Visual assessments of the surface diusion
properties of concert halls
Fergus R. Fricke*
Department of Architectural and Design Science, University of Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia
Received 17 September 1999; received in revised form 15 October 1999; accepted 1 November 1999
Abstract
The diusion of sound in concert halls has been recognized as signi®cant in determining the
acoustic quality of halls. The diusion of sound in rooms is dicult to assess as are the dif-
fusing properties of surfaces. The collective diusing properties of the walls, ¯oor and ceiling
in a hall would seem to be almost impossible to assess objectively but such an assessment is
needed for the design of concert halls. In this paper it is shown, indirectly, that visual assess-
ments of the diusion of concert hall interiors appears to be repeatable and reproducible to a
degree which will allow halls to be designed to one of three broad classi®cations; ``excellent'',
``good'' or ``fair''. # 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
It has been suggested by Beranek [1] that there are six orthogonal acoustic prop-
erties of auditoria: IACC
E3
, t
I
, EDT, G
mid
, BR and SDI. IACC
E3
is the average
early interaural correlation coecient, t
I
is the time delay between the direct and
®rst re¯ected sound at the centre of the main seating area, EDT is the early decay
time in the hall, G
mid
is a measure of the average sound level in a hall at mid-fre-
quencies, BR is the bass ratio, i.e. the ratio of the reverberation time at low to mid
frequencies and SDI is the surface diusivity index of the hall surfaces, i.e. a mea-
sure of the diusing properties of the interior surfaces. SDI, like diusion, is a dif-
®cult quantity to estimate or measure for a particular surface and to get a
representative value for a concert hall would seem even more dicult. This paper
investigates the ecacy of using visual assessments of the acoustic diusion of sur-
faces for the assessment of concert halls.
Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
www.elsevier.com/locate/apacoust
0003-682X/00/$ - see front matter # 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S0003-682X(99)00060-2
* Tel.: +612-9351-4877; fax: +612-9351-3031.
E-mail address: ferg@arch.usyd.edu.au (F.R. Fricke).
Visual assessments of the acoustic diusion properties of surfaces in concert halls
have been made but the repeatability and reproducibility of these has not been
assessed. The paper by Haan and Fricke [2] was based on one person's assessments
of the acoustic diusion properties and there was only indirect evidence of the dif-
fusion properties of the surfaces surveyed; there was no direct linking of assessed
diusion and the actual diusion. Such a comparison would be extremely valuable but is
unlikely to occur because of the amount of work involved and the diculty in assessing
the diusion of seating areas and balconies and the interaction of the various elements
making up a concert hall.
Another approach to the evaluation was taken by Fricke and Han [3] when they
used assessed values of the level of ``discontinuities'' in surfaces in a neural network
analysis to predict the acoustic quality of concert halls. They used 10 inputs to the
neural network; room volume, surface area, maximum length, width and height, the
number of seats, the mean rake angle of the seating, the stage height, a stage enclo-
sure index and a SDI. The last two of these inputs were estimated by a group of
practicing architects from photos and drawings of the halls.
This approach [2] was an indirect way of linking the assessed surface diusion
values to acoustic quality. It does not indicate whether the visual assessments of
diusion do in fact aect acoustic diusion (nor does it help substantiate Beranek's
concert hall acoustic model as geometric inputs, rather than acoustic inputs, were
used.). Theirs was a realistic attempt to assess the importance of diusion but it
assumes, amongst other things, that visual assessments of ``discontinuities'' in surfaces
are related to the acoustic diusing properties of those surfaces. It also assumes that
the visual assessments are reproducible and that surface treatments are the main
producers of diusion and not factors like the seating, the shape of the halls and hall
reverberation characteristics. No account was taken of the patterns of asperities or
``roughness'' elements on surfaces that will in¯uence the diusion of sound. The
eect of curved surfaces on diusion was also dicult to assess.
This paper takes another approach. If visual assessments are going to work then
they must be repeatable and consistent, amongst individuals or groups which make
them. The paper investigates these aspects. It also uses surface diusion data toge-
ther with other orthogonal measures of the acoustics of auditoria [EDT, t
I
, G
mid
, BR
and (1-IACC
E3
)] (see Beranek [1], p. 517) to estimate the acoustic quality of halls
using neural network analysis. Besides being an indirect way of assessing whether
visual assessments of concert hall diusion can be used to predict the acoustic
quality of halls the neural network analysis developed should also be a valuable
semi-independent evaluation of whether Beranek's six orthogonal variables can be
used to predict the performance of halls.
