Identity and Adolescent Adjustment
Laura Ferrer-Wreder
Barry University
Aleksandra Palchuk
University of California, Davis
Senel Poyrazli
The Pennsylvania State University
Meg L. Small and Celene E. Domitrovich
The Pennsylvania State University
This article is the report of an investigation of relations among identity coherence/
identity confusion, the ego strength of competence, antisocial behavior, academic
competence, and perceptions of school environment in a sample of 574 adolescents.
The primary results of this cross-sectional study suggest significant associations be-
tween identity-related constructs and indicators of adolescent adjustment. Study im-
plications are discussed in terms of identity-related interventions.
Identity development involves making sense of, and coming to terms with, the per-
sonal and social worlds one inhabits, recognizing choices and making decisions
within contexts, and finding a sense of unity within one’s self while claiming a
place in the world (Erikson, 1968). A positive or mature identity is one that is
self-selected, unified, competent, and, in a civil society, prosocial. A prolonged
negative or immature identity leaves a young person not optimally prepared for the
future.
Rosenthal, Gurney, and Moore (1981) developed the Erikson Psychosocial
Stage Inventory (EPSI) to measure a global “sense of identity.” The EPSI identity
Identity: An International Journal of Theory
and Research, 8:95–105, 2008
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1528-3488 print/1532-706X online
DOI: 10.1080/15283480801938143
Correspondence should be addressed to Laura Ferrer-Wreder, Barry University, Department of
Psychology, 11300 N.E. 2nd Avenue, Miami Shores, FL 33161–6695. E-mail: lwreder@mail.barry.edu
subscale, which is of particular interest in the present study, offers a snapshot of
where a person perceives he or she is in terms of identity coherence/identity confu-
sion. Identity coherence is defined in this study as “a workable and internally con-
sistent sense of identity” (Schwartz, Pantin, Prado, Sullivan, & Szapocznik, 2005,
p. 394) and identity confusion is defined as a lack of clarity regarding who one is,
including roles and belief systems (see Schwartz, Mason, Pantin, & Szapocznik,
this issue).
In this study, we viewed identity coherence/identity confusion as a single di-
mension, with identity coherence and confusion at opposing ends of this dimen-
sion (e.g., Gray, Ispa, & Thornberg, 1986; Morrison, Ispa, & Thornburg, 1994).
There is disparate empirical evidence regarding the most appropriate factor struc-
ture for the EPSI identity scale (c.f., Gray et al., 1986; Morrison et al., 1994;
Schwartz et al., 2005). Because this is an empirical question, other conceptualiza-
tions of this construct may be appropriate (see Schwartz et al., this issue).
Erikson (1968) and contemporary identity researchers have placed consider-
able value on the benefits of going through a process that results in (at least tempo-
rary) the successful resolution of one’s identity crisis. The extant research litera-
ture, although small with regard to identity coherence/identity confusion as
measured by the EPSI, generally has supported this theoretical stance (e.g., Moore
& Boldero, 1991; Rosenthal, Moore, & Taylor, 1983).
Focusing more closely on the specific indicators of adjustment in the present
study, significant relations have been found in cross-sectional studies between
identity coherence/identity confusion and adolescent behavior problems. For ex-
ample, youth (predominately White early adolescents living in urban, economi-
cally disadvantaged communities) with a well-developed identity were more likely
to have a lower level of self-reported delinquent behavior than youth with a rela-
tively less developed identity (De Haan & MacDermid, 1999). A parallel associa-
tion was demonstrated in a study by Arehart and Smith (1990) who found that, in
comparison to a mainstream, ethnically diverse sample of secondary school stu-
dents, youth detained for delinquent behavior were less likely to have resolved
their psychosocial crises. Another cross-sectional study with a sample of predomi-
nately Hispanic immigrant early adolescents living in urban economically disad-
vantaged communities showed significant associations, in theoretically expected
directions, between identity confusion/identity coherence and behavior problems
(Schwartz et al., 2005).
