 307, 647^651.
Parente, D. (2001). Implicit and explicit memory in junior high and college students. Psycho-
logical Reports
, 88, 313^315.
Parker, N. (1977). Accident litigants with neurotic symptoms. Medical Journal of Australia, 3,
318^321.
Sayag, R. (2000). Course and correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder following road accidents: Pro-
spective research
. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Tel Aviv.
Sigel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. NewYork: McGraw-Hill.
Solomon, Z. (1993). Combat stress reaction:The enduring toll of war. NewYork: Plenum Press.
Solomon, Z., Benbenishty, R., Neria,Y., Abramowitz, M., Ginzburg, K., & Ohry, A. (1993).
Assessment of PTSD: Validation of the revised PTSD Inventory. IsraelJournal of Psychiatry
and Related Sciences
, 30, 110^115.
Stratton, J. G., Parker, D. A., & Snibbe, J. R. (1986). Post-traumatic stress: Study of police
o⁄cers involved in shootings. Psychological Reports, 55, 127^131.
Street, A. E. & Arias, I. (2001). Psychological abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder in
battered women: Examining the roles of shame and guilt.Violence and Victims, 16, 65^78.
Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual di¡erences in proneness to shame and guilt:
Development of the Self-Conscious A¡ect and Attribution Inventory. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology
, 59, 102^111.
Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral a¡ect: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology
, 61, 598^607.
Tangney, J. P. (1992). Situational determinants of shame and guilt in young adulthood.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
, 18, 199^206.
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., & Gramzow, R. (1989).TheTest of Self-Conscious A¡ect. Fairfax,VA:
George Mason University.
Witzum, E., Goodman,Y., & Shalev, A. (1996). PTSD: Reality or imagination? IsraelJournal
of Psychotherapy
, 11, 19^26.
Wolman, C. B. (1973). Dictionary of behavioral science. NewYork: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Young, A. (1995). Reasons and causes for post-traumatic stress disorder. Transcultural
Psychiatric Research Review
, 32, 287^298.
Zahava Solomon, PhD, is the director of the Adler Research Center and professor of
Psychiatric epidemiology and social work at Tel Aviv University, Israel. She has pub-
lished over 200 scienti¢c articles and 6 books on man-made psychological trauma. Her
work focuses on war, captivity, the Holocaust, and terror. She is the recipient of
numerous grants and awards, including the ISTSS Laufer award for outstanding sci-
enti¢c achievement in the ¢eld of PTSD.
Tamar Lowinger works as a social worker in a psychiatric hospital and has worked
with reckless drivers inthe past. Shereceivedboth her B.A. and her M.A. fromthe School
of SocialWork atTel Aviv University, Israel.This article is based on her master’s thesis.
344
T. Lowinger and Z. Solomon