Accepted Manuscript
Title: Higher testosterone levels are associated with unfaithful
behavior in men
Authors: C. Klimas, U. Ehlert, T.J. Lacker, P. Waldvogel, A.
Walther
PII:
S0301-0511(18)30700-2
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107730
Article Number:
107730
Reference:
BIOPSY 107730
To appear in:
Received date:
15 September 2018
Revised date:
22 May 2019
Accepted date:
10 July 2019
Please cite this article as: Klimas C, Ehlert U, Lacker TJ, Waldvogel P, Walther A,
Higher testosterone levels are associated with unfaithful behavior in men, Biological
Psychology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107730
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication.
As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript.
The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof
before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process
errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that
apply to the journal pertain.
1
Article Type:
Original research
Higher testosterone levels are associated with unfaithful
behavior in men
Running Head:
Testosterone levels in unfaithful men
Klimas, C.
1
, Ehlert, U.
2,3
, Lacker, T.J.
2,3
Waldvogel, P.
2
& Walther, A.
1,2,3
1
Biological Psychology, TU Dresden, Dresden, Germany
2
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
3
Dynamics of Healthy Aging, University Research Priority Program, University of Zurich, Zurich,
Switzerland
Submitted to Journal:
Biological Psychology
Correspondence:
Dr. Andreas Walther
andreas.walther@tu-dresden.de
Highlights
A robust relation between higher testosterone and increased unfaithful
behavior was observed.
Infidelity was measured using direct and sensitive indirect (crosswise)
questioning.
In this sample of men aged between 40 and 75 years, 37.5% men
answered having been unfaithful in the current relationship
Subsample analysis indicates a positive association between testosterone
and infidelity to be present primarily in men without sexual dysfunction.
Abstract
Background: Infidelity in romantic relationships is a common, but severe issue often causing
breakup and severe psychological impairment. Higher levels of testosterone are related to
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
2
mating-behavior, sexual desire, and infidelity in men with sexual dysfunctions. Previous
studies, have insufficiently addressed the potential role of testosterone in infidelity in healthy
men.
Methods: A sample of 224 middle-aged self-reporting healthy men being currently in a
relationship completed questionnaires on relationship characteristics, infidelity, and provided
overnight-fasting saliva samples for testosterone quantification.
Results: In the sample, 37.5% men answered having been unfaithful in the current relationship,
while 29% were identified as fulfilling criteria for a sexual dysfunction. Adjusting for
covariates, a significant positive association for the frequency of unfaithful behavior and
testosterone levels emerged. Subsample analysis indicates a positive association between
testosterone and infidelity only to be present in men without sexual dysfunction.
Conclusion: Unfaithful behavior in males is associated with higher testosterone levels.
Key words:
Infidelity; testosterone; unfaithful behavior; sexual activity; sexuality; men; relationship
satisfaction; sexual dysfunction
Introduction
Infidelity is a severe problem in society and may lead to individual complaints like depression,
psychosomatic problems (Plack et al., 2008) and even symptoms of post-traumatic stress
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
3
disorder (Gordon et al., 2004). Couples who had experienced infidelity before show less
relationship satisfaction and experience greater relationship problems than couples without
history of extrapair sexual activities (Plack et al., 2008). Therefore, infidelity threatens the
continuation of a relationship (Plack et al., 2008), making it one of the primary causes of divorce
(Amato and Previti, 2003; Scott et al., 2013).
Several studies have concluded that unfaithful behavior is more common in men
compared to women (Allen & Baucom, 2004; Haversath & Kröger, 2014; Laumann, Michael,
Gagnon, & Michaels, 1995; Wiederman, 1997). While some authors have identified trends for
infidelity rates becoming increasingly similar over time (Petersen & Hyde, 2010), others
observed higher rates in women compared to men, when applying a broad construct of
unfaithful behavior, which additionally accounted for kissing or romantic involvement, without
the necessity of sexual intercourse (Brand, Markey, Mills, & Hodges, 2007). However, when
examining exclusively sexual infidelity in large cohort studies, up to 15% of women, and 25%
of men admitted having had at least one sexual extradyadic activity in the current relationship
(Laumann, 1994; Wiederman, 1997). In a recently examined German sample, lifetime
prevalence of sexual infidelity even yielded 49% for heterosexual men and 29% for
heterosexual women, while the substantial gender difference was also present for homosexual
men (34%) and homosexual women (4%) (Haversath & Kröger, 2014).
From an evolutionary point of view, gender dissimilarities within the tendency of
extrapair sexual intercourse can be explained through the reproductive benefits for men when
having various short-term matings, potentially leading to a higher number of offspring
(Symons, 1979). This is supported by the fact, that men’s sperm is plentiful due to generation
of millions per day, which leads to high libido and enables the insemination of many females
(Buss & Schmitt, 1993). It was also shown that the sex hormone testosterone facilitates mating-
related behavior in terms of competitive behavior towards other males (Anestis, 2006; Gould
& Ziegler, 2007). Considering that men have on average six-fold higher testosterone levels than
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
4
women (Clifton et al., 2016), and show considerably higher rates of unfaithful behavior than
women, higher testosterone levels might present an underlying biological parameter related to
more extradyadic sexual activities.
Supporting the mating effort hypothesis, research suggests that men who are involved
in a romantic relationship show 21% lower testosterone levels than single men (Burnham et al.,
2003), while fatherhood additionally lowers endogenous testosterone levels (Gettler et al.,
2011; Perini et al., 2012). Intriguingly, comparable effects were found in women (Barrett et
al., 2013; Kuzawa, Gettler, Huang, & McDade, 2010; van Anders, Hamilton, & Watson, 2007).
Men experiencing a reduced testosterone decline during the transition to fatherhood show
higher sexual activity at 4.5-year follow-up (Gettler, McDade, Agustin, Feranil, & Kuzawa,
2013), while higher testosterone in the phase of early fatherhood is associated with increased
perceived constraint due to fatherhood (Waldvogel & Ehlert, 2017). Interestingly, paired men
who still express great interest in extrapair sexual activities have higher testosterone levels than
those who see their relationship as exclusive (Edelstein, Chopik, & Kean, 2011; McIntyre et
al., 2006).
In previous studies, elevated testosterone levels in men and also women were associated
with higher sex drive in terms of seeking more frequent sexual encounters (Edelstein et al.,
2011; van Anders et al., 2007). For instance, polyamorous individuals show higher levels of
testosterone than monogamously partnered individuals (Alvergne, Faurie, & Raymond, 2009;
van Anders et al., 2007). In line with this, testosterone was shown to be positively associated
with sexual desire (Anderson, Bancroft, & Wu, 1992; Edelstein et al., 2011; McIntyre et al.,
2006).
Moreover, men suffering from hypogonadism show reduced libido and after exogenous
testosterone administration libido levels are significantly increased (e.g. Brock et al., 2016;
Corona et al., 2016; Cunningham et al., 2016, Wu et al., 2016), while the libido increasing
effect of testosterone has also been observed for men with normal testosterone levels (Anderson
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
5
et al., 1992). Further, testosterone is positively linked to ejaculatory reflex (Corona et al., 2008)
and negatively associated with erectile dysfunction (Bandini et al., 2009; Walther et al., 2017).