Examining an even wider array of adolescent adjustment indicators, a handful
studies have dealt with links between identity coherence/identity confusion and
academic achievement, substance use, and sexual behavior. Results from the
aforementioned De Haan and MacDermid (1999) study indicated a significant
positive relation between identity coherence and self-reported grades. Notably, no
significant association was found between the study’s identity indices and self-re-
ported substance use (De Haan & MacDermid).
96
FERRER-WREDER ET AL.
In one of the few, if not only, longitudinal studies on the subject, Schwartz et al.
(this issue) found that as identity confusion increased over time so did the proba-
bility of cigarette and alcohol use and sexual behavior. In this study, a group of
early adolescents, who were predominately Hispanic immigrants living in urban
economically disadvantaged communities, were followed for three years. As in a
previous cross-sectional study (Schwartz et al., 2005), identity confusion was
found to partially mediate the relation between family functioning and adjustment.
In this study, we sought to illuminate the associations between identity coher-
ence/identity confusion and several indices of adolescent adjustment. We sought to
do so in an ethnically diverse sample of adolescents that included high school-aged
adolescents, thereby expanding the extant identity development literature and add-
ing to the evidence base for identity-related intervention efforts (see Archer, p. 89,
this issue; Kurtines & Montgomery, 2008). Ethnicity was not included in the study
hypotheses and analyses because the ethnic composition of the sample was pre-
dominately African American (70%) with too few participants in other ethnic
groups for contrast analyses with sufficient statistical power.
Primary hypotheses in this study were as follows: Participants with low re-
ported levels of antisocial behavior would be characterized by greater identity co-
herence, relative to those who reported higher levels of antisocial behavior (e.g.,
Arehart & Smith, 1990; De Haan & MacDermid, 1999; Schwartz et al., 2005).
Low academic competence would be associated with low identity coherence (e.g.,
De Haan & MacDermid, 1999).
An ancillary hypothesis was that low levels of the ego strength of competence
would be associated with low identity coherence because the successful resolution
of psychosocial crisis is thought to give rise to ego strengths (Erikson, 1968). The
resolution of the psychosocial, industry, and inferiority crisis in school-aged youth
establishes the ego strength of competence as the basis for mastery and skills de-
velopment and better prepares a person to successfully address his or her identity
crisis (Markstrom, Sabino, Turner, & Berman, 1997). The ego strength of compe-
tence was defined in the present study as a young person’s global evaluation of his
or her own abilities. We could not locate a published study that examined how the
ego strength of competence relates to identity coherence/identity confusion as
measured by the EPSI. We therefore felt it worthwhile to examine whether compe-
tence does indeed relate to the resolution of identity crises in expected ways.
An ancillary research question concerned possible relations between adoles-
cents’ perception of the school environment and identity coherence/identity confu-
sion and the ego strength of competence. Perception of the school environment
was defined as a construct consisting of adolescents’ views on student and teacher
respect, school quality, and students’ sense of belongingness and engagement in
their school (Mitra, 2004). Although we could not locate a published study that ex-
amined the specific relation between these constructs, we felt that this was a key
research question because of the importance of school for adolescents.
IDENTITY AND ADOLESCENT ADJUSTMENT
97
METHOD
Participants and Procedure
Over the course of three years, 6th- through 12th-grade students in an urban school
district in the northeastern United States participated annually in a student survey.
Students completed a confidential paper-and-pencil survey in either their
homeroom or English classroom, taking approximately one hour. Institutional re-
view board approval and a certificate of confidentiality were obtained for these
survey administrations. Across the three years of survey administrations, annual
participation, on average, involved 2,791 students who received parental or guard-
ian consent, assented to participate, and returned a survey (i.e., 71% average re-
sponse rate).
The analyses conducted in the present study center on two cohorts of 11th grad-
ers (N = 574) with the following demographics. The percentage of boys in the sam-
ple was 47% (n = 270) and the percentage of girls was 52% (n = 296; 1% missing, n
= 8). The average age was 16.5 years, with participants ranging in age from 15 to
19 years old. The sample was predominately African American/Black (70%, n =
399) and Latino/Hispanic (12%, n = 69), with the remainder of the sample endors-
ing one of the following ethnic identifiers: American Indian/Native American,
Asian, White/Non-Latino, or Other (16%, n = 95; 2% missing, n = 11).
