1
Y
EAR
T
WO
Second Language Acquisition
Academic Year 2011-2012
#18: Motivational factors and attributions
1. Defining motivation
By and large, the social-psychological construct of motivation is thought of as
an inner drive, impulse,
emotion, or desire that moves one to a particular action. In other words, it refers to "the choices people
make as to what experiences or goals they will approach or avoid, and the degree of effort they will
exert in that respect" (Brown 2007:168-169). However, when applied to language-learning situations,
motivation may be interpreted as "the attitudes and affective states that influence the degree of effort
that learners make to learn an L2" (Ellis 1997: 75). In the light of extensive research, it is believed to be
a key to learning (Crookes & Schmidt 1991). In fact, motivation appears to be the second strongest
predictor of success in the study of L2, after aptitude (Gass & Selinker 2008: 426; after Skehan 1989).
2. Motivation and human needs
Fig. 1. Maslow's hierarchy of basic needs
(Salkind & Rasmussen 2008: 634)
Social psychologist Abraham Maslow (1970)
put forth the concept of a hierarchy of basic
human needs (cf. Fig. 1) which are believed
to account for motivation. The strata of
Maslow’s pyramid begin with
deficiency
needs, such as physiological necessities
(air, water, food), and then advance to
higher needs of security, identity, and self-
esteem, the fulfilment of which, in turn,
leads to
growth needs: from cognitive and
aesthetic needs to self-actualisation.
Other psychologists have acknowledged
further
fundamental
needs,
such
as
achievement, autonomy, affiliation, order,
change, endurance, aggression. Educational
psychologists,
for
example,
define
motivation in terms of certain
needs or
drives.
Cognitive
psychologist
David
Ausubel identified six such needs (Ausubel
et al. 1978: 368-379):
o
the need for
exploration, i.e. for seeing "the other side of the mountain", for probing the
unknown;
o
the need for
manipulation, i.e. for "operating" on the environment and causing change;
o
the need for
activity, i.e. for movement and exercise, both physical and mental;
o
the need for
stimulation, i.e. the need to be stimulated by the environment, by other people, or
by ideas, thoughts, and feelings;
S
ELF
-
A
CTUALI
-
SATION
A
ESTHETIC
N
EEDS
C
OGNITIVE
N
EEDS
E
STEEM
N
EEDS
B
ELONGINGNESS AND
L
OVE
N
EEDS
S
AFETY
N
EEDS
P
HYSIOLOGICAL
N
EEDS
G
ROWTH
N
EEDS
D
EFICIENCY
N
EEDS
2
o
the need for
knowledge, i.e. the need to process and internalise the results of exploration,
manipulation, activity, and stimulation, to resolve contradictions, to quest for solutions to
problems and for self-consistent systems of knowledge;
o
the need for
ego enhancement, i.e. for the self to be known and to be accepted and approved of
by others.
Brown (2007) believes that motivation, like self-esteem, can be viewed as global, situational, or task-
oriented. Learning a foreign language might require some of all three kinds of motivation. For example,
a learner may be highly "globally" motivated but his low "task" motivation may hinder his participation in
group activities or his written performance in L2.
3. Learner orientations - instrumental vs. integrative motivation
In the 1970s a significant tradition of motivational studies applied to L2 learning was developed in
Canada by Robert Gardner and his associates (cf. Gardner & Lambert 1972; Lambert 1972; Gardner &
MacIntyre 1991; MacIntyre & Gardner 1991; Gardner
et al. 1992), who proposed a distinction between
instrumental and integrative orientations of the L2 learner. Instrumental motivation refers to a need to
acquire a language as a way to achieve instrumental goals, such as pursuing a professional career,
reading literature in the field, translating, etc. Integrative motivation, on the other hand, is activated
when the learner wants to get integrated with the L2 speech community and culture - to identify himself
with and become a part of that social group.
However, more recent studies show that there is no single motivational factor determining the learning
of L2: certain learners in certain contexts may indeed profit from integrative orientation, while others in
different contexts may be more successful if they are instrumentally oriented. The findings also lend
support to the more and more widespread conviction that the two types of motivation are not
necessarily mutually exclusive. Second language learning is rarely stimulated by attitudes that are purely
instrumental or purely integrative. In most situations, the two types of motivation complement each
other.
4. Learner orientations - intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivation
Motivation is also typically examined in terms of the
intrinsic and extrinsic orientations of the learner.
Learners who study for their own self-perceived needs and goals are intrinsically oriented. In
contradistinction to the above type, those who pursue a goal exclusively to receive an external reward
from someone else are extrinsically motivated. Among typical extrinsic rewards are money, prizes, good
grades, and even certain types of positive feedback. Behaviours carried out solely to avoid punishment
are also extrinsically motivated, though punishment avoidance can also be viewed by some learners
intrinsically as a challenge that can enhance their sense of competence and self-determination.