Likewise, testosterone patches were shown to increase libido in women with hypoactive sexual
desire disorder (Braunstein et al., 2006). Thus, it has been shown that parenthood, sexual desire,
or relationship types are consistently associated with testosterone levels in males, while similar
associations in females are discussed. Besides this, testosterone administration studies
consistently lead to increased libido and sexual activity (Barrett et al., 2013; Braunstein et al.,
2006; Kuzawa et al., 2010; van Anders, Hamilton, & Watson, 2007).
Furthermore, research on personality traits and androgenization indicated positive
associations of testosterone with histrionic behavior like seductivity, self-centeredness and
sensation seeking (Bandini et al., 2009; Perini et al., 2012). Therefore, it can be assumed that
individuals with higher testosterone levels are more prone to extrapair sexual activities, as they
might easier experience monotony in monogamous relationships, but enjoy seduction of and
admiration by others.
Prior studies showed a positive association between testosterone and promiscuity, but
fell short for measuring infidelity with sufficient content validity (Edelstein et al., 2011;
McIntyre et al., 2006). In the study by Edelstein et al. (2011) examining 132 male and 122
female undergraduate students aged between 18 to 37 years, the infidelity measure consisted of
a subscale of a questionnaire measuring general promiscuity and degree of unrestricted sexual
orientation, while not explicitly asking about infidelity in the current relationship. McIntyre et
al. (2006) directly asked male undergraduate students about their history of extrapair sex, but
subsequently integrated this single item in the broader composite extrapair sexual interest
capturing also participants’ mere willingness to have extrapair sex. Fisher and colleagues
(2009) examining 2,592 heterosexual men reported significantly higher testosterone levels in
unfaithful men compared to faithful men. However, in this study, the sample consisted
exclusively of outpatients with sexual dysfunction, which has previously been shown to be
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
6
negatively related to testosterone levels suggesting overall lower testosterone levels in this
sample than in the general male population. Unfaithful behavior was further identified
employing a face-to-face interview potentially increasing the risk of social desirability bias and
reducing infidelity reports. This is further supported by the relatively low infidelity rate of 17%
identified for this sample.
We therefore hypothesize that higher testosterone levels are associated with more
frequent unfaithful behavior and that the group of men reporting having engaged in extradyadic
sexual activities, shows higher testosterone levels compared to men having never engaged in
extradyadic sexual activities. In order to replicate and extend previous results, the current study
is the first to examine the relation between testosterone and infidelity in healthy men and men
identified as having a sexual dysfunction in parallel as well as including both hetero- and
homosexual men. Furthermore, we tested for infidelity through direct and indirect questioning
using for indirect questioning the cross-wise model – a questionnaire technique, which is
advantageous over anonymous self-reports to reduce risk of socially desirable answers –
thereby increasing the probability of a true and representative report of unfaithful behavior.
Methods
Participants
The examined sample represents a subsample of a larger project on men's health described in
detail elsewhere (Walther, Phillip, Lozza, & Ehlert, 2016). Of 224 men fulfilling the criterion
of being in a romantic relationship recruited via online and newspaper announcements for a
study on healthy aging, complete data on measures of infidelity and testosterone levels was
available. Since recent studies show no differences between homo- and heterosexual men with
regard to sex hormones (Juster et al., 2016), men of different sexual orientation were included
in the sample. The final sample comprised of 212 self-reporting (94.6%) heterosexual, 2 (0.9%)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
7
homosexual, 8 (3.6%) bisexual, and 2 (0.9%) asexual men. Sample characteristics for the 224
men aged between 40 and 75 years are represented in Table 1.
Measures and Procedures
After registering for the online platform and providing informed consent, participants
independently completed online psychometric test batteries one week prior to biological
examination. The psychometric test battery included sociodemographic information, the
International Index for Erectile Function (IIEF; Wiltink, Hauck, Phädayanon, Weidner, &
Beutel, 2003), the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS; Sander & Böcker, 1993), the Brief
Symptom Inventory subscale on depressive symptoms (Zabora et al., 2001), the German
version of the Short Form Health Survey (SF36; Jenkinson et al., 1993), questions on alcohol
drinking habits, and questions on relationship characteristics. Further, we directly asked the
highly delicate question, whether and how often participants had ever engaged in extradyadic
sexual activities without agreement of the current partner with the following response options
“never”, “seldom”, “occasionally” and “regularly”. In the following, we classified participants
having responded either “seldom”, “occasionally”, and “regularly” to this question as
“unfaithful” and those having responded “never” as “faithful”.
We also used the Crosswise Model (Yu, Tian, & Tang, 2008), a sensitive indirect
questioning technique in order to reduce social desirability bias by providing an anonymous
response format (Jann, Jerke, & Krumpal, 2012), when assessing unfaithful behavior in the
past. Within the cross-wise model, participants are asked two simple yes-or-no-questions, a
non-sensitive one and a sensitive one. Participants then indicate whether the answer is “yes” to
both questions (option A), “no” to both questions (option A) or whether only one of the two
answers is true (option B). When looking at the responses of a single subject, the investigator
cannot conclude which of the two questions was affirmed or denied by the participant. Thus,
the Crosswise Model allows for participants´ anonymity and increases the likelihood of
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
8
admitting norm violations. By considering the observed marginal distributions within the
overall sample, the proportion of subjects, who has responded with “yes” to the sensitive
question, can be estimated. Therefore, it is important that the probability of a potential yes-
answer to the non-sensitive question is known. In the present study participants were asked the
non-sensitive question in the first place: “Is your mother´s birthday in January or February?”.
The probability, that participants responded positively to the question was
2
12
= 0.167. The
sensitive question was asked in order to reveal unfaithful behavior of the participants:
“Considering your current relationship, have you ever had sexual contacts to a person outside
of this partnership without prior agreement of your partner?”. Subjects then provided a joint
answer for both questions. They chose option A when responses were identical and option B if
the answers differed. The proportion of unfaithful behavior was computed using the proposed
formula by Jann and colleagues (2012) shown below, with the unknown prevalence for
infidelity
, the observed proportion of respondents choosing answer option A,
𝜙̂, and the
known prevalence of a yes-answer to the non-sensitive question, p, in our study p = 0.167.
𝜋
̂
=
(𝜙̂ + 𝑝 − 1)
(2𝑝 − 1)
Procedures of testosterone sampling and quantification are described in detail elsewhere
(Walther et al., 2016). In brief, participants arrived at the biological examination after over-
night fasting and underwent different biological examinations (e.g. height and weight
measurement for the calculation of the body mass index). Saliva was sampled in Salicaps at
8:00 am under standardized conditions and testosterone was quantified by enzyme linked
immunosorbent assay in the biochemical laboratories of the Department of Psychology of the
University of Zurich. For testosterone, intra- and inter-assay coefficients were below 5% and
10%, respectively, while sensitivity was 1.8 pg/ml.
Statistical analyses
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
9
To identify confounding variables being significantly different between the two groups
(unfaithful vs. faithful) two-tailed T-tests or χ
2
-tests were computed. To examine the relation
between the frequency of unfaithful behavior and testosterone levels one-tailed zero-order
bivariate correlations were calculated. Subsequently, analyses were repeated using one-tailed
partial correlations entering variables differing between the two groups or variables known to
influence salivary testosterone levels as covariates. Multiple linear regression analysis was
performed, predicting frequency of infidelity by testosterone levels and respective covariates.
Bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals were reported on a 95%-level (BCa CI
95%
).
Binary logistic regression modeling was applied to predict infidelity as dichotomous variable
by testosterone levels and respective covariates. Analyses have been conducted with IBM SPSS
Statistics, Version 24.0. (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.) significance level was set at alpha = .05.
Confounders
By using T-tests or χ
2
-tests, potentially differing variables between the self-reporting faithful
and the unfaithful group were examined (see Table 1). Identified potential confounders were
age, sexual function (IIEF), relationship satisfaction (RAS), depressive symptoms (BSI-18-D),
and alcohol consumption. Variables such as relationship duration or BMI were not significantly
different between groups.
Subsequently, a theoretical approach was used to further include potential variables
associated with salivary testosterone secretion. To control for the well-described effects of
general health, body composition, tobacco, recent sexual activity, or different medication types
on salivary testosterone secretion (Dai, Gutai, Kuller, & Cauley, 1988; English et al., 2001;
Walther & Ehlert, 2015), SF36 item 1 (How would you describe your general health status?
With answer options from “poor” to “excellent”), BMI, tobacco consumption, sexual activity
within the last 12 hours, and medication intake were included as covariates. To control for a
potential confounding effect of education, the highest degree of education ranging between “did
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
10
not finish regular school” to “tertiary education” was included as covariate. To account for the
sampling method using saliva to extract testosterone, the potential contamination with blood by
gingival bleeding in the last days was controlled for (Kivlighan, Granger, & Schwartz, 2005).
Finally, to control for sleep restriction and sampling time variations due to participant delay to
examination appointments (Leproult, 2011), waking time of the participant and starting time of
saliva sampling were included as covariates.
Results
224 healthy men, who stated to be married or in a relationship at time of data collection, were
included in the analysis. Participants’ age ranged from 40 to 75 years (M = 58.05, SD = 10.69),
and mean relationship duration was 21.49 years (SD = 16.02). 84 participants (37.5%) reported
having engaged in extradyadic sexual activities one or more times without consent of the
current partner during their present relationship. When applying the crosswise model, 128 out
of 220 respondents (four participants did not agree to answer this specific question) chose
answer option A (
𝜙̂ = 0.58). After having applied the formula for estimated prevalence of
infidelity, results yielded a percentage of 37.7%, thereby verifying participants’ statements
about their sexual extrapair activities (see Table 2). By using the IIEF cut-off score for the
identification of sexual dysfunction as recommended by Wiltink et al. (2003), a total of 29%
men were identified as fulfilling criteria for at least a mild form of sexual dysfunction. Salivary
testosterone concentration ranged from 7.7 to 163.1 pg/ml (M = 66.42, SD = 26.37) indicating
comparable levels with other large male cohorts in the same age range reporting salivary
testosterone levels (Clifton et al., 2016).
One-tailed zero-order bivariate correlation between the frequency of unfaithful behavior
and testosterone levels yielded a positive trend (r = 0.095, p = .079). Subsequently conducted
partial correlation analyses, including identified covariates, revealed a significant positive
association between the frequency of unfaithful behavior and testosterone levels (r
p
= .14, BCa
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
11
CI
95%
[.012, .280], p = .022). The calculated effect size for Cohen’s d = .28 suggests a small to
moderate effect (Kelley & Preacher, 2012). Point-biserial correlations between testosterone and
the dichotomous variable for infidelity (faithful = 0, unfaithful = 1) revealed a trend for a
positive association (r = .11, p = .057), indicating higher testosterone levels in unfaithful men.
This association became significant after controlling for respective covariates (r
p
= .17, BCa
CI
95%
[.030, .336], p = .007).
Multiple regression analyses were calculated, predicting frequency of extrapair sexual
activities through testosterone and respective covariates. Using the enter method it was
observed that the model explained a significant amount of variance in the frequency of
unfaithful behavior, F
15,207
= 2.11, p = .011, R
2
= .13, R
2
adjusted
= .07. The effect size of f
2
= .27
indicated a medium to large effect (Cohen, 1992). Testosterone significantly predicted the
frequency of extrapair sexual activities (β = .15, p = .045). The covariates alcohol consumption
(β = .17, p = .014), depression (β = .16, p = .037) and waking time (β = .14, p = .037) emerged
as additional significant covariates within the model. Further results as well as bias corrected
and accelerated confidence intervals are presented in table 3.
Results of the binary logistic regression analyses revealed that testosterone (b = .016, p
= .015) significantly predicted, whether participants were faithful or not, with higher
testosterone levels in unfaithful men. Also, alcohol consumption (b = .48; p = .001) and
depression (b = .48; p = .001) emerged as significant predictors.
Subsample analysis examining only men with identified sexual dysfunction and men
without sexual dysfunction is presented in the supplementary material. Although the reduction
of the sample sizes to 65 men (with sexual dysfunction) and 159 men (without sexual
dysfunction) leads to underpowered analyses, it emerges that healthy men are primarily
responsible for the significant positive association between testosterone and infidelity in the
entire sample (see supplementary material table 1a,b for correlational analysis as well as tables
2-5 for regression models).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
12
Discussion
Analyses revealed a positive association between testosterone levels and the frequency of
unfaithful behavior as well as infidelity operationalized as dichotomous variable (0 = not
unfaithful, 1 = unfaithful) in a self-reporting healthy sample of 224 hetero- und homosexual
men. This finding is line with previous research identifying for a large sample of 2,592 men
significantly higher testosterone levels in the 337 men reporting to be unfaithful (Fisher et al.,
2009). However, the entire sample by Fisher and colleagues (2009) consisted of Italian
heterosexual male outpatients with sexual dysfunctions, who had consulted a clinic for
andrology, which probably led to a selection bias towards a rather unhealthy sample since male
sexual dysfunction, particularly erectile dysfunction, has consistently been related to worse
general health (Guo et al., 2010). The here presented sample, by contrast, consisted only of self-
reporting healthy men, while it needs to be mentioned that also in the present sample 29% of
men were identified to have at least a mild form of a sexual dysfunction. Yet, this is a low
percentage for men in this age-group (Wagle, Carrejo, & Tan, 2012). Moreover, in the Fisher
et al. (2009) study unfaithful behavior was assessed through a face-to-face interview, which did
not allow for participants’ anonymity and probably led to an underestimation of infidelity in
the sample with an infidelity rate of 17%. By contrast, our analysis revealed sexual extrapair
activities in 37.7% for the current relationship (mean relationship duration 22 years), which is
more consistent with a recent anonymous internet survey in Germany reporting for heterosexual
and homosexual men a lifetime infidelity rate of 49% and 34%, respectively. Therefore, this
study is the first to report in a sample of self-reporting healthy men that increased testosterone
levels are associated with infidelity, while subsample analysis further indicates that this
association is driven by the portion of men of this sample, which were not suffering from any
sexual dysfunction (see supplementary material).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
13
Other studies also reported an association of testosterone and infidelity, while the
operationalized variable was often not infidelity but a related construct. For example, Edelstein
et al. (2011) assessed unfaithful behavior via the behavioral subscale of the Sociosexual
Orientation Inventory (SOI; Simpson and Gangestad, 1991), originally conceptualized for
measuring general promiscuity and degree of unrestricted sexual orientation (Penke &
Asendorpf, 2008). McIntyre and colleagues (2006), on the other hand, asked for participants’
history of extrapair sexual activities, but integrated this single item in the broader composite
extrapair sexual interest, capturing also participants’ mere willingness to have extrapair sex as
well as their individual SOI-score. Hence, in both studies, infidelity was confounded by general
sexual openness, thereby limiting content validity of the measurement. Associations between
higher testosterone and multiple partners are also reported by van Anders, Hamilton and
Watson (2007). Yet, the authors investigated monogamous vs. polyamorous individuals. The
latter responded to questions for people with two or more current partners and were mostly
recruited from polyamory groups. Subjects in long-term relationships were characterized as
monogamous, but were not surveyed about potential extradyadic sexual activities. We therefore
argue, that Anders and colleagues (van Anders et al., 2007) investigated the link between
testosterone and different types of romantic and sexual relations varying in their degree of
commitment and exclusiveness (i.e. monogamy vs. polygamy). But, there was no examination
of the relation between testosterone and infidelity per se. However, in line with our findings,
the studies mentioned above report higher testosterone levels to be associated with greater
desire towards extrapair sexual engagement and more frequent uncommitted sexual behavior
(Edelstein et al., 2011; McIntyre et al., 2006; van Anders et al., 2007). The identified association
between higher testosterone levels and increased frequency of unfaithful behavior is small to
moderate in effect size and thus in line with the broad literature. Infidelity is conceptualized as
a multifactorial condition. It was shown that relationship satisfaction (Mark, Janssen, &
Milhausen, 2011), or depressive symptomatology (Gorman & Blow, 2008) influence the risk
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
14
for infidelity. This was further confirmed in our analysis showing lower levels of relationship
satisfaction and higher levels of depressive symptoms in unfaithful men (see table 1).