Measures
Demographic survey.
Participants were asked to provide information on
their gender, age, grade, and ethnicity.
Identity.
The EPSI (Rosenthal et al., 1981) is a 72-item self-report question-
naire rated on a 5-point Likert scale. For the purposes of this study, only the iden-
tity subscale was used. On this subscale, six items are indicative of identity coher-
ence and six items represent identity confusion. A sample item for coherence is “I
know what kind of person I am”; for confusion, “I feel mixed up.” Response op-
tions ranged on a Likert scale from 1 (hardly ever true) to 5 (almost always true).
Identity confusion items were reverse coded. The total identity score (TOT-I) con-
sisted of the average of 12 items, with higher scores denoting greater identity co-
herence. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for TOT-I in this sample
was .74.
Ego strength of competence.
A subscale of the Psychological Inventory
of Ego Strengths (PIES; Markstrom et al., 1997) was used to measure the ego
strength of competence. A sample item is the following: “I have strengths that help
me to be effective in certain situations.” Response options ranged on a Likert scale
98
FERRER-WREDER ET AL.
from 1 (not descriptive of me) to 5 (very descriptive of me). The total ego strength
of competence score (TOT-ES) was comprised of the average of eight questions,
with higher scores indicating lower ego strength of competence. The Cronbach al-
pha reliability coefficient for the TOT-ES in the present sample was .68.
Antisocial behavior.
The Communities That Care Youth Survey (CTC Sur-
vey; Arthur, Pollard, Catalano, & Baglioni, 2002) taps into 19 validated indicators
of risk and 11 validated indicators of protective factors. For this study, 12 items
were selected from the CTC Survey’s scales and averaged to provide an overall in-
dex of students’ self-reported antisocial behavior. A sample item is the following:
“How many times in the past year (12 months) have you: been arrested?” Response
options ranged on a Likert scale from 0 (never) to 8 (40 or more times). Higher
scores indicated more antisocial behavior. The Cronbach alpha reliability coeffi-
cient for this subscale in the study sample was .85.
Academic competence.
The academic competence scale (derived from
Chen, Anthony, & Crum, 1999; Harter, 1982) is a six-item questionnaire rated on a
4-point Likert scale, with response options differing across items. A sample item is
the following: “Some people finish their schoolwork very quickly. Others take a
long time to finish. Do you finish your schoolwork: ‘very quickly,’ ‘pretty quickly,’
‘a bit slowly,’ or ‘very slowly’?” The total academic competence score (TOT-AC)
represents the average of the six items, with higher scores denoting lower aca-
demic competence. Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient for the TOT-AC in the
present sample was .58.
Perceptions of school environment.
Four subscales from the Student
Voice Survey (Mitra, 2004) were used to measure participants’ perceptions of their
school environment. The items (e.g., “students help to make important school de-
cisions”; “this school is always trying to improve”) were answered with a Likert
scale, ranging from 0 (disagree a lot) to 3 (agree a lot). The total school environ-
ment score (TOT-ENV) represents the average of four subscales, with higher
scores indicating more favorable perceptions of the school environment.
Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for these subscales in this study sample
ranged from .56 to .78.
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
Dummy coding.
Adolescent responses to the CTC Survey’s antisocial be-
havior subscale have been characterized by limited variability in past statewide ad-
IDENTITY AND ADOLESCENT ADJUSTMENT
99
ministrations of this instrument. Therefore, we conceptualized antisocial behavior
(Ab) as a categorical, dummy variable created by using a dummy coding proce-
dure. Participants with less than .49 average antisocial behaviors were classified as
reporting low levels of Ab and those with .5 and higher average reported antisocial
acts were considered as reporting elevated levels of Ab.
Outliers.
Using non-model-based techniques, data for the continuous vari-
ables were evaluated for multivariate outliers by examining leverage indices for
each individual and defining an outlier as a leverage score four times greater than
the mean leverage score. Seven outliers were detected. There were no coding er-
rors and the outliers proved to be inconsequential for the analysis. All major con-
clusions remained intact when they were omitted from the analysis. The analysis
reported here therefore includes these outlier cases.