2. Neural network analysis of concert hall quality using acoustic measures
There seems to be reasonable agreement on the need for acoustic measures of
auditoria and concert halls. It is recognized too that there are optimum values of
these measures but there does not appear to have been any attempt to check whether
254
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
combinations of less than optimal acoustic measures can still result in a highly
regarded concert hall. Nor does there appear to be a substantiation of how much
each of the six orthogonal measures contributes to the overall perception of the
acoustics of a hall [1,3]. Beranek suggests that the contribution of each parameter is
(1-IACC
E3
): 25%, EDT: 25%, SDI: 15%, G
mid
: 15%, t
I
: 10% and BR: 10% but
there is no compelling evidence that these percentages are correct for all combina-
tions of acoustic measures or that the contribution of each of these factors can be
summed linearly. Neural network analysis (described in texts such as [4] and [5])
provides a possible method of investigating whether the relationship between
acoustic quality and the acoustic parameters is linear or non-linear. It could also
provide a more objective assessment of the contribution of each parameter to the
overall acoustic quality. In this paper neural network analysis is used to investigate
the importance of surface diusion only.
For the neural network analysis Beranek's data was used. The inputs were as
above. The outputs were grouped as in Beranek's work (A+, A: categorized as
``excellent'', B+: categorized as ``good'' and B, C+, C: categorized as ``fair''. For
training the network the majority of halls were in the ``good'' category (18) while in
the ``excellent'' category there were seven and the ``fair'' category there were ®ve.
Two halls from each category were used for validation. Thus 24 halls were used for
training the network and six for validation. The number of halls for the training and
validation are minimal and are one of the limitations of the present work.
The neural network software, ``Brainmaker'' [6], was used with three layers of
neurons and with three neurons in the hidden layer. Training was undertaken to a
tolerance of 0.35 (this level of training tolerance is governed by the three categories
of acoustic quality that were used.) The validation resulted in the correct categor-
ization of four of the six halls. Such a result does not seem good but as the training
tolerance was large this is a reasonable result, especially as both of the ``excellent''
halls were correctly predicted. One of the two incorrectly predicted halls (Lie-
derhalle, Stuttgart) was a borderline case and a change of 10% in the assessment of
SDI resulted in the correct prediction of acoustic category (This is an important
result and will be dealt with below.). The second incorrectly predicted hall result was
for the Barbican Concert Hall in London. The neural network analysis prediction
indicated that this hall would be classed as ``good'' whereas Beranek put it in the
lowest of his three acoustic categories. While this miscategorization may be a result
of the failure of the neural network analysis it may also be an indication of limita-
tions of the six input parameter model or the assessment of the acoustic quality.
Despite the result for the Barbican Concert Hall the neural network approach seems
to support Beranek's six parameter model.
It seems then that we have a useful method of predicting the acoustic quality of
concert halls using Beranek's six acoustic quantities as inputs. This assessment can
be undertaken using Beranek's procedure or using neural network analysis, which
does not rely on weightings or on a linear summation, to obtain an acoustic eva-
luation. The neural network analysis can also be used to determine how sensitive
the analysis is to changes in SDI and hence how accurately SDI needs to be
determined.
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
255
The neural network analysis shows that:
1. Using Beranek's six acoustic parameters as inputs (and his SDI values of hall
surfaces) better concert halls are achieved with higher SDI values, as suggested
by Beranek [1], Haan [7, 8] and others.
2. That the importance of SDI varies from hall to hall. There are some halls that
are relatively insensitive to SDI change, except at very high or very low values
of SDI, while other halls show a fairly steady change over the whole range of
SDI values. For instance, the Concertgebouw, Amsterdam, and Colston Hall,
Bristol are predicted to have the highest of the three Beranek hall acoustic
ratings for 0.4<SDI<1.0. Boston Symphony Hall is only a top rating hall if
the SDI is unity. No amount of diusion would seem to improve the Royal
Albert Hall, or the Roy Thompson Hall. The DeDoelen and Costa Mesa halls
show that the amount of diusion has a more uniform eect and the halls
could fall into the lowest, middle or highest acoustic categories, depending on
the amount of surface diusion.
3. In some cases a relatively small error in assessing the SDI value could result in
a hall being ranked at the opposite end of the quality scale. For example, the
Concertgebouw would change from the highest ranking to the lowest if its SDI
changed from 0.3 to 0. As was indicated above, the Liederhalle, Stuttgart was
sensitive to a 10% change in the SDI. This is, to some extent, an artifact of
classifying halls into one of three groups and would not be as much an issue if
a continuous scale was used.