Interestingly, relationship satisfaction and depression are also related to testosterone levels
(Almeida et al., 2008; Feldman et al., 2002; Hooper et al., 2011). It is therefore assumed that
circulating testosterone levels in males stand in a mutually influencing relation with multiple
factors, while one such factor assembly consists of infidelity and associated constructs such as
promiscuity, willingness for extrapair activities, and engaging in polygamous relationships.
Men, naturally high in testosterone, tend to seek more frequent sexual encounters,
probably due to evolutionary reproductive benefits of having multiple sexual partners (Symons,
1979). Likewise, recent research has shown that men with high testosterone levels are perceived
as more attractive by women in terms of short-term mate judgments (O’Connor et al., 2014;
Roney et al., 2006). Research on personality traits and androgenization has shown that
testosterone levels are associated with histrionic behavior like seductivity (Bandini et al., 2009;
Perini et al., 2012) and narcissism (Pfattheicher, 2016). In line with this, high-testosterone
participants of our study may find it easier than low-testosterone subjects to approach potential
female sex partners because of the potentially more appealing impression they make on women.
In turn, this would offer greater opportunities for extrapair sexual activities.
Alternatively, the observed testosterone-infidelity-link might be mediated through
socioeconomical status (e.g. working position, income). Associations between higher
testosterone levels and higher status have been widely supported in previous literature
(Sherman, Lerner, Josephs, Renshon, & Gross, 2016), whereas elevated power in professionals
was shown to be associated with infidelity in both men and women (Lammers, Stoker, Jordan,
Pollmann, & Stapel, 2011). In turn, one could resume that the experienced power in men with
higher status increases their confidence in attracting potential sex partners. Likewise, men with
higher status and income may easily afford extrapair activities by hiring sex trade workers,
which men with lower income could not. However, statistics within a large sample of almost
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
15
4,000 Mexican men about categories of extramarital sex partner demonstrated that only 5% had
extramarital sexual intercourse with a prostitute, whereas 54% reported extrapair sexual
activities with a friend, followed by the category mistress (17%), and coworker (14%)
(Pulerwitz, Izazola-Licea, & Gortmaker, 2001).
Positive associations between testosterone and sexual function were previously shown
in a large sample of middle-aged and older men (O’Connor et al., 2011). Interestingly, in the
present sample, unfaithful men, exhibiting higher testosterone levels, showed lower sexual
function scores compared to faithful men. Therefore, it is possible that unfaithful partners might
have experienced greater relationship distress before engaging in unfaithful behavior, which in
turn reduces relationship satisfaction and intimacy motivation (Allen et al., 2005; Treas &
Giesen, 2000). Both, relationship satisfaction and intimacy motivation, have been shown to be
important protective factors against sexual dysfunction in aging men (Walther et al., 2017).
Consistent with previous studies in this area (Fincham & May, 2017), in our study
relationship satisfaction was significantly different between groups, with lower satisfaction
among unfaithful men. Further, group comparisons revealed higher alcohol consumption in
unfaithful men, which is also in line with previous findings (Atkins, Yi, Baucom, &
Christensen, 2005; Graham, Negash, Lambert, & Fincham, 2016). Alcohol is considered a
testicular toxin (Maneesh, Dutta, Chakrabarti, & Vasudevan, 2006), leading to a decline of
plasma testosterone even in moderate drinkers (Sierksma et al., 2004). However, review articles
summarize that subjective sexual arousal increases after consumption of alcohol (Källmén &
Gustafson, 1998) and fosters sexual risk-taking (George & Stoner, 2000). Moreover, a
disinhibiting side effect of alcohol has been widely proposed (George & Stoner, 2000;
Gustafson & Källmén, 1989), while experimental studies show that the loosening of inhibition
through drinking relies on expectancies and self-fulfilling prophecies rather than on actual
pharmacological mechanisms (George, Stoner, Norris, Lopez, & Lehman, 2000). In sum, the
mentioned psychological consequences of alcohol probably makes frequent drinking men
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
16
showing unfaithful behavior more often. An alternative explanation would be, that unfaithful
men tend to cope with alcohol consumption (Cook & Clark, 2005).
Findings in the present study do not allow for general conclusions about endogenous
testosterone levels predicting men’s disposition for infidelity. Recent research has suggested a
social modulation of androgenization rather than a simple hormonal causation (van Anders &
Watson, 2006). For example, testosterone levels increase as a direct consequence of sexual
arousal (Pirke, Kockott, & Dittmar, 1974; Stoléru, Ennaji, Cournot, & Spira, 1993),
masturbation (Purvis, Landgren, Cekan, & Diczfalusy, 1976) and sexual intercourse (Dabbs &
Mohammed, 1992). Moreover, findings from longitudinal studies show that masturbation
frequency predicts higher testosterone levels even five years later (Das & Sawin, 2016).
Furthermore, testosterone supplementation – being used by more and more men – increases
sexual desire and activity (Cunningham et al., 2016). This leads to the assumption that higher
testosterone levels in the unfaithful group might reflect a general effect of basal endogenous as
well as stimulated testosterone levels increasing the overall sexual desire and activity in males.
Moreover, unfaithful men in the present study showed lower relationship satisfaction,
which is often accompanied by diminished intimacy and lowered commitment to the long-term
partner. This may increase the urge to receive sexual satisfaction elsewhere. In line with this,
there is consistent evidence that those men still (or again) seeking potential partners, have
higher testosterone levels than men in a committed relationship (Alvergne, Faurie, & Raymond,
2009; Burnham et al., 2003; Gettler et al., 2013; van Anders et al., 2007). This underlines the
supporting effect of testosterone on male´s mating efforts and indicates that these efforts may
include short-term sexual activities namely extrapair sexual encounters.