Non-model-based outliers were detected using a limited information approach
in which a single indicator for a given endogenous variable was regressed onto a
single indicator of all variables that directly influenced that variable in the model.
The endogenous variable was regressed onto the predictor variables and the stan-
dardized df beta coefficients were examined for each individual (e.g., outlier = ab-
solute standardized df beta coefficient greater than 1).
Nonnormality.
Multivariate normality was evaluated using statistical meth-
ods in AMOS 5.0 based on Mardia’s test for multivariate normality (Mardia,
1970). The multivariate kurtosis score was 5.74 (significant). An examination of
univariate indices of skewness and kurtosis for all continuous study variables re-
vealed no skewness or kurtosis values above an absolute value of 2.0.
Missing data.
Listwise deletion was used for the 8% of the sample that had
missing data on at least one study variable.
Analyses for the Study Hypotheses and Research Question
The fit of the model in Figure 1 was evaluated with AMOS 5.0 (Arbuckle &
Wothke, 2004) using the sample covariance matrix as input and a maximum likeli-
hood solution. Criteria for good model fit for global fit indices were: a comparative
fit index (CFI) of .95 or greater, a root mean square error (RMSEA) value less than
.08, a p value for close fit test greater than .05, a goodness of fit index of .90 or
greater, and a standardized root mean residual value less than .05. Criteria for good
model fit for focused fit indices were: modification indices less than 10 and stan-
dardized residual values less than 2.
In the first model tested, causal
1
paths were defined from exogenous variables
to endogenous variables based on the study hypotheses and research question. Ex-
100
FERRER-WREDER ET AL.
1
The term causal path is used strictly in the statistical sense.
101
FIGURE
1
Structural
equation
model
examining
relationships
among
identity
coherence/identity
confusion,
the
eg
o
strength
of
competence,
antiso-
cial
beha
vior
,academic
competence,
and
perception
of
school
en
vironment.
Note:
+
=
Exogenous
v
ariables
are
assumed
to
be
correlated;
*
p
<
.05;
**
p
<
.01;
Ñ
this
path
represents
a
mean
dif
ference
=
.142,
estimated
standard
error
=
.063,
p
<
.05;
a
=
for
ev
ery
2
Lik
ert
scale
point
increase
on
the
academic
competence
scale
(i.e.,
high
numbers
or
increases
are
indicati
v
e
of
lo
wer
academic
competence),
identity
coherence
w
as
predicted
to
decrease
by
.30
Lik
ert
scale
points
;b
=
for
ev
ery
2
Lik
ert
scale
point
increase
on
the
eg
o
strength
of
competence
scale
(higher
numbers
or
increases
are
indicati
v
e
of
lo
wer
eg
o
strength),
identity
coherence
is
predicted
to
decrease
by
.84
Lik
ert
scale
points
;
c
=
for
ev
ery
2
Lik
ert
scale
point
increase
on
the
academic
compe-
tence
scale
(higher
numbers
or
increases
are
indicati
v
e
of
lo
wer
academic
competence),
eg
o
strength
of
competence
is
predicted
to
increase
(higher
n
um-
bers
or
increases
are
indicati
v
e
of
lo
wer
eg
o
strength)
by
1.22
Lik
ert
scale
points.
ogenous variables were assumed to be correlated. Global fit indices for this model
indicated bad fit. Modification indices showed that fit could be improved if a
causal path was defined from academic competence to the ego strength of compe-
tence. Theoretically, such a connection was reasonable and a causal path was de-
fined between these variables. The revised model was reanalyzed.
For the final model, a variety of indices of model fit were evaluated. The
chi-square test of model fit was statistically nonsignificant (
c
2
= 2.687(3), p <
0.442). The RMSEA was 0.000 with a 90% confidence interval of .000 to 0.068.