At this stage of the research there are no reliable statistics on the errors to be
expected from miscategorizing SDI values but it would seem that about 70 to 80%
of halls would be correctly categorized using an SDI which was within 0.2 and
about 90% if the SDI was within 0.1. This is of concern because it is unlikely that
two people would rank the SDI of halls in the same way even if they were given the
same instructions and same photographs. The following section looks at the possible
variations in the assessment of SDI values.
3. Surface diusion assessments
This part of the present study is concerned with comparing the assessments made
of the SDIs of dierent halls. Data on the surface diusion index was obtained from
four sources:
1. Haan's data [8].
2. Beranek's data [1].
3. Han's data [9].
4. Current work (identi®ed as Clarke/Fricke in the rest of this paper).
The SDI assessments of Haan, Han and the current study were all done using
photographs and drawings. In the cases of Haan and Clarke/Fricke this was an
independently assessed SDI using three categories; ``low,'' ``medium'' and ``high''
256
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
diusion. The walls and the ceiling of each hall were independently assessed using
these three categories and then a hall SDI was determined in ®ve categories. The ®ve
categories were ``low±low'' (low wall and ceiling diusion), ``low±medium'' (low wall
diusion and medium ceiling diusion or low ceiling diusion and medium wall
diusion), ``medium±medium'' (medium wall and ceiling diusion), ``medium±high''
(medium wall diusion and high ceiling diusion or medium ceiling diusion and
high wall diusion) and ``high±high'' (high wall and ceiling diusion). A situation
where the ceiling had ``low'' diusion and the walls had ``high'' diusion (``low±
high'') was classi®ed the same as ``medium±medium''). No weighting, based on the
area of the walls and ceiling, was undertaken. Haan [2] developed a description of
surfaces that ®tted into each of these three surface categories but his categories were
not used by Clarke/Fricke. Haan's SDI evaluations, together with those of Beranek,
Han, Clarke and Fricke are shown in Table 1.
The Clarke/Fricke assessments were undertaken independently by two people.
Clarke was an independent researcher with architectural training but with limited
expertise in the area of concert hall acoustics. She was asked to ®nd photographs
and drawings of 61 halls and, based upon those, make her judgements. She found
sucient information on 58 halls to make judgements. A year later she was asked to
reassess these halls, using the same methodology. A similar assessment procedure
was used by the author. The assessments of Clarke and Fricke are presented inde-
pendently in Table 1.
The assessment of surface diusion by Han [9,10], should be less idiosyncratic than
those of the other SDI assessors. Practicing architects in Australia and Korea were
shown photographs and drawings of halls and asked to rate the walls and the ceiling
as either ``¯at'', ``small discontinuities'', ``medium discontinuities'' or ``large dis-
continuities''. The choice of the word ``discontinuities'' was unfortunate as curved
surfaces could have been judged as ``¯at'' even though they had signi®cant diusing
qualities. As these questions, together with others on the same questionnaire, were self-
administered, there is no knowing how these questions were interpreted. For instance,
it is not known whether the respondents referred only to the surfaces or took into
account balconies, caryatids, etc. On the positive side, the four categories for each
surface gave the potential for a ®ner determination of the diusion rating for halls.
Beranek's evaluations of SDI appear to have been made visually, in a similar way to
that of Haan and Clarke/Fricke, although the details of his 11 point scale are not
known. In order to compare the dierent assessments of SDIs, Beranek's data was
grouped: 0 and 0.1 were classed as ``0'', 0.2 and 0.3 were classi®ed as ``0.3'', 0.4, 0.5 and
0.6 were classi®ed as ``0.5'', 0.7 and 0.8 were classi®ed as ``0.8'' and 0.9 and 1 were
classi®ed as ``1''. Han's scale was grouped in the same way. A correlation analysis of the
dierent SDI assessments was carried out using Statistica software [11].
4. Results
The results of the SDI assessments of concert halls are presented in Table 1. Not
every assessor has information on all the halls so that there are some blanks in the table.