Limitations
When interpreting these results, one needs to consider several limitations of this study. First,
although a relatively large sample of a homogenous group of men was examined post hoc power
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
17
analysis based on identified effect size suggest that the present study was slightly
underpowered. Further, the cross-sectional nature of the study limits causal inferences. This
should be addressed in future studies applying experimental-longitudinal study designs
potentially examining the association between testosterone levels and infidelity prospectively
in men undergoing testosterone treatment. Further, although under standardized conditions, the
testosterone measurement was obtained at a single time-point being susceptible for short-term
fluctuations. Repeated sampling or long-term measurement of testosterone provided by
cumulative measures (e.g. hair testosterone) might be more adequate to reflect an individual’s
hormonal state. Though significant effects were identified, effect sizes are small to moderate
and a multi-factorial biopsychosocial model is suggested to underlie infidelity. Although there
are many findings that support an evolutionary explanation between testosterone and mating
effort (infidelity), we acknowledge that there are as many psychological and social theories that
explain a greater prevalence of infidelity in men. Due to our focus on the possible hormonal
component of infidelity in men, we have mainly discussed the evolutionary-biological
perspective, but do not deny the relevance of parallel psychological and social theories. Finally,
although infidelity was asked directly and calculated via the cross-wise measurement, the topic
is highly sensitive and skeptical participants still may have answered in a socially desirable
way.
Conclusion
Our findings indicate that males with higher testosterone levels show increased extrapair sexual
activity. Men identified as not having a sexual dysfunction are mainly responsible for this
significant association in the present sample. Data about unfaithful behavior of participants
have shown high reliability after verifying direct statements about frequency of extrapair sexual
activities via the Crosswise Model. Direct and indirect prevalence of infidelity among subjects
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
18
were nearly equally high, indicating a low level of underreporting of infidelity in the present
study.
Although, dedicating to various sexual partners once had evolutionary benefits, in
today´s monogamous societies extrapair sexual activities often lead to severe relationship
conflicts thereby threatening not only the mental health and life quality of the betrayed partner,
but also that of the unfaithful partner. Higher testosterone levels may therefore be regarded as
potential risk factor for unfaithful behavior. This is especially important considering that
testosterone supplementation in men has received substantial attention for treating sexual
dysfunction or depression (Elliott et al., 2017; Walther et al., 2019) and an increasing number
of men worldwide is using testosterone products. Medical practitioners may thus consider to
inform men prior to using testosterone products about the possibility of an increased sex drive
due to an elevated basal testosterone level. This might be important to anticipate in exclusive
romantic relationships.
Author contributions
AW and UE designed the concept of the study. AW organized and conducted the study and
collected the data. CK and AW analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript.
UE, TL, and PW contributed with important intellectual content and edited subsequent versions
of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was fully funded by the University Research Priority Program – Dynamics of
Healthy Aging of the University of Zurich.
Conflict of interest
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
19
None declared.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all study participants for contributing to the study. Furthermore, we
would like to thank Thomas Bernauer for his support in implementing the crosswise
questioning procedure in the study design.
References
Allen, E. S., Atkins, D. C., Baucom, D. H., Snyder, D. K., Gordon, K. C., & Glass, S. P.
(2005). Intrapersonal, Interpersonal, and Contextual Factors in Engaging in and
Responding to Extramarital Involvement. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,
12(2), 101–130. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpi014
Allen, E. S., & Baucom, D. H. (2004). Adult Attachment and Patterns of Extradyadic
Involvement. Family Process, 43(4), 467–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-
5300.2004.00035.x
Almeida, O. P., Yeap, B. B., Hankey, G. J., Jamrozik, K., & Flicker, L. (2008). Low free
testosterone concentration as a potentially treatable cause of depressive symptoms in
older men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(3), 283–289. doi:
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.33
Alvergne, A., Faurie, C., & Raymond, M. (2009a). Variation in testosterone levels and male
reproductive effort: Insight from a polygynous human population. Hormones and
Behavior, 56(5), 491–497. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.013
Amato, P. R., & Previti, D. (2003). People’s reasons for divorcing gender, social class, the life
course, and adjustment. Journal of Family Issues, 24(5), 602–626. doi:
10.1177/0192513X03254507
Anderson, R. A., Bancroft, J., & Wu, F. C. (1992). The effects of exogenous testosterone on
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
20
sexuality and mood of normal men. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Metabolism, 75(6), 1503–1507. doi: 10.1210/jcem.75.6.1464655
Anestis, S. F. (2006). Testosterone in juvenile and adolescent male chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes): effects of dominance rank, aggression, and behavioral style. American
Journal of Physical Anthropology, 130(4), 536–545. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20387
Atkins, D. C., Yi, J., Baucom, D. H., & Christensen, A. (2005). Infidelity in Couples Seeking
Marital Therapy. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(3), 470–473. doi: 10.1037/0893-
3200.19.3.470
Bandini, E., Corona, G., Ricca, V., Fisher, A. D., Lotti, F., Sforza, A., … Maggi, M. (2009).
Hysterical Traits Are Not from the Uterus but from the Testis: A Study in Men with
Sexual Dysfunction. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 6(8), 2321–2331. doi:
10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01322.x
Barrett, E. S., Tran, V., Thurston, S., Jasienska, G., Furberg, A.-S., Ellison, P. T., & Thune, I.
(2013). Marriage and motherhood are associated with lower testosterone
concentrations in women. Hormones and Behavior, 63(1), 72–79. doi:
10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.10.012
Brand, R. J., Markey, C. M., Mills, A., & Hodges, S. D. (2007). Sex Differences in Self-
reported Infidelity and its Correlates. Sex Roles, 57(1–2), 101–109. doi:
10.1007/s11199-007-9221-5
Braunstein, G. D., Sundwall, D. A., Katz, M., Shifren, J. L., Buster, J. E., Simon, J. A., …
Watts, N. B. (2006). Safety and Efficacy of a Testosterone Patch for the Treatment of
Hypoactive Sexual Desire Disorder in Surgically Menopausal Women: a Randomized,
Placebo-Controlled Trial. Journal of Urology, 175(2), 660–660. doi: 10.1016/S0022-
5347(05)00380-0
Burnham, T. C., Chapman, J. F., Gray, P. B., McIntyre, M. H., Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T.
(2003). Men in committed, romantic relationships have lower testosterone. Hormones
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
21
and Behavior, 44(2), 119–122. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00125-9
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual Strategies Theory: An evolutionary perspective
on human mating. Psychological Review, 100(2), 204–232. doi: 10.1037/0033-
295X.100.2.204
Clifton, S., Macdowall, W., Copas, A. J., Tanton, C., Keevil, B. G., Lee, D. M., … Wu, F. C.
W. (2016). Salivary testosterone levels and health status in men and women in the
British general population: Findings from the third national survey of sexual attitudes
and lifestyles (natsal-3). Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 101(11),
3939–3951. doi: 10.1210/jc.2016-1669
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159.
Cook, R. L., & Clark, D. B. (2005). Is there an association between alcohol consumption and
sexually transmitted diseases? A systematic review. Sexually Transmitted Diseases,
32(3), 156–164.
Corona, G., Jannini, E. A., Mannucci, E., Fisher, A. D., Lotti, F., Petrone, L., … Maggi, M.
(2008). Different Testosterone Levels Are Associated with Ejaculatory Dysfunction.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5(8), 1991–1998. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-
6109.2008.00803.x
Cunningham, G. R., Stephens-Shields, A. J., Rosen, R. C., Wang, C., Bhasin, S., Matsumoto,
A. M., … Snyder, P. J. (2016). Testosterone Treatment and Sexual Function in Older
Men with Low Testosterone Levels. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Metabolism, (July), jc.2016-1645. doi: 10.1210/jc.2016-1645
Dabbs, J. M., & Mohammed, S. (1992). Male and female salivary testosterone concentrations
before and after sexual activity. Physiology & Behavior, 52(1), 195–197.
Dai, W. S., Gutai, J. P., Kuller, L. H., & Cauley, J. A. (1988). Cigarette smoking and serum
sex hormones in men. American Journal of Epidemiology, 128(4), 796–805.
Das, A., & Sawin, N. (2016). Social Modulation or Hormonal Causation? Linkages of
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
22
Testosterone with Sexual Activity and Relationship Quality in a Nationally
Representative Longitudinal Sample of Older Adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
45(8), 2101–2115. doi: 10.1007/s10508-016-0815-2
Edelstein, R. S., Chopik, W. J., & Kean, E. L. (2011). Sociosexuality moderates the
association between testosterone and relationship status in men and women.
Hormones and Behavior, 60(3), 248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.05.007
Elliott, J., Kelly, S. E., Millar, A. C., Peterson, J., Chen, L., Johnston, A., … Wells, G. A.
(2017). Testosterone therapy in hypogonadal men: a systematic review and network
meta-analysis. British Medical Journal, 7, 1–10. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-015284
English, K. M., Pugh, P. J., Parry, H., Scutt, N. E., Channer, K. S., & Jones, T. H. (2001).
Effect of cigarette smoking on levels of bioavailable testosterone in healthy men.
Clinical Science, 100(6), 661. doi: 10.1042/CS20010011
Feldman, H. a., Longcope, C., Derby, C. a., Johannes, C. B., Araujo, A. B., Coviello, A. D.,
… McKinlay, J. B. (2002). Age trends in the level of serum testosterone and other
hormones in middle-aged men: Longitudinal results from the Massachusetts Male
Aging Study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 87(2), 589–598.
doi: 10.1210/jc.87.2.589
Fincham, F. D., & May, R. W. (2017). Infidelity in romantic relationships. Current Opinion
in Psychology, 13, 70–74. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.03.008
Finkelstein, J. S., Lee, H., Burnett-Bowie, S.-A. M., Pallais, J. C., Yu, E. W., Borges, L. F.,
… Leder, B. Z. (2013). Gonadal steroids and body composition, strength, and sexual
function in men. The New England Journal of Medicine, 369, 1011–1022. doi:
10.1056/NEJMoa1206168
Fisher, A. D., Corona, G., Bandini, E., Mannucci, E., Lotti, F., Boddi, V., … Maggi, M.
(2009). Psychobiological Correlates of Extramarital Affairs and Differences between
Stable and Occasional Infidelity among Men with Sexual Dysfunctions. The Journal
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
23
of Sexual Medicine, 6(3), 866–875. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2008.01140.x
George, W. H., & Stoner, S. A. (2000). Understanding Acute Alcohol Effects on Sexual
Behavior. Annual Review of Sex Research, 11(1), 92–124. doi:
10.1080/10532528.2000.10559785
George, W. H., Stoner, S. A., Norris, J., Lopez, P. A., & Lehman, G. L. (2000). Alcohol
expectancies and sexuality: a self-fulfilling prophecy analysis of dyadic perceptions
and behavior. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 61(1), 168–176.
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Agustin, S. S., Feranil, A. B., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2013). Do
testosterone declines during the transition to marriage and fatherhood relate to men’s
sexual behavior? Evidence from the Philippines. Hormones and Behavior, 64(5), 755–
763. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.08.019
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Feranil, A. B., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). Longitudinal
evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 108(39), 16194–16199. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105403108
Gordon, K. C., Baucom, D. H., & Snyder, D. K. (2004). An integrative intervention for
promoting recovery from extramarital affairs. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,
30(2), 213–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2004.tb01235.x
Gorman, L., & Blow, A. (2008). Concurrent depression and infidelity: Background, strategies
for treatment, and future research. Journal of Couple & Relationship Therapy, 7(1),
39–58.
Gould, L., & Ziegler, T. E. (2007). Variation in fecal testosterone levels, inter-male
aggression, dominance rank and age during mating and post-mating periods in wild
adult male ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta). American Journal of Primatology, 69(12),
1325–1339. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20438
Graham, S. M., Negash, S., Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2016). Problem Drinking and
Extradyadic Sex in Young Adult Romantic Relationships. Journal of Social and
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
24
Clinical Psychology, 35(2), 152–170. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2016.35.2.152
Guo, W., Liao, C., Zou, Y., Li, F., Li, T., Zhou, Q., … Mao, X. (2010). Erectile Dysfunction
and Risk of Clinical Cardiovascular Events: A Meta-Analysis of Seven Cohort
Studies. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 7(8), 2805–2816. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-
6109.2010.01792.x
Gustafson, R., & Källmén, H. (1989). The effect of alcohol intoxication on primary and
secondary processes in male social drinkers. British Journal of Addiction, 84(12),
1507–1513.
Haversath, J., & Kröger, C. (2014). [Extradyadic Sex and its Predictors in Homo- and
Heterosexuals.]. Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie, 64(12),
458–464. doi: 10.1055/s-0034-1390423
Hooper, A. E. C., Gangestad, S. W., Thompson, M. E., & Bryan, A. D. (2011). Testosterone
and romance: the association of testosterone with relationship commitment and
satisfaction in heterosexual men and women. American Journal of Human Biology,
23(4), 553–555.
Isidori, A. M., Giannetta, E., Gianfrilli, D., Greco, E. A., Bonifacio, V., Aversa, A., … Lenzi,
A. (2005). Effects of testosterone on sexual function in men: results of a meta-
analysis. Clinical Endocrinology, 63(4), 381–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-
2265.2005.02350.x
Jann, B., Jerke, J., & Krumpal, I. (2012). Asking Sensitive Questions Using the Crosswise
Model: An Experimental Survey Measuring Plagiarism. Public Opinion Quarterly,
76(1), 32–49. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfr036
Juster, R. P., Almeida, D., Cardoso, C., Raymond, C., Johnson, P. J., Pfaus, J. G., … Lupien,
S. J. (2016). Gonads and strife: Sex hormones vary according to sexual orientation for
women and stress indices for both sexes. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 72, 119–130.
doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.06.011
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
25
Källmén, H., & Gustafson, R. (1998). Alcohol and Disinhibition. European Addiction
Research, 4, 150–162.
Kelley, K., & Preacher, K. J. (2012). On effect size. Psychological Methods, 17(2), 137.
Kivlighan, K. T., Granger, D. A., & Schwartz, E. B. (2005). Blood contamination and the
measurement of salivary progesterone and estradiol. Hormones and Behavior, 47(3),
367–370. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.12.001
Kuzawa, C. W., Gettler, L. T., Huang, Y., & McDade, T. W. (2010). Mothers have lower
testosterone than non-mothers: Evidence from the Philippines. Hormones and
Behavior, 57(4–5), 441–447. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.01.014
Lammers, J., Stoker, J. I., Jordan, J., Pollmann, M., & Stapel, D. A. (2011). Power Increases
Infidelity Among Men and Women. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1191–1197. doi:
10.1177/0956797611416252
Laumann, E. O., Michael, R. T., Gagnon, J. H., & Michaels, S. (1995). The Social
Organization of Sexuality-Reply. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical
Association, 274(7), 536–537. doi: 10.1001/jama.1995.03530070031020
Laumann, Edward O. (Ed.). (1994). The social organization of sexuality: sexual practices in
the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Leproult, R., & E, V. C. (2011). Effect of 1 week of sleep restriction on testosterone levels in
young healthy men. JAMA, 305(21), 2173–2174.
Maneesh, M., Dutta, S., Chakrabarti, A., & Vasudevan, D. M. (2006). Alcohol abuse-duration
dependent decrease in plasma testosterone and antioxidants in males. Indian Journal
of Physiology and Pharmacology, 50(3), 291–296.
Mark, K. P., Janssen, E., & Milhausen, R. R. (2011). Infidelity in heterosexual couples:
Demographic, interpersonal, and personality-related predictors of extradyadic sex.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(5), 971–982.
McIntyre, M., Gangestad, S. W., Gray, P. B., Chapman, J. F., Burnham, T. C., O’Rourke, M.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
26
T., & Thornhill, R. (2006). Romantic involvement often reduces men’s testosterone
levels--but not always: The moderating role of extrapair sexual interest. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 642–651. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.642
O’Connor, D. B., Lee, D. M., Corona, G., Forti, G., Tajar, A., O’Neill, T. W., … the
European Male Ageing Study Group. (2011). The Relationships between Sex
Hormones and Sexual Function in Middle-Aged and Older European Men. The
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 96(10), E1577–E1587. doi:
10.1210/jc.2010-2216
O’Connor, J. J. M., Pisanski, K., Tigue, C. C., Fraccaro, P. J., & Feinberg, D. R. (2014).
Perceptions of infidelity risk predict women’s preferences for low male voice pitch in
short-term over long-term relationship contexts. Personality and Individual
Differences, 56, 73–77. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.029
Penke, L., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Beyond global sociosexual orientations: A more
differentiated look at sociosexuality and its effects on courtship and romantic
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1113–1135. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1113
Perini, T., Ditzen, B., Hengartner, M., & Ehlert, U. (2012). Sensation seeking in fathers: The
impact on testosterone and paternal investment. Hormones and Behavior, 61(2), 191–
195. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.004
Petersen, J. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). A meta-analytic review of research on gender
differences in sexuality, 1993–2007. Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 21–38. doi:
10.1037/a0017504
Pfattheicher, S. (2016). Testosterone, cortisol and the Dark Triad: Narcissism (but not
Machiavellianism or psychopathy) is positively related to basal testosterone and
cortisol. Personality and Individual Differences, 97, 115–119. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.015
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
27
Pirke, K. M., Kockott, G., & Dittmar, F. (1974). Psychosexual stimulation and plasma
testosterone in man. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 3(6), 577–584. doi:
10.1007/BF01541140
Plack, K., Kröger, C., Hahlweg, K., & Klann, N. (2008). Außerpartnerschaftliche
beziehungen - Die individuelle belastung der partner und die partnerschaftliche
zufriedenheit nach dem erleben von untreue kurzbericht. Zeitschrift Fur Klinische
Psychologie Und Psychotherapie, 37(4), 231–235. doi: 10.1026/1616-3443.37.4.231
Pulerwitz, J., Izazola-Licea, J.-A., & Gortmaker, S. L. (2001). Extrarelational Sex Among
Mexican Men and Their Partners’ Risk of HIV and Other Sexually Transmitted
Diseases. American Journal of Public Health, 91(10), 1650–1652. doi:
10.2105/AJPH.91.10.1650
Purvis, K., Landgren, B. M., Cekan, Z., & Diczfalusy, E. (1976). Endocrine effects of
masturbation in men. The Journal of Endocrinology, 70(3), 439–444.
Roney, J. R., Hanson, K. N., Durante, K. M., & Maestripieri, D. (2006). Reading men’s faces:
women’s mate attractiveness judgments track men’s testosterone and interest in
infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1598), 2169–
2175. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3569
Sander, J., & Böcker, S. (1993). Die Deutsche Form der Relationship Assessment Scale
(RAS): Eine kurze Skala zur Messung der Zufriedenheit in einer Partnerschaft.
Diagnostica.
Scott, S. B., Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., Allen, E. S., & Markman, H. J. (2013). Reasons
for Divorce and Recollections of Premarital Intervention: Implications for Improving
Relationship Education. Couple & Family Psychology, 2(2), 131–145. doi:
10.1037/a0032025
Sherman, G. D., Lerner, J. S., Josephs, R. A., Renshon, J., & Gross, J. J. (2016). The
interaction of testosterone and cortisol is associated with attained status in male
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
28
executives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(6), 921–929. doi:
10.1037/pspp0000063
Sierksma, A., Sarkola, T., Eriksson, C. J. P., Gaag, M. S., Grobbee, D. E., & Hendriks, H. F.
J. (2004). Effect of Moderate Alcohol Consumption on Plasma
Dehydroepiandrosterone Sulfate, Testosterone, and Estradiol Levels in Middle-Aged
Men and Postmenopausal Women: A Diet-Controlled Intervention Study. Alcoholism:
Clinical and Experimental Research, 28(5), 780–785. doi:
10.1097/01.ALC.0000125356.70824.81
Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: evidence
for convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 60(6), 870–883.
Stoléru, S. G., Ennaji, A., Cournot, A., & Spira, A. (1993). LH pulsatile secretion and
testosterone blood levels are influenced by sexual arousal in human males.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 18(3), 205–218. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(93)90005-6
Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.
Treas, J., & Giesen, D. (2000). Sexual Infidelity Among Married and Cohabiting Americans.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 62(1), 48–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-
3737.2000.00048.x
van Anders, S. M., Hamilton, L. D., & Watson, N. V. (2007). Multiple partners are associated
with higher testosterone in North American men and women. Hormones and
Behavior, 51(3), 454–459. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.002
van Anders, S. M., & Watson, N. V. (2006). Social neuroendocrinology: Effects of social
contexts and behaviors on sex steroids in humans. Human Nature, 17(2), 212–237.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-006-1018-7
Wagle, K. C., Carrejo, M. H., & Tan, R. S. (2012). The Implications of Increasing Age on
Erectile Dysfunction. American Journal of Men’s Health, 6(4), 273–279. doi:
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
29
10.1177/1557988311431629
Waldvogel, P., & Ehlert, U. (2017). Testosterone is Associated with Perceived Constraint in
Early Fatherhood. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology. doi: 10.1007/s40750-
017-0077-9
Walther, A., Breidenstein, J., & Miller, R. (2019). Association of Testosterone Treatment
with Alleviation of Depressive Symptoms in Men: A Systematic Review and Meta-
analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 1;76(1), 31–40. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2734
Walther, A., & Ehlert, U. (2015). Steroid secretion and psychological well-being in men 40+.
In T. Rice & L. Sher (Eds.), Neurobiology of men’s mental health. New York, Nova.
(pp. 287–322).
Walther, A., Mahler, F., Debelak, R., & Ehlert, U. (2017). Psychobiological Protective
Factors Modifying the Association Between Age and Sexual Health in Men : Findings
From the Men ’ s Health 40 + Study. American Journal of Men’s Health, 11(3), 737–
747. doi: 10.1177/1557988316689238
Walther, A., Phillip, M., Lozza, N., & Ehlert, U. (2016). The rate of change in declining
steroid hormones : a new parameter of healthy aging in men ? Oncotarget, 7(38), 1–
28. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.11752
Wiederman, M. W. (1997). Extramarital sex: Prevalence and correlates in a national survey.
Journal of Sex Research, 34(2), 167–174. doi: 10.1080/00224499709551881
Wiltink, J., Hauck, E. W., Phädayanon, M., Weidner, W., & Beutel, M. E. (2003). Validation
of the German version of the International Index of Erectile Function (IIEF) in
patients with erectile dysfunction, Peyronie’s disease and controls. International
Journal of Impotence Research, 15(3), 192–197. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijir.3900997
Yu, J.-W., Tian, G.-L., & Tang, M.-L. (2008). Two new models for survey sampling with
sensitive characteristic: design and analysis. Metrika, 67(3), 251.
Zabora, J., Brintzenhofeszoc, K., Jacobsen, P., Curbow, B., Piantadosi, S., Hooker, C., …
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
30
Derogatis, L. (2001). A new psychosocial screening instrument for use with cancer
patients. Psychosomatics, 42(3), 241–246.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
31
Figure caption
Figure 1. Testosterone levels of faithful and unfaithful men. Values are presented as mean and
95% confidence intervals.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
32
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample (N=224)
unfaithful
(N=84)
faithful
(N=140)
Characteristics
M
SD
M
SD
T
p
Age
59.80
10.62
57.01
10.62
1.903
.058
BMI
25.13
3.16
25.60
2.90
-1.126
.262
Sexual function (IIEF)
42.21
16.76
46.77
15.62
-2.057
.041
Health status (SF-36)
2.44
.61
2.39
.71
-.59
.555
Relationship duration
23.64
16.41
20.20
15.69
1.563
.120
Relationship
satisfaction (RAS)
4.05
.70
4.27
.67
2.241
.026
Depressive symptoms
(BSI-18-D)
1.87
2.87
1.13
2.34
-2.101
.037
Sexual activity (last
12 hours)
2.00
.00
1.99
.12
-1.42
.158
χ
2
p
Education level
5.25
1.17
5.21
1.36
6.883
.142
Tobacco consumption
1.40
.971
1.31
.897
3.164
.531
Alcohol consumption
3.94
1.09
3.34
1.09
18.813
.002
Medication intake
1.63
.485
1.68
.469
.531
.466
Note: BMI: body mass index; IIEF: International Index of Erectile Function; SF-36: Short Form Health Survey;
relationship duration in years; RAS: Relationship Assessment Scale; BSI-18-D: Brief Symptom Inventory 18 Items
Depression Subscale, tobacco consumption assessed by an item with a 5-point likert scale from “non-smoker” to
“smoking 10 or more cigarettes per day”, alcohol consumption on a 6-point likert scale from “I never drink
alcohol” to “2 or more alcoholic drinks per day; Medication intake assessed with “yes” or “no”.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
33
Table 2. Prevalence in % of infidelity via direct questioning and Crosswise-Model.
Direct Questioning
(DQ)
Crosswise-Model
(CM)
Difference
(CM-DQ)
infidelity
37.5
37.7
0.2
observations
224
220
Table 3. Linear model predicting frequency of extrapair sexual activity during the current
relationship y multiple predictors.
b
SE b
ß
p
Constant term
-.194
5.384
NA
.930
Testosterone levels (pg/mg)
.004 [.001, .007]
.002
.146
.045
Age
.007 [-.003, .017]
.006
.108
.213
Education level
-.020 [-.092, .053]
.036
-.038
.571
BMI
-.001 [-.034, .036]
.016
-.005
.943
Medication
.023 [-.166, .232]
.113
.016
.837
Cigarette consumption
.028 [-.054, .114]
.051
.038
.584
Alcohol consumption
.105 [.019, .203]
.042
.171
.014
General health status (SF36-1)
-.043 [-.194, .123]
.072
-.042
.552
Sexual function (IIEF-SF)
-.002 [-.009, .003]
.003
-.057
.458
Sexual activity
.561 [.211, .960]
.488
.077
.251
Depression (BSI-D)
.042 [-006, .093]
.020
.156
.037
Relationship satisfaction (RAS)
-.093 [-.276, .076]
.077
-.094
.225
Gingival bleeding
.105 [-.315, .499]
.187
.038
.574
Awakening time
.002 [.001, .003]
.001
.144
.037
Starting time of saliva sampling
-.117 [-1.284, .115]
.217
-.036
.589
Note. NA: not available. 95% bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals reported in parentheses. Confidence
intervals and standard errors based on 1000 bootstrap samples. Explained variance of the frequency of extrapair sexual
activity by the model was 13% (R
2
= .13). BMI: body mass index, IIEF-SF: International Index of Erectile Function
– Sexual Function Scale; SF-36-1: Short Form Health Survey – Item 1; RAS: Relationship Assessment Scale; BSI-
18-D: Brief Symptom Inventory 18 Items Depression Subscale.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
34
Table 4. Binary logistic regression model predicting unfaithful vs. faithful men by multiple
predictors
b
SE b
95% CI for Odds Ratio
p
Lower
Odds
Upper
Constant term
-26.523
54413.032
NA
NA
NA
<.001
Testosterone levels
(pg/mg)
.016
.006
1.01
1.02
1.03
.015
Age
0.33
.019
.99
1.03
1.07
.081
Education level
.015
.123
.797
1.015
1.29
.904
BMI
-.014
.054
.89
.986
1.10
.801
Medication
.197
.372
.59
1.22
2.52
.597
Cigarette consumption
.097
.170
.79
1.10
1.54
.570
Alcohol consumption
.476
.146
1.21
1.61
2.14
.001
General health status
(SF36-1)
.000
.242
.62
1.00
1.61
.999
Sexual function (IIEF-SF)
-.014
.011
.97
.986
1.01
.212
Sexual activity
21.19
.511
NA
NA
NA
.999
Depression (BSI-D)
.112
.068
.98
1.12
1.28
.100
Relationship satisfaction
(RAS)
-.186
.250
.51
.83
1.35
.456
Gingival bleeding
.490
.636
.47
1.63
5.68
.441
Awakening time
.003
.003
.99
1.00
1.01
.188
Starting time of saliva
sampling
-2.86
.4.037
.001
.058
157.02
.479
Note. NA: not available. 95% confidence intervals for odds ratio. Explained variance of the frequency of extrapair sexual
activity by the model was 21% (R
2
= .21). BMI: body mass index, IIEF-SF: International Index of Erectile Function – Sexual
Function Scale; SF-36-1: Short Form Health Survey – Item 1; RAS: Relationship Assessment Scale; BSI-18-D: Brief
Symptom Inventory 18 Items Depression Subscale.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT