1
Y
EAR
T
WO
Second Language Acquisition
A
CADEMIC
Y
EAR
2011-2012
#16: Neurolinguistic factors
1. Hemispheric dominance
Brain dominance theory has been advanced on the basis of evidence from
research studies indicating that individuals use different parts of the brain to
approach different types of information. Whereas both sides of the brain are
involved in information processing, most people have a preference for one or the
other hemisphere and are accordingly believed to be left-brain dominant or right-
brain dominant. Such brain specialization (or hemispheric dominance) is the effect
of the lateralization of cognitive functions during the process of maturation. Brain
hemisphere information processing styles can be summarized as follows (Bielska
2006: 57):
A
L
EFT
-B
RAIN
D
OMINANT
I
NDIVIDUAL
A
R
IGHT
-B
RAIN
D
OMINANT
I
NDIVIDUAL
focuses on component parts; detects discrete features
focuses on wholes; organizes component parts into a whole
analytical; prefers step-by-step and part-by-part processing
relational; looks for patterns and construction
linear; processes input sequentially
global; integrates input simultaneously
abstract; uses a small part of information to represent the
whole
concrete; relates to things in their current state
logical; draws conclusions based on reasons and facts
intuitive; bases on hunches, feelings or visual images
temporal; sequences one thing after another
spatial; sees where things are in relation to other things
verbal; processes speech using words to name, describe and
define
non-verbal; relies on visual-spatial orientation
learns from rules, exemplification, and trial and error
does not benefit from specific rule formation or error
correction
oriented towards musical notation
oriented towards musical melody and lyrics
ignores emotional cues or makes inappropriate responses
recognizes and interprets emotional cues
In terms of pedagogical implications, the above preferences reveal that right-
brain learners are probably poorly served by analytical instruction. By the same
token, learners with a dominant left-brain hemisphere information processing
style may not benefit from the use of right-brain mode teaching techniques
(Arrowsmith Young & Danesi 2001).
2. Handedness: dexterity vs. sinistrality
Neurologists have determined that in nearly all right-handed individuals most
language functions are located in the left hemisphere, while in the left-handed
population that proportion accounts for only about two-thirds of the cases
(Bielska 2006: 58). Furthermore, according to Arrowsmith Young & Danesi (2001),
hemispheric style correlates with gender.
2
Studies reported in Andreou
et al. (2005: 429-430; e.g. Lamn, 1997; Lamn &
Epstein, 1999) indicate inferior achievements of left-handers in foreign language
performance. Left-handers apparently have less capacity for the developing of
adequate phonological skills needed for reaching high levels of proficiency in L2.
Furthermore, left-handedness seems to affect SLA more than sex since studies
have shown that in comparison with right-handers of both sexes, left-handers of
both sexes are overrepresented in the lowest level L2 classes while being
underrepresented in the higher level L2 classes (Lamn & Epstein 1999).
Andreou
et al. (2005: 431) also report on the results of a questionnaire survey
which comprised items pertaining to hand preference (
dexterity, i.e. right-
handedness, or
sinistrality , i.e. left-handedness) in writing, drawing, throwing a
ball, use of scissors, toothbrush, knife (without fork), spoon, broom (upper hand),
striking a match and opening a jar or a box. It appears that handedness seems to
play no major role in second language acquisition. However, a particular language
learning talent can be an innate, inherited trait, associated with characteristics
belonging to the so-called Geschwind cluster, in which left-handedness coincides
with twinning and having allergies in childhood, among other factors (Larsen-
Freeman 2001: 14).
3. Modality
There are essentially three modality preferences, indicative not only of various
systems to experience outside reality but also of individual learner differences -
visual, auditory and kinaesthetic ("action"). These correspond to general
predispositions to use different sensual approaches to learning (Skehan 1998:
267). According to a research study conducted by Reid (1987) on the preferred
input channel, 40 per cent of learners appear to prefer visual input, 30 per cent
auditory input, and 30 per cent kinaesthetic input (major preferences only). She
also suggests that modality preferences may be fairly fixed in individuals, which
might also reflect more general processing preferences.