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
257
Table 1
Five visual assessments of the surface diusivity indices of concert halls
a
Name of hall
SDI
FF
SC
YH
CH
LB
Festspielhaus, Salzburg, Austria
0.8
1
0.5
0.8
1
Grosser Musikvereinssaal, Vienna, Austria
0.8
1
0.3
1
1
Palais des Beaux-Arts, Brussels, Belgium
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.3
Radiohuset Studio 1, Copenhagen, Denmark
0.5
0.3
0.5
0.3
0.5
Tivoli Koncertsal, Copenhagen, Denmark
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
Carl Nielsen Hall, Odense, Denmark
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
National Concert Hall, Dublin, Eire
1
1
0.3
0.5
Berliner Philharmonie Hall, Berlin, Germany
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.8
Beethovenhalle, Bonn, Germany
0.8
0.8
0.5
0.3
Gewandhaus, Leipzig, Germany
0.5
0.5
0.5
1
Herkulessaal, Munich, Germany
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.8
Gasteig Philharmonie Hall, Munich, Germany
1
1
0.8
0.5
0.8
Liederhalle, Beethovensaal, Stuttgart, Germany
0.3
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
Frederic R. Mann Auditorium, Tel Aviv, Israel
0.5
0.3
0.5
Concertgebouw, Amsterdam, Netherlands
1
1
0.3
1
1
Concert Hall De Doelen, Rotterdam, Netherlands
1
0.8
0.5
1
1
Music Center, Utrecht, Netherlands
0.8
0.8
0.5
0.3
Berwald Hall, Stockholm, Sweden
0.8
0.8
0.3
0.5
Stadt-Casino, Basel, Switzerland
0.8
1
0.3
0.8
0.8
Grosser Tonhallesaal, Zurich, Switzerland
0.8
1
0.5
0.8
1
Colston Hall, Bristol, UK
0.3
0.3
0.5
0.3
0.3
St. David's Hall, Cardi, UK
0.8
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.5
Usher Hall, Edinburgh, UK
0.8
0.8
0.3
0.5
Philharmonic Hall, Liverpool, UK
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
Royal Festival Hall, London, UK
0.8
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
Barbican Concert Hall, London, UK
0.8
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.3
Derngate Centre, Northampton, UK
0.8
0.8
0.3
0.5
Royal Concert Hall, Nottingham, UK
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.5
Free Trade Hall, Manchester, UK
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
Lyric Theatre, Baltimore, USA
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.8
Boston Symphony Hall, Boston, USA
1
1
0.3
1
1
Orchestra Hall, Chicago, USA
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.5
Severance Hall, Cleveland, USA
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.8
0.8
Boettcher Concert Hall, Denver, CO, USA
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.3
Avery Fisher Hall, New York, USA
1
0.8
0.3
0.3
0.8
Carnegie Hall, New York, USA
0.8
0.8
0.5
0.8
Academy of Music, Philadelphia, USA
0.8
1
0.5
0.5
Eastman Theatre, Rochester, New York, USA
0.8
0.8
0.3
0.5
L. M. Davies Symphony Hall, San Francisco, USA
1
1
0.5
0.8
0.8
War Memorial Opera House, San Francisco, USA
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.5
The Grand Hall, Worcester, MA, USA
1
1
0.3
0.8
0.8
Roy Thomson Hall, Toronto, Canada
0.8
1
0.5
0.3
Concert Hall, Sydney Opera House, Australia
1
1
0.5
0.5
The Melbourne Concert Hall, Melbourne, Aust.
0.5
0.5
0.5
a
FF, Fricke's assessments; SC, Clarke's assessments; YH, the ®ve-category, modi®ed Han assessments;
CH, Haan's assessments; LB, the ®ve-category, modi®ed Beranek assessments.
258
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
The results of the correlation analysis are shown in Table 2. The correlation values were
obtained using the 20 cases that are in common to each of the SDI assessments. The
values in bold in Table 2 indicate that the correlations are signi®cant at the 5% level.
Han's results are not signi®cantly correlated with any of the other evaluations and so
it would seem that better guidelines would need to be provided if assessments of surface
diusivity were to be made by architects or members of other non-cognate professions.
Most of the other correlations are low though the majority of the assessments are
within 0.1 of each other. These low correlations are, to some extent, a result of the
way in which the results of Haan and Beranek have been modi®ed to allow com-
parisons to be made with those of Fricke and Clarke (when, for instance, Beranek's
assessments of SDIs are correlated with the modi®ed form used here the correlation
is only 0.81.).
One encouraging result was that there was no signi®cant dierence between Clar-
ke's two assessments which were separated by about a year (r=0.926). Also,
although the correlations between assessments are sometimes low any of these
assessments can still be used to make predictions of SDI, and hence of concert hall
acoustic quality, using a neural network trained on these assessed values. However,
the analysis would not be the same as that based on the SDI values of Beranek.
5. Conclusions
It seems, from the results of the neural network analysis, that Beranek's approach
to the prediction of the acoustic performance of concert halls is valid (a better way
though, of predicting the acoustic performance, may be to train a neural network).
However there are some diculties associated with it such as the variation of
quantities such as IACC, G and EDT throughout the hall, the diculty of comput-
ing some of these quantities, especially at the early stage of a design, and the
inability to assess SDI values in a reproducible way.