However, the intrinsic-extrinsic construct must be differentiated from Gardner and Lambert’s
instrumental-integrative motivation. Although many instances of intrinsic motivation may indeed turn
out to be integrative, some may not. For example, a learner could, for highly developed intrinsic
purposes, want to learn L2 in order to progress in a career or to pursue an academic programme.
Similarly, another learner might adopt a positive
attitude toward the second language speech
community for extrinsic reasons: parental reinforcement, teacher’s encouragement, etc.
3
5. The motivational grid
Brown (2007: 175) plotted the relationship between the two above-mentioned dichotomies on the
following diagrammatic representation:
Motivational Taxonomy
INTRINSIC
EXTRINSIC
INTEGRATIVE
A L2 learner wishes to integrate
with the L2 culture (e.g., for
immigration or marriage)
Someone else wishes the L2 learner to know the
L2 for integrative reasons (e.g., Japanese-
American parents send their children to a
Japanese language school)
INSTRUMENTAL
A L2 learner wishes to achieve
goals utilising L2 (e.g., for a
career)
An external power wants the L2 learner to learn
L2 (e.g., a corporation sends a Japanese
businessman to the U.S. for language training)
6. Resultative motivation
Various other kinds of motivation have also been identified. One of the most interesting concepts is
resultative motivation. It is described by Ellis (1997) in the following words (pp. 75-76):
An assumption of […] research […] is that motivation is the
cause of L2 achievement. However, it
is also possible that motivation is the
result of learning. That is, learners who experience success
in learning may become more, or in some contexts, less motivated to learn. This helps to explain
the conflicting research results. In a context like Canada, success in learning French may
intensify English-speaking learners' liking for French culture. However, in California success in
learning English may bring Mexican women into situations where they experience discrimination
and thus reduce their appreciation of American culture.
7. Attribution theory
Alongside emotional security, a language student needs to develop a positive attitude to himself as a
learner. According to attribution theory, which studies causes that learners assign their success or
failure to, behind one's performance in L2 there are factors within one's control as well as those beyond
one's control. By and large, attribution theory shows how people make causal explanations and what
responses to questions beginning with 'why' are given. It accounts for the behavioral and emotional
consequences of those explanations. As Weiner (1984) states, achievement behavior may be influenced
by a task's objective qualities and to a considerable extent by an individual's attribute responsibility for
success on the task.
Psychologists believe that when learners feel that their performance is determined by factors which they
can control - such as their own efforts or abilities - they will show achievement behavior and respond
positively to failure, trying harder next time. On the other hand, when they are convinced that their
output is determined by factors outside their control - such as luck or the task’s difficulty - they may
respond negatively to failure and give up altogether, believing that they will not succeed, however hard
they may try.
4
Attribution theory originated in the 1950s and 1960s as one of the approaches to the study of
motivation. It suggests that people vary in the way they attribute causes to events. The four principal
determinants of success are:
ability, task difficulty, effort, and luck. These are analyzed in terms of
stability and locus of control (see the table below; after Skehan 1989: 51). The former two are relatively
unchangeable and are therefore in the stability dimension. In contrast, the latter two are unstable and
they are subject to modification.
LOCUS OF CONTROL
STABILITY
Internal
External
Stable
Ability
Task difficulty
Unstable
Effort
Luck
Graham (1994) adds further traditional attributions: mood, family background and help or hindrance
from others.
For the learning/teaching context, the worst situation obtains when individuals "consistently attribute
bad outcomes to stable internal attributes, and good outcomes to unstable external causes" (Skehan
1989: 52) - in other words, when they are convinced that their failure is due to their lack of ability.
Clearly, for language learning it is essential to what a student ascribes his or her achievement. If the
unstable factors, for example effort, are believed to be the causes of success, the motivation tends to
be higher. Learners who think that they have succeeded thanks to effort which they have invested are
usually more persistent and motivated, since they perceive themselves as having some bearing on the
learning process.
According to Dőrnyei (2001), there are two reasons why attribution theory is relevant for the study of
language learning. Firstly
, the feeling of failure in learning an L2 is very common: most learners are not
fully satisfied with their level of L2 and they typically fail in at least one L2 during their lifetime.
Secondly, a lot - and quite rightly so - can be attributed to linguistic aptitude. As Covington (1998)
states, "it is not so much the event of failure that disrupts academic achievement as it is the meaning of
failure. Thus, rather than minimizing failure, educators should arrange schooling so that falling short of
one's goals will be interpreted in ways that promote the will to learn". Additionally, promoting effort
attributions is important. Teachers should stress the role of effort and make students believe that it can
lead to success, but they should not lay emphasis on ability because, unlike effort, it does not offer
everyone en equal chance.