The p value for the test of close fit was 0.843. The CFI was 1.00 and the goodness
of fit index was 0.998. The standardized root mean square residual was 0.0174. All
indices pointed toward good model fit. Inspection of the residuals and modifica-
tion indices revealed no significant points of ill fit in the model. Figure 1 presents
the final model with relevant standardized and unstandardized (in parentheses) pa-
rameter estimates.
DISCUSSION
This study was designed to contribute to the knowledge base on how identity co-
herence/identity confusion relates to adolescent adjustment. Only a handful of re-
searchers have examined the same or similar phenomena in either predominately
African American, Latino/Hispanic, or both adolescent samples. The present
study makes a significant addition to this literature by investigating these links in a
sample of middle to late adolescents who were predominately African American
and Latino/Hispanic. This study also widens the generalizability of previous re-
search findings with other samples.
The main findings of this study reaffirm previous research that has shown bene-
fits associated with identity coherence or, conversely, risks connected to identity
confusion. Specifically, for the first primary study hypothesis, participants with
lower levels of antisocial behavior had higher levels of identity coherence in com-
parison to those with more elevated antisocial behavior. This finding is consistent
with previous research (e.g., Arehart & Smith, 1990; De Haan & MacDermid,
1999; Schwartz et al., 2005). We also found that, as academic competence de-
creased, so did identity coherence (e.g., De Haan & MacDermid, 1999).
Expanding the conceptual aspect of the model beyond the identity coherence/
identity confusion–adjustment connection, a theory-based hypothesis was pro-
posed (Erikson, 1968); namely, that low levels of the ego strength of competence
would be associated with low identity coherence. This proposition was supported
by parameter estimates which suggested that, for every unit ego strength of compe-
tence decreased, identity coherence also decreased by approximately half a unit.
Interestingly, parameter estimates indicated that as academic competence de-
creased, so did predicted levels of the ego strength of competence. It is notable that
102
FERRER-WREDER ET AL.
academic competence appears to be connected to both identity coherence/identity
confusion and the ego strength of competence. Previously published research has
not identified these specific relations. Yet, such relations are consistent with theory
(Erikson, 1968).
The study findings provided no support for a significant association between
adolescents’ perception of the school environment and identity coherence/identity
confusion and the ego strength of competence. Fit indices, modification indices,
and standardized residual values in the initial and final model did not call for
causal paths to be defined among these variables. Despite the lack of significance
for this variable in the present study, future research should continue to explore re-
lations between contextual variables and indices of identity development (see
Schwartz et al., this issue). Certainly, the total percentage of variance accounted
for in identity coherence/identity confusion and the ego strength of competence in
the final model highlights the need to explore more complex models that include
other key variables.
It is notable that identity-related variables were connected to academic com-
petence, but not adolescent perceptions of the school environment. Future re-
search should closely examine which kinds of school environments or experi-
ences are associated with positive identity development. Qualitative research
exploring potential connections between the meaning of school and the daily
experience of the school and community environments in relation to identity
development may prove important for the next generation of identity-related
interventions.
The study design was cross-sectional. Therefore, causality cannot be assumed
for any of the identified associations. Longitudinal and intervention studies should
continue to be conducted to provide more insight into the relations identified in
this study. An additional limitation was that this study was not, at the outset, de-
signed in a way that lent itself to the ideal use of structural equation modeling
techniques.
Further, the total percentage of variance accounted for in the identity coher-
ence/identity confusion and the ego strength of competence variables suggests that
more variance could be accounted for by exploring increasingly complex models
that include other potentially key latent constructs. Despite its limitations, this
study contributes to what is known about identity coherence/identity confusion
and adolescent adjustment. This and other salient descriptive studies have rele-
vance to the intervention field (see Archer, this issue).
Life span developmental research should be used systematically as a founda-
tion for intervention efforts. Interventions can also further our understanding of
fundamental developmental processes (Kurtines et al., in press). There is a broad
historical evidence base that has linked several indicators of identity development
to adjustment. In addition to this wider literature, a small but growing number of
descriptive studies also have pointed to key connections between identity coher-
IDENTITY AND ADOLESCENT ADJUSTMENT
103
ence/identity confusion and multiple adolescent problem behaviors and positive
youth development in ethnically diverse participant samples.
How can we make use of identity coherence/identity confusion or the ego
strength of competence constructs in an intervention framework? These constructs
represent potentially valuable proxy markers that offer an indication of how
psychosocial development is proceeding at a given point in time. Behind these
proxies are the mechanisms of identity development and other coinciding lines of
human development at work. Research should continue to explore and develop
ways to influence the mechanisms of identity development and examine how po-
tential proxy markers relate to these mechanisms. However, what is clear from the
present study and other related research is that there are direct and significant con-
nections between identity development and adolescent adjustment. Also, when we
advocate ameliorating multiple adolescent problem behaviors and promoting posi-
tive youth development, attention to adolescent identity development can inform
these efforts in a meaningful—and perhaps necessary—way.
REFERENCES
Arbuckle, J., & Wothke, W. (2004). AMOS release 5.0 [Computer software]. Chicago: Small Waters.
Arehart, D. M., & Smith, P. H. (1990). Identity in adolescence: Influences of dysfunction and
psychosocial task issues. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 63–72.
Arthur, M. W., Pollard, J. A., Catalano, R. F., & Baglioni, A. J. (2002). Measuring risk and protective
factors for substance use, delinquency, and other adolescent problem behaviors—The Communities
That Care Youth Survey. Evaluation Review, 26, 575–601.
Chen, L. S., Anthony, J. C., & Crum, R. M. (1999). Perceived cognitive competence, depressive symp-
toms, and the incidence of alcohol-related problems in urban school children. Journal of Child and
Adolescent Substance Abuse, 8, 37–53.
De Haan, L. G., & MacDermid, S. M. (1999). Identity development as a mediating factor between ur-
ban poverty and behavioral outcomes for junior high school students. Journal of Family and Eco-
nomic Issues, 20, 123–148.
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
Gray, M. M., Ispa, J. M., & Thornburg, K. R. (1986). Erikson Psychosocial Stage Inventory: A factor
analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46, 979–983.
Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child Development, 53, 87–97.
Kurtines, W. M., Ferrer-Wreder, L., Berman, S. L., Cass Lorente, L., Silverman, W. K., & Montgomery,
M. J. (in press). Introduction to special issue on promoting positive youth development: New direc-
tions in developmental theory, methods, and research. Journal of Research on Adolescence.
Kurtines, W. & Montgomery (2008). (Eds.). Promoting positive youth development: New directions in
developmental theory, methods, and research. Journal of Adolescent Research, 23, 1–130.
Mardia, K. V. (1970). Measures of multivariate skewness and kurtosis with applications. Biometrika,
57, 519–530.
Markstrom, C. A., Sabino, V. M., Turner, B., & Berman, B. C. (1997). The psychosocial inventory of
ego strength: Development and validation of a new Eriksonian measure. Journal of Youth and Ado-
lescence, 26, 705–732.
104
FERRER-WREDER ET AL.
Mitra, D. L. (2004). The significance of students: Can increasing “student voice” in schools lead to
gains in youth development? Teachers College Record, 106, 651–688.
Moore, S., & Boldero, J. (1991). Psychosocial development and friendship functions in adolescence.
Sex Roles, 25, 521–536.
Morrison, J. W., Ispa, J. M., & Thornburg, K. R. (1994). African American college students’
psychosocial development as related to care arrangements during infancy. Journal of Black Psychol-
ogy, 20, 418–429.
Rosenthal, D. A., Gurney, R. M., & Moore, S. M. (1981). From trust to intimacy: A new inventory for
examining Erikson’s stages of psychosocial development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 10,
525–537.
Rosenthal, D. A., Moore, S. M., & Taylor, M. J. (1983). Ethnicity and adjustment: A study of the
self-image of Anglo-, Greek-, and Italian-Australian adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
12, 117–135.
Schwartz, S. J., Pantin, H., Prado, G., Sullivan, S., & Szapocznik, J. (2005). Family functioning, iden-
tity, and problem behavior in Hispanic immigrant early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence,
25, 392–420.
IDENTITY AND ADOLESCENT ADJUSTMENT
105