Visual determination of SDI seems to be the only viable method at present. If
visual determination is to be of use it must be de®nable and repeatable to within
satisfactory limits. What are these limits? From the present work it looks like SDI
must be determined to 0.1 or possibly 0.2 in order to get acceptable predictions
of acoustic quality of halls. If 0.2 is acceptable hall surfaces must be placed into
Table 2
Correlation matrix for ®ve independent assessments of the SDI of 20 halls
ab
FF
SC
YH
CH
LB
FF
1.00
0.78
ÿ0.24
0.54
0.59
SC
0.78
1.00
ÿ0.31
0.75
0.82
YH
ÿ0.24
ÿ0.31
1.00
ÿ0.41
ÿ0.32
CH
0.54
0.75
ÿ0.41
1.00
0.86
LB
0.59
0.82
ÿ0.32
0.86
1.00
a
For explanation of initials, see Table 1 footnote.
b
The signi®cant correlations are shown in bold.
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
259
one of ®ve SDI categories. This seems possible but a 10 category evaluation to achieve
0.1 does not without knowing more about how much the ceiling, walls and seating
contribute to the diusion, whether area weighted values of dierent surface SDIs
should be used and whether surfaces, partially obscured by balconies, should carry the
same weighting as those surfaces which are more exposed. In some halls it is dicult to
distinguish the walls from the ceiling (e.g. the Concert Hall of the Sydney Opera House)
while in others there are marked dierences in the ®nishes of the walls and ceiling and so
a re®nement of the visual assessment of SDI would be dicult.
The previous paragraph also indicates some of the limitations of the ®ve category
SDI evaluation used in the present work. There are other limitations such as those
introduced because dierent camera lenses and angles used as well as dierent
lighting conditions for the photos on which the evaluations were carried out. Dif-
ferent instructions given to subjects assessing surfaces are also likely to aect the
SDI evaluations. And then there are the unknown eects of factors such dierent
types of diusing elements on surfaces, their interaction and the in¯uence of factors
other than surface treatments, on the diusivity achieved in halls. All this means of
course there is plenty of scope for further work.
More work needs to undertaken on a better de®nition of what constitutes various
diusion categories. Reliable statistics on how important the errors in the assess-
ment of SDI are on the evaluation of concert hall acoustics need to be obtained.
Instructions on how to recognize small-scale diusion elements and determine their
importance need to be produced. What form should the diusion elements take, e.g.
what is the dierence in diusion properties between regular and irregular placed
elements, should also be investigated.
Whether the visual assessments of diusion have any correspondence to actual
diusion is also of concern. This needs to be investigated though for the analysis of
how good a room is acoustically this does not seem to be an important issue as the
visual assessments appear to be satisfactory if a neural network analysis is used and
halls placed into one of three acoustic categories.
It may well be that visual assessments are not the way to go but until there is a
more ``appropriate'' way it is worth persevering with the visual assessments and
trying to identify the limitations of this method.
Acknowledgements
The work of Sue Clarke, a Research Assistant in the Department of Architecture,
Planning and Allied Arts at the University of Sydney, in searching for photographic
material on halls and assessing the diusion of surfaces, is gratefully acknowledged.
References
[1] Beranek LL. Concert and opera halls: how they sound. Woodbury, NY, Acoustical Society of
America, 1996.
[2] Haan CH, Fricke FR. An evaluation of the importance of surface diusivity in concert halls.
Applied Acoustics 1997;51:53±69.
260
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
[3] Ando Y. Concert hall acoustics. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 1985.
[4] Simpson PK. Arti®cial neural systems. London: Pergamon Press, 1990.
[5] Bishop C. Neural networks for pattern recognition. Oxford: OUP, 1995.
[6] California Scienti®c Software. BrainMaker V3.1. Nevada City (http://www.calsci.com), 1994.
[7] Haan CH, Fricke FR. Surface diusivity as a measure of the acoustic quality of concert halls. In:
Proceedings of the Conference of the Australian and New Zealand Architectural Science Associa-
tion, Sydney, 1993. p. 81±90.
[8] Haan CH. Geometry as a measure of the acoustic quality of auditoria. Ph.D. Dissertation, Uni-
versity of Sydney, 1993.
[9] Han YG. Acoustic design of auditoria using neural network analysis. Master of Design Science
Dissertation, University of Sydney, 1997.
[10] Fricke FR, Han YG. A neural network analysis of concert hall acoustics. Acustica 1999;85:113±20.
[11] STATISTICA. Tulsa (OK): StatSoft Inc (http://www.statsoftinc.com), 1999.
F.R. Fricke / Applied Acoustics 60 (2000) 253±261
261