By and large, learners with visual modality preference naturally like reading,
studying graphic information, writing and drawing, and they are generally better
stimulated by colour than sound. On the other hand, because information is
better understood and remembered when it is being read, processing auditory
input can be difficult, while extensive listening can easily induce impatience.
Typically, such learners will take detailed notes in class and follow the instructor
with their eyes. Additionally, their oral output is usually kept short or best
avoided, if possible.
3
In contrast, learners with auditory modality preference easily take in information
which they hear. Consequently, they are not terrified of listening activities, though
they might find reading and writing tasks somewhat more challenging. Such
learners also make good participants of role plays, dialogues or class discussions,
and are good at story telling and presenting prepared talks. Whereas oral work is
their obvious strength, written tests are not.
Needless to say, auditory learners are
easily stimulated by music.
Finally, learners with kinaesthetic modality preference learn best when they are
being engaged in hands-on, physical activities or experiments. In other words,
they need to involve sight and hearing as well as action in order to learn. Besides,
because they are tactile learners, they like to touch objects and use hands in the
process of learning. Objects are also among their typical learning tools and
memory aids, while their oral output is characterized by active gesticulation. On
the other hand, sitting still in the classroom may be a real challenge and extended
concentration quite problematic.
4. Neurolinguistic Programming (NLP)
The primary representational systems with which individuals experience the world
can be subsumed under the acronym VAKOG. Besides Visual, Auditory and
Kinaesthetic modalities, it also includes Olfactory (smell) and Gustatory (taste)
(Revell & Norman, 1997: 31). To be sure, Harmer (2001) writes that the role that
the last two may potentially play in language learning or teaching has not as yet
been accounted for, but - sensibly - one should not expect a lot.
The perception of the world through the five senses is the basis for
Neurolinguistic Programming (NLP), which is apparently concerned with the ways
in which one finds out about the rules that people use to succeed in different
fields and then help other individuals to succeed as well. According to O’Connor &
Seymour (1993: 23), the
neuro part of the term stands for behaviour which results
from the neurological processes of seeing, hearing, touching, smelling and
tasting,
linguistic shows that people use language to arrange thoughts and
patterns of behaviour in order to communicate with other people, whereas
programming describes the ways individuals choose to organize concepts and
actions undertaken to pursue specific goals. The authors in question underline
that NLP is a method to discover one's natural abilities as well as a practical way
to achieve what one wants to accomplish (O’Connor & Seymour 1993: 22).
4
A major pedagogical implication of modality preferences and NLP is that teacher
trainers need to raise prospective language instructors' awareness of pupils'
different learning styles. In order to meet various learner needs, teachers must
organize a whole array of activities, appealing to different modalities. Teeler
(2000: 60f.), for example, suggests that when introduced to the Internet as a
language learning tool kinaesthetic learners behave differently from visual
learners. The latter need some demonstration of what to do before starting the
task, whereas the former simply get on and do it.
5. Learning style
The concept of learning or cognitive styles refers to the characteristic ways in
which individuals orientate to problem-solving. Generally speaking, learning
styles refer to consistent and rather enduring tendencies within an individual.
Thus, according to a traditional definition, the term learning style denotes "the
characteristic cognitive, affective and physiological behaviours that serve as
relatively stable indicators of how learners perceive, interact with and respond to
the learning environment" (Keefe, 1979). The most important taxonomies are
summarized below:
Field dependence/independence (FD vs. FI)
A field-dependent learning style is one in which a learner tends to look at the
whole of a learning task which contains many items. Such a learner has difficulty
studying a particular item when it occurs within a "field" of other items. A field
independent learning style, in contrast, is one in which a learner is able to identify
or focus on particular items and is not distracted by other items in the
background or context. In Witkin
et al.'s words,
In a field-dependent mode of perceiving, perception is strongly dominated by the overall
organization of the surrounding field, and parts of the field are experienced as 'fused'. In a
field-independent mode of perceiving, parts of the field are experienced as discrete from
organized ground (...) 'field dependent’ and ‘field independent', like the designations 'tall'
and 'short' are relative (Witkin
et al. 1971:4).
According to Brown (2007: 106), FD can be defined as
the tendency to be
"dependent" on the total field "so that the parts embedded within the field are not
easily perceived, though that total field is perceived more clearly as a unified
whole". In other words, one is able to see the whole picture, the general
configuration of a problem or idea. On the other hand, FI describes
the ability to
recognize a particular item in the group of many distracting items. That is why, in
5
order to investigate subjects' perception of a "field", researchers have devised
various tests, of which the most widely used one is the space-orientation test
called GEFT (
Group Embedded Figures Test),
requiring the subjects to locate a
simple geometrical figure within a more complex design and thus extrapolating
from visual-spatial abilities.
Both FD and FI have their strengths and weaknesses. For example, FI enhances
concentrating on an activity or distinguishing parts from the whole, but when
excessive, it may cause one to perceive only the small items and prevent one from
noticing their relation to the whole. Where the relationship between FD/FI
cognitive styles and L2 learning is concerned, one hypothesis that has been
investigated is whereas FI learners are good at deductive, analytical tasks and
paying attention to details, doing better on measures of formal language learning
(e.g., discrete point tests), FD learners like inductive lessons and focus on
communication, learning successfully in informal, naturalistic language learning
environments, as they tend to socialize and empathize more (Abraham & Vann
1987). Yet according to Hansen (1984), FI learners also do better on integrative
tests and tests of communicative competence, designed to favour FD learners.
Recapitulating, both kinds may profit from different types of learning situation.
Category width
The concept of category width accounts for the distinction between two types of
learners – broad categorizers
and narrow categorizers. The former accept a wide
range of items as belonging to one category, risking the inclusion of items that
might not fit it, and so they tend to overgeneralize - thus, for example, applying
a restricted language rule to all verb forms. In contrast, the latter tend to exclude
items from categories, even when they may fit them. As claimed by Stern (1983:
374), they "limit the rule to a specific context in which it was encountered".
Where pedagogical implications of category width are concerned, the most
successful learners seem to be the ones in between the two extremes - neither
narrow, nor broad categorizers. As stated by Stern, "the good language learner is
reasonably precise in the application of rules and yet prepared to take risks in
order to test the limits of a rule" (ibid.).
Reflectivity/impulsivity
Cognitive style may also distinguish between reflectivity
and impulsivity. If
treated as a dichotomy rather than a continuum, this distinction perceives
language learners as making either slower (reflective) decisions or spontaneous
(impulsive) guesses. Accordingly, reflective learners consider a problem carefully
6
and extensively, whereas impulsive, intuitive learners tend to "gamble" before
arriving at the solution to a given problem. They can answer promptly, with little
concern for accuracy. To be sure, as indicated by Jamieson (1994: 120f.),
reflective learners tend to make fewer errors than impulsive students, but they
may require more time to find the right answer.
Learner style
Rather than elaborate on learning or cognitive styles, Willing (1987) has put forth
the concept of learner styles. These are:
LEARNER STYLE
MAIN CHARACTERISTICS
Converger
solitary, avoiding groups; independent and sure of his own abilities; analytic; imposing his own structures on
learning
Conformist
emphasising learning about language over learning to use it; dependent on those in authority; happy to work in
a non-communicative classroom, doing what he is told; a classroom of conformists prefers to see well-
organised teachers
Concrete
like a conformist, but also enjoying social aspects of learning; profiting from direct experience; interested in
language use and language as communication rather than language as a system; enjoying games and group
work
Communicative
language use orientated; comfortable out of class, showing confidence and willingness to take risks; much more
interested in social interaction with other speakers of the language than in analysis of how L2 works; perfectly
happy to operate without the guidance of the teacher
Other relevant but partly overlapping dichotomies
focusers - concentrating on one feature at a time, in a step-
by-step process
vs.
scanners - dealing with several features at the same time
and allowing their ideas to crystallize slowly
serialists - operating with simple hypotheses (consisting of a
simple proposition)
vs.
holists - operating with complex hypotheses (involving
multiple propositions)
part learners - trying to remember something by separating
it into immediate constituents
vs.
global learners - trying to remember something as a
whole
analytic learners - i.e. rule-formers; taking L2 word for
word; more accurate but less fluent
vs.
gestalt learners - i.e. data-gatherers; more fluent but less
accurate
________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________
REFERENCES AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FURTHER STUDY
Abraham, R. & Vann, R. 1987. "Strategies of two language learners: A case study". In Wenden, A. &
Rubin, J. (Eds.),
Learner Strategies in Language Learning: Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Andreou, G., Vlachos, F. & Andreou, E. 2005. "Affecting factors in second language learning".
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34/5: 429-438.
Arrowsmith Young, B. & Danesi, M. 2001.
Studying How the Brain Learns. Are There Any Useful
Implications for Instruction? Toronto: University of Toronto.
Bielska, J. 2006.
Between Psychology and Foreign Language Learning. Katowice: Wydawnictwo
Uniwersytetu Śląskiego.
Brown, H. D. 2007.
Principles of Language Learning and Teaching. Fourth Edition. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
7
Deacon, T. 1997.
The Symbolic Species. London: Penguin.
Field, J. 2003.
Psycholinguistics. A Resource Book for Students. London: Routledge.
Fromkin, V. et al. 2011.
An Introduction to Language. 9th Edition. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace
College Publishers.
Geschwind, N. 1979. "Specialization of the human brain".
Scientific American 241/3: 180-201.
Gordon, H. W. & Lee, P. A. 1986. "A relationship between gonadotropins and visuospatial
function".
Neuropsychologia, 24/7: 563–576.
Hansen, L. 1984. "Field dependence-independence and language testing: Evidence from six Pacific
island cultures".
TESOL Quarterly 18: 311-324.
Harmer, J. 2001.
The Practice of English Language Teaching. Third Edition. Harlow: Longman.
Hatch, E. M. 1983.
Psycholinguistics. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Jamieson, J. 1994. "The cognitive styles of reflection/impulsivity and field
independence/dependence and ESL success". In Brown, H. D. & Gonzo, S. (Eds.),
Readings
on Second Language Acquisition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall Regents. 119-137.
Keefe, J. 1979. "Learning style: An overview". In Keefe, J. (ed.)
Student Learning Styles: Diagnosing
and Describing Programs. Reston, VA: National Secondary School Principals.
Lamn, O. 1997. "Sinistrality and developmental reading difficulties". In J. Shimron (ed.)
Studies in
the Psychology of Language. Jerusalem: Magnes. 228–247.
Lamn, O. & Epstein, R. 1999. "Left handedness and achievements in foreign language studies".
Brain and Language 70: 504–517.
Larsen-Freeman, D. 2001. "Individual cognitive/affective learner contributions and differential
success in second language acquisition". In Breen, M. P. (ed.)
Learner Contributions to
Language Learning. New Directions in Research. Harlow: Longman. 12-24.
Miller, G. A. 1981.
Language and Speech. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman & Co.
Nowicka, A. & Fersten, E. 2001. "Sex-related differences in interhemispheric transmission time in
the human brain".
Neuroreport 12/18: 4171–4175.
O’Connor, J. & Seymour, J. 1993.
Introducing Neuro-Linguistic Programming: Psychological Skills
for Understanding and Influencing People. Thorsons Publishers.
Reid, J. 1987. "Learning style preferences of ESL students".
TESOL Quarterly 21: 87-111.
Reid, J. M. (Ed.), 1998.
Understanding Learning Styles in the Second Language Classroom. Upple
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall Regents.
Revell, J. & Norman, S. 1997.
In Your Hands: NLP in ELT. Saffire Press.
Schumann, J. H. 2001. "Brain". In Duranti, A. (ed.)
Key Terms in Language and Culture. Oxford:
Blackwell. 15-18.
Skehan, P. 1998.
A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Stern, H. H. 1983.
Fundamental Concepts of Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Teeler, D. 2000.
How to Use the Internet in ELT. Pearson Education.
Willing, K. 1987.
Learning Styles and Adult Migrant Education. Adelaide: National Curriculum
Resource Centre.
Witkin, H., Oltman, O., Raskin, E. & Karp, S. 1971.
A Manual for the Embedded Figures Test. Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychology Press.