8. Locus of control
Locus of control […] is a personality dimension which can be presented as a continuum from
external to internal control over life events […]. [It] is not objectively but subjectively
determined, i.e. people perceive life events not as they really are but according to environmental
circumstances. In other words, life events can be either caused by our own actions (internal
control) or by the environment, i.e. other people, God, luck, fate etc. (external control). […] It is
assumed that people with more internal control are better prepared to guide their individual
development and their assimilation with the environment. Consequently, they appear to be more
ambitious and more academically gifted. On the other hand, too much internal control may
result in individuals blaming themselves for all negative events which were, in fact, caused by
5
other people. Individuals with external locus of control are more conformist, submissive, less
ambitious, more dependent on others, and generally less gifted academically. (Michońska-
Stadnik 2006: 147-148)
The process of change of the locus of control from the external to the internal world is called
regulation. During those periods when the child is most controlled by another person, he or she
is considered to be other-regulated, and upon the attainment of the ability to function
independently in the task, he or she is considered to be self-regulated. (Buckwalter 2001:
382)
9. Ways of encouraging language learners
Dőrnyei (2001) suggests some means of encouraging foreign-language students:
o
Providing feedback on effort: feedback that teachers give to their students is the most
convincing means towards promoting effort attributions. If students fail, teachers should
underline the low effort and communicate to students that they can do better in the future, but
if students fail in spite of the fact that they put effort into their work, teachers should explain
that effort has to be complemented with sufficient skills and strategies.
o
Refusing to accept ability attributions: teachers should not pay attention to ability and when
students assess their attributions to low ability, e.g. 'I'm not good at languages', teachers should
make it clear that they are convinced about their abilities.
o
Modeling effort-outcome linkages: teachers may talk about their personal experiences how they
managed to do a difficult task by putting a lot of effort to succeed. They can also encourage
learners to offer effort explanations: by giving suitable prompting and support. For instance,
teachers may ask learners what they found thought-provoking about a language task, what they
have found out from the experience.
o
Making effort and perseverance a class norm: teachers should make remarks such as 'I like the
way you try' or 'That was a nice piece of effort' and underline the significance of effortful
behaviour. This should be made classroom norm. Strategic effort attributions help learners to
believe that working hard in a particular way is what leads to success. Teachers can encourage
them to view failures as problem solving situations in which the search for an improved strategy
becomes their main focus of attention.
REFERENCES AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FURTHER READING
Ausubel, D. A., Novak, J. D. & Hanesian, H. 1978.
Educational Psychology. A Cognitive View. Second
Edition. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Brown, H. D. 2007.
Principles of Language Learning and Teaching. Fifth Edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall Regents.
Buckwalter, P. 2001. "Repair sequences in Spanish L2 dyadic discourse: A descriptive study".
Modern
Language Journal 85/3: 380-397.
6
Covington, M. V. 1998.
The Will to learn: A Guide for Motivating Young People. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Crookes, G. & R. Schmidt, W. 1991. "Motivation: Reopening the research agenda".
Language Learning 41:
469-512.
Dörnyei, Z. 1994. “Understanding L2 motivation: On with the challenge!”
The Modern Language Journal
78/4: 515-523.
Dőrnyei, Z. 2001.
Attitudes, Orientations, and Motivations in Language Learning: Advances in Theory,
Research, and Applications. Nottingham: University in Nottingham.
Dörnyei, Z. 2005.
The Psychology of the Language Learner. Individual Differences in Second Language
Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ellis, R. 1997.
Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gardner, R. C. & Lambert, W. E. 1972.
Attitudes and Motivation in Second Language Learning. Rowley,
MA: Newbury House Publishers.
Gardner, R. C. & MacIntyre, P. D. 1991. "An instrumental motivation in language study: Who says it isn’t
effective?"
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 13: 57-72.
Gardner, R. C., Day, J. B. & MacIntyre, P. D. 1992. ‘Integrative motivation, induced anxiety, and language
learning in a controlled environment.’
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 14: 197-214.
Gass, S. M. & Selinker, L. 2008.
Second Language Acquisition. An Introductory Course. Third Edition.
New York: Routledge.
Graham, S. 1994. "Classroom motivation from an attributional perspective". In O’Neil, Jr, H. F. & M.
Drillings (Eds.),
Motivation: Theory and Research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence: Erlbaum, 31-48.
Lambert, W. E. 1972.
Language, Psychology, and Culture: Essays by Wallace E. Lambert. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
MacIntyre, P. D. & Gardner, R. C. 1991. "Language anxiety: Its relationship to other anxieties and to
processing in native and second languages".
Language Learning 41: 513-534.
Maslow, A. H. 1970.
Motivation and Personality. Second Edition. New York: Harper & Row.
Michońska-Stadnik, A. 2006. "Some aspects of environmental and gender constraints on attributions of
second language learners". In Zybert, J. (Ed.),
Issues in Foreign Language Learning and Teaching.
Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego. 147-160.
Salkind, N. J. & Rasmussen, K. (Eds.), 2008.
Encyclopedia of Educational Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Skehan, P. 1989.
Individual Differences in Second-Language Learning. London: Arnold.
Weiner, B. 1984. "Principles for a theory of a student motivation and their application within an
attributional framework". In R. Ames and C. Ames (Eds.),
Research on Motivation in Education:
Student Motivation (Vol.I). San Diego: Academic Press. 15